Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 24 July 2013
Sec. Pathological Conditions
Volume 7 - 2013 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00088

General developmental health in the VPA-rat model of autism

  • 1Laboratory of Neural Microcircuits, Brain Mind Institute, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Lausanne, Switzerland
  • 2Department of Neuroscience and Pharmacology, Copenhagen University, Copenhagen, Denmark
  • 3Computer Science and Mathematics Department, School of Arts and Sciences, Lebanese American University, Byblos, Lebanon

Autism is a neurodevelopmental condition diagnosed by impaired social interaction, abnormal communication and, stereotyped behaviors. While post-mortem and imaging studies have provided good insights into the neurobiological symptomology of autism, animal models can be used to study the neuroanatomical, neurophysiological and molecular mediators in more detail and in a more controlled environment. The valproic acid (VPA) rat model is an environmentally triggered model with strong construct and clinical validity. It is based on VPA teratogenicity in humans, where mothers who are medicated with VPA during early pregnancy show an increased risk for giving birth to an autistic child. In rats, early embryonic exposure, around the time of neural tube closure, leads to autism-like anatomical and behavioral abnormalities in the offspring. Considering the increasing use of the VPA rat model, we present our observations of the general health of Wistar dams treated with a single intraperitoneal injection of 500 or, 600 mg/kg VPA on embryonic day E12.5, as well as their male and female offspring, in comparison to saline-exposed controls. We report increased rates of complete fetal reabsorption after both VPA doses. VPA 500 mg/kg showed no effect on dam body weight during pregnancy or, on litter size. Offspring exposed to VPA 500 mg/kg showed smaller brain mass on postnatal days 1 (P1) and 14 (P14), in addition to abnormal nest seeking behavior at P10 in the olfactory discrimination test, relative to controls. We also report increased rates of physical malformations in the offspring, rare occurrences of chromodacryorrhea and, developmentally similar body mass gain. Further documentation of developmental health may guide sub-grouping of individuals in a way to better predict core symptom severity.

Introduction

Autism is a severe and pervasive neurodevelopmental disorder, diagnosed by the age of 3 upon clinical presentation of impaired social interaction, abnormal communication, and repetitive behaviors. Autism is highly heritable (Sullivan et al., 2012), and many gene loci serve as potent risk factors (Bonora et al. in Moldin and Rubenstein, 2006; Betancur, 2011; Matsunami et al., 2013). However, the identified susceptibility genes are rare, incompletely penetrant and, often not specific to autism, and thus have not served as a direct cause of autism. In addition, environmental factors seem to play an increasingly important role, as indicated by prevalence estimates over the last 2 decades that rose from as low as 7 to as high as 72.6 cases per 10,000 (Fombonne, 2009). Importantly, broadening and improvement of the diagnostic criteria alone do not explain such an increase (Weintraub, 2011). In support of the role of the environment in autism is the accumulated evidence that biochemical insults during early embryogenesis increase the risk for autistic symptoms in the child. Examples include maternal rubella infection (Chess, 1971), ethanol (Nanson, 1992), misoprostol (Bandim et al., 2003), thalidomide (Stromland et al., 1994), and valproic acid (VPA; Clayton-Smith and Donnai, 1995; Moore et al., 2000; Ornoy, 2009; Christensen et al., 2013; Roullet et al., 2013). Thus, genetic and environmental factors seem to interact in biologically complex mechanisms to yield the broad heterogeneity in symptom severities (Zahir and Brown, 2011).

In order to elucidate the neurobiology underling autism, animal models have been developed. While an animal model may not entirely simulate a human disorder, major symptoms may be captured sufficiently to allow a rigorous study of molecular, proteomic, cellular, circuit, and behavioral alterations in order to better understand the underlying pathology. Several animal models of autism have been presented, including genotype based models affecting the oxytocin system (Modi and Young, 2012), the Reelin signaling pathway (Lakatosova and Ostatnikova, 2012; Folsom and Fatemi, 2013), the Wnt canonical pathway (Kalkman, 2012), the engrailed genes (Kuemerle et al., 2007), MeCP2 (Gonzales and LaSalle, 2010) and, neuroligins (Xu et al., 2012). Alternatively, the most used environmentally triggered model of autism results from embryonic exposure to VPA in the rat (e.g., VPA-rat model; Vorhees, 1987; Rodier et al., 1996; for review see Rodier et al., 1997; Markram et al., 2007), and a VPA-mouse model is also available (Chapman and Cutler, 1984; Wagner et al., 2006; Gandal et al., 2010; Roullet et al., 2010; Kataoka et al., 2013).

In humans, VPA is widely used as an anti-epileptic, and also prescribed as a mood stabilizer and against migraines, and it is currently in clinical trial as an anti-cancer agent. However, VPA is teratogenic (Nau et al., 1982; Ornoy, 2009), where exposure during gestation increases the risk for various congenital malformations in the child, grouped as Fetal Valproate Syndrome (FVS; Kozma, 2001; Kini, 2006; Ornoy, 2009). FVS features also include decreased verbal intelligence and an association between VPA exposure in utero and autism has been consistently reported (reviewed in Ornoy, 2009; Roullet et al., 2013). In particular, Moore and colleagues showed 60% of those diagnosed with FVS exhibit 2 or more autistic features, and 11% develop full blown autism (Moore et al., 2000). In the same line, Rasalam and colleagues indicated that 8.9% of children exposed to VPA in utero develop autistic features (Rasalam et al., 2005). Based on the nature of physical malformations and brainstem morphological changes in autism, it has become apparent that VPA exposure in the first trimester of gestation represents the highest risk for the child to develop autism, in particular after exposure during the time of neural tube closure and genesis of brainstem cranial nerve nuclei cells (Kemper and Bauman, 1998; Rodier, 2002, 2004; Arndt et al., 2005; Ornoy, 2009).

The idea of early embryogenesis as the critical time for autism led to development of the VPA rat model (Rodier et al., 1997; Kemper and Bauman, 1998; Arndt et al., 2005; Kim et al., 2011). Strong clinical validation of the model is given by observation that offspring of rats exposed to a single dose of VPA on embryonic day 12.5, around the time of neural tube closure and brain stem nuclei formation in rats, present neuroanatomical and behavioral characteristics similar to human autism. Specifically, VPA-exposed offspring present (1) a reduced number of motor cells in cranial nerve motor nuclei in the brain stem (Rodier et al., 1996; Rodier, 2002); (2) a reduced number of Purkinje cells in the cerebellum, observed both in VPA-treated offspring (Rodier et al., 1997; Ingram et al., 2000) and in autism (Kemper and Bauman, 1998); (3) decreased social interactions, increased repetitive behaviors, lower sensitivity to pain, impaired sensorimotor gating or eye blink conditioning, and increased anxiety described in VPA-treated offspring (Schneider et al., 2001, 2006, 2007; Schneider and Przewlocki, 2005; Stanton et al., 2007; Markram et al., 2008; Bambini-Junior et al., 2011; Kim et al., 2011; Zhang et al., 2012) and commonly found in the autistic spectrum (McAlonan et al., 2002; Gaigg and Bowler, 2007; Perry et al., 2007; Hofvander et al., 2009; MacNeil et al., 2009; van Steensel et al., 2011). Moreover, rat offspring treated at E12.5 exhibit strongly amplified conditioned fear memories, which generalize to non-conditioned stimuli and are resistant to extinction, in parallel to increased synaptic plasticity in the amygdala (Markram et al., 2008; see also Sui and Chen, 2012). Abnormal high anxiety, fears and phobias are also commonly reported in autism (Kanner, 1943; Muris et al., 1998; however, see Bernier et al., 2005; Evans et al., 2005; Hofvander et al., 2009; Settipani et al., 2012). Similar results have been obtained by slightly modified VPA treatment protocols in the rat (e.g., Rodier et al., 1996; Chomiak et al., 2010; Dufour-Rainfray et al., 2010; Narita et al., 2010; Kim et al., 2011; Tashiro et al., 2011), and in the mouse (e.g., Wagner et al., 2006; Gandal et al., 2010; Mehta et al., 2011; Roullet and Crawley, 2011; Hara et al., 2012; Kataoka et al., 2013). Taking into account these striking behavioral and anatomical parallels between the VPA model and human autism, the VPA rat model is well suited to study synaptic and circuit alterations in order to elucidate the potential neuropathology of autism.

The data on the VPA model not only further support a closer association between neural tube closure and autistic traits, but provide new insights into the neuropathology of autism. Studies in our lab demonstrated that the neocortex and amygdala of VPA-treated rat offspring is hyper-reactive and hyper-plastic due to hyper-connectivity between neurons (Rinaldi et al., 2007; Markram et al., 2008; Rinaldi et al., 2008a,b; Silva et al., 2009). These observations indicate excessive processing and storage of information in these nervous systems. They suggest that hyper-functional microcircuits could underlie autism, and further research is warranted.

In view of the increased use of the VPA model to study the neurobiology of autism, the goal of the present paper is to provide better practical reference to those who seek to reproduce the model. First, we describe a VPA dose dependence of offspring survival, because there is no consistent data on pregnancy outcome in the literature. We then present data on the global developmental health of VPA-treated dams and their offspring exposed to 500 mg/kg VPA during pregnancy. With this study we hope to motivate the documentation of such variables as potential predictors of autistic-like vulnerability.

Materials and Methods

Animals and VPA-Model

Outbred Wistar Han rats (Janvier Laboratories, France) were mated in house or ordered pregnant, and pregnancy was determined by the presence of a vaginal plug on embryonic day 1 (E1). To produce experimental rats based on the VPA rat model of autism (Rodier et al., 1997), we prepared sodium salt of valproic acid (NaVPA, Sigma-Aldrich) in 0.9% saline (100 mg/ml, pH 7.3). On E12.5, VPA-dams received a single intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection of NaVPA, either 500 mg/kg, 3.3 ml/kg, or 600 mg/kg, 3.3 ml/kg; control (CTR) dams received a single injection of saline vehicle (i.p., 0.9%, 3.3 ml/kg). Dams were housed individually and allowed to raise their own litters until weaning on postnatal day 23 (P23). Both male and female offspring were included in the study. The offspring were housed with 2–3 rats in same-sex, same-treatment cages. Standard plastic laboratory cages were used with bedding and ad libitum access to food and water, cleaned once a week. Animals were kept in a 12 h light-dark schedule with lights on at 6:30 am, in rooms under controlled humidity and temperature. All the procedures were in conformity with the Swiss National Institutional Guidelines on Animal Experimentation for the ethical use of animals, and approved by the Swiss Cantonal Veterinary Office Committee for Animal Experimentation.

Dams and Pregnancy

To determine the toxicity of different doses of VPA, we measured the complete fetal re-absorption rate (miscarriage), as the percent of dams from which a vaginal plug had been detected that did not give birth to a single pup, in CTR (n = 40), VPA 500 mg/kg (n = 26), and VPA 600 mg/kg (n = 37) dams. Statistical comparisons between the number of reabsorptions in each VPA treatment and the control group were done with X2 tests, and statistical significance is reported at the alpha level of 0.05.

To confirm the lower toxicity of VPA (500 mg/kg) on pregnancy, we measured the body mass of dams (CTR n = 9, VPA n = 15) on E12.5, E14, E21, P1, and, P6, for dams that gave birth to at least one pup. Analysis was done with a univariate two-way mixed factorial ANOVA (RM 2-ANOVA), using treatment (CTR or VPA) as the between subject factor, and time of weighing (E12.5, E14, E21, P1, P6) as the repeated measures within subjects factor. Statistical significance is reported at the alpha level of 0.05.

For further investigation of pregnancy outcome, the litter size was counted 2 days after birth in litters where at least one pup was born in CTR (n = 29) and VPA (500 mg/kg; n = 33). For analysis of the effect of VPA treatment on litter size, a two-tailed Student's t-test was applied, and statistical significance is reported at the alpha level of 0.05.

Offspring Body Mass

Body mass gain of the offspring from dams treated with either vehicle (CTR) or VPA (500 mg/kg) was measured weekly for 33 postnatal weeks (PW 2–12, 15–25, and 27–38). A total of six unbalanced cohorts were evaluated (CTR n = 191, VPA n = 191), where measurement time points were inconsistent across cohorts (e.g., some cohorts measurements were from the first 12 weeks, others for months 1–7, others only for particular weeks). Thus, standard ANOVA tests were inappropriate, and we used a mixed effect model to analyse the effects of treatment (CTR or VPA) and sex (male or female) on offspring body mass over time (age of animal). The model was built using the lmer() function from the lme4 package in R (version 2.11.1) and, the interactions of age by sex, age by treatment, and sex by treatment were taken into account. We report |t-value| >2.5 to be statistically significant.

Offspring Brain Mass

The brain mass of CTR (n = 55) and VPA (500 mg/kg, n = 50) offspring were measured on P1 and P14. Brains were extracted excluding the olfactory bulb, and excluding parts of the brainstem posterior to the cerebellum. A total of two balanced cohorts were used, one with paraformaldehyde (PFA) fixed and, the other with fresh brains. For the fixation procedure, animals were sedated with pentobarbital (i.p. 100 mg/kg, 100 mg/ml) and transcardially perfused with phosphate buffered saline (PBS), followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS. The brains were extracted and placed in 4% PFA in PBS for 24 h, for post-fixation at room temperature, then transferred to a 30% sucrose solution in distilled water and stored at 4°C for 2–4 days until density equilibration. For fresh brain measurements, animals were removed from their home cage and decapitated, the brains dissected immediately, and placed on the weighing dish. Separate statistical analyses for P1 and P14 were done to address the effect of treatment and sex on absolute brain mass, with a univariate two-way ANOVA (2-ANOVA). Post-hoc analysis of the effect of treatment was carried out with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Complimentary investigation of body mass as a covariate in brain mass was done with a univariate analysis of the effect of treatment, sex, age on brain mass with a 3-way mixed model analysis of covariance (3-ANCOVA), using treatment (CTR or VPA), sex (male or female), and age (P1 and P14), as the between subject factors, and body mass as a covariate. Statistical significance is reported at the alpha level of 0.05.

Offspring Malformations

We documented any major physical malformation of the offspring from dams treated with either vehicle or 500 mg/kg of VPA, present at any time during the life of the animal. We compared the rates between the 2 treatment groups with 2-tailed FET, and report statistical significance at the alpha level of 0.05.

Offspring Chromodacryorrhea

Chromodacryorrhea is a condition found in rats due to porphyrin over-production by the Harderian glands in the eye. This exudate accumulation can form red (blood-looking) crusts around the eyes, nose, and the neck fur of the animals after grooming. Chromodacryorrhea can be used as an indicator of general stress. We documented the sign of any visible red crusts around the eyes or nose on rats, at any time during the lifetime of the animal, and compared these rates between the 2 treatment groups with 1-tailed FET. Statistical significance is reported at the alpha level of 0.05.

Offspring Olfactory Discrimination Behavioral Test

This test measures nest-seeking behavior mediated by olfactory cues present in the home cage bedding (Gregory and Pfaff, 1971). The apparatus consisted of a clean standard housing cage (transparent polycarbonate, 375 length × 215 width × 20 height cm) with a line drawn through the center, and with two olfactory stimuli placed at opposite ends of the cage: one was a plastic Petri dish (10 cm diameter) filled with home bedding (nest odor), the other was a dish filled with clean bedding (neutral odor). On P10, male and female offspring were individually placed in the center of the cage, on the middle line, and the latency to reach the first stimulus was measured, determined by when the pup stepped with both forepaws into one of the dishes. The cage was cleaned between animals with 5% Ethanol. For clarity, statistical tests and sample sizes are specified in the results section, where statistical significance is reported at the alpha level of 0.05.

Statistical Software

Levene's tests, Student's t-tests, Welch's correction, 2-ANOVA and Bonferroni correction were run on GraphPad Prism software (version 4.00 for Windows, GraphPad Software, San Diego California, USA). The mixed effect linear model was run on R Freeware (version 2.11.1, www.r-project.org, ISBN 3-900051-08-9). Pearson's Chi-Square tests (X2), FET (used when X2 expected counts were bellow 5), and 3-ANCOVA were ran on IBM SPSS software (version 19 for Windows or Mac, Chicago, IL, USA).

Results

Dams and Pregnancy

The general health of both VPA-treated and control dams was normal. However, Figure 1 shows the effect of two VPA doses, 500 mg/kg and 600 mg/kg, on fetal reabsorption rates, compared with CTR. For dams with a detected vaginal plug, while 2.5% (1/40) of CTR dams showed complete fetal reabsorption, significantly higher rates were found for both VPA 500 mg/kg (23.1%, 6/26, FET p = 0.013 2-tailed), and VPA 600 mg/kg [54.1%, 20/37, X2(1, 77) = 19.67, p < 0.0001] in comparison with CTR. All further studies were carried out with the VPA-treatment dose of 500 mg/kg in order to avoid a very high fetal reabsorption rate.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Fetal reabsorption rates for control (CTR), VPA 500 mg/kg and, VPA 600 mg/kg. *Indicate VPA doses with higher rates than CTR. Data shown as percent of pregnancies.

Figure 2A shows the effect of VPA (500 mg/kg) on pregnancy body mass compared to CTR, from measurements taken prenatally at E12.5, E14, and E21, and postpartum, on P1 and P6. We observed a significant main effect of day of weighing [F(4, 97) = 112.3, p < 0.0001], but no main effect of treatment [F(1, 97) = 2.289, p = 0.1335], and no interaction between treatment and day factors [F(4, 97) = 0.2812, p = 0.8895]. Figure 2B shows the effect of VPA (500 mg/kg) on litter size (number of pups born) for pregnancies carried out to term (at least 1 live pup born) compared with CTR. We observed no statistically significant differences between the two treatment groups[t(60) = 1.58, p = 0.118].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Pregnancy outcome in control (CTR) and VPA 500 mg/kg treated dams. (A) Dam body mass (grams) during embryonic days 12.5, 14, 21 (prenatal) and postnatal days 1 and 6, suffer no significant effect of treatment. (B) Litter size (number of pups born) is not different between treatments. Data shown as mean (M) and standard error of the mean (S.E.M.).

Offspring Body Mass

As shown in Figure 3, prenatal treatment with VPA 500 mg/kg had no statistically significant effect on the body mass of offspring measured from postnatal weeks 2 until 38. A mixed effect model applied to assess the effect of age, sex and treatment on offspring body weight showed that the most significant contributing factor to the variability in body mass was the interaction of age by sex (age:sexMale, t-value = 34.47) while treatment had no effect (statusVPA, t-value = 0.20).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Offspring body mass from control (CTR) and VPA 500 mg/kg groups, over 33 postnatal weeks. Females are lighter than males but there is no effect of treatment. Data shown as mean (M) and standard error of the mean (S.E.M.).

Offspring Brain Mass

The brain mass of offspring treated with either CTR or VPA (500 mg/kg) is shown in Table 1, measured as absolute grams and normalized to body mass. We observed a statistically significant main effect of treatment on absolute brain mass at P1 [F(1, 16)= 16.45, p = 0.0009], but no main effect of sex [F(1, 16) = 1.49, p = 0.241], and no interaction between sex and treatment [F(1, 16) = 0.181, p = 0.676]. Bonferroni corrected post-hoc comparisons at P1 indicated a significantly smaller absolute brain size in VPA females (p < 0.05), as well as in VPA males (p < 0.05), in comparison to same-sex same-age CTR groups. Similar analysis repeated for P14 indicated a significant main effect of prenatal treatment [F(1, 81) = 45.13, p < 0.0001], and a main effect of sex [F(1, 81) = 4.37, p = 0.04], but no interaction between treatment and sex [F(1, 81) = 0.77, p = 0.384]. Bonferroni corrected post-hoc comparisons at P14 indicated a significantly smaller absolute brain size in VPA females (p < 0.001), as well as in VPA males (p < 0.001), in comparison to same-sex same-age CTR groups.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Offspring brain mass.

Since the ratio of brain to body mass is not necessarily constant across age, we do not present any statistical comparisons on the normalized brain measure (a ratio), but present it for comparison to other work. We performed instead a 3-ANCOVA, where absolute brain mass means were statistically corrected for body weight (covariate). This revealed a significant main effect of prenatal treatment [F(1, 95) = 12.86, p = 0.001], a significant main effect of age [F(1, 95) = 170.14, p < 0.0001], but no significant main effect of sex [F(1, 95) = 0.87, p = 0.35]. In addition, we found no interaction between treatment and sex [F(1, 95) = 0.18, p = 0.67], but a significant interaction between treatment and age [F(1, 95) = 6.21, p = 0.014] on corrected brain mass. Thus, when body mass is used as a covariate, the microencephaly effect of VPA is more prominent at P14 then at P1.

Offspring Malformations

In general, the controls as well as VPA-exposed offspring exhibited good health. However, offspring exposed to VPA 500 mg/kg occasionally exhibited some physical abnormalities including shorter snouts, multiple toes, or dwarfism (data not shown, the latter were excluded from experiments). In addition, as shown in Table 2, VPA-treated offspring presented statistically significant higher frequency of tail kinks (9% of all animals, FET p < 0.0001 2-tailed; 10% of males, FET p = 0.003 2-tailed; 9% females, FET p = 0.014 2-tailed), relative to CTR (0% of either sex).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Offspring tail malformations.

Offspring Chromodacryorrhea

The frequency of occurrences of chromodacryorrhea as summarized in Table 3 represent rare events in the VPA, with a non-significant trend for a difference between CTR and VPA-exposed offspring (all animals, FET p = 0.07, 1-tailed; males, FET p = 0.133, 1-tailed; females, FET p = 0.523, 1-tailed).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Offspring chromodacryorrhea.

Offspring Olfactory Discrimination Behavioral Test

We assessed behavioral olfactory discrimination, where CTR and VPA 500 mg/kg males and females pups aged P10 were given a choice between nest and clean bedding. We observed no effect of treatment on the number of pups that reached the nest versus neutral odors, neither in males [CTR nest n = 18/22, VPA nest n = 18/25; X2(1, 47) = 0.63, p = 0.43] nor in females [CTR nest n = 17/23, VPA nest n = 23/28; X2(1, 51) = 0.51, p = 0.48]. Subsequent analysis of the latency (in seconds) to reach the nest odor when nest was as the first choice (Figure 4A), indicated that the VPA group had a significantly different variance from the CTR in males [Levene's Test F(17, 17) = 9.841, p = 0.004], and in females [Levene's Test F(22, 16) = 15.45, p < 0.0001]. In addition, we observed a non-significant trend for a difference between CTR and VPA in the latency to reach the nest odor in males [Welch's corrected Student t-tests t(17) = 1.96, p = 0.067], and a significant difference in females [Welch's corrected Student t-tests t(25) = 2.46, p = 0.021]. Lastly, Figure 4B shows the latency to reach the neutral odor when it was the first choice; we observed no differences between CTR and VPA in males [t(9) = 1.76, p = 0.11], and no difference in females [t(9) = 0.08, p = 0.94].

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Olfactory discrimination behavior at P10 from control (CTR) and VPA 500 mg/kg offspring. Latencies to reach the odor of 1st choice, being either from (A) nest bedding, where females show an effect of treatment, and males show a trend, or from (B) clean bedding, where no effects were detected. Data shown as mean (M) and standard error of the mean (S.E.M.).

Discussion

Dose-Dependent Effects of VPA Administration on Pregnancy Outcome

As recently reviewed by Roullet et al. (2013), clinical validity of the VPA model in rats and mice has been observed even with slight variations in the protocol for VPA administration. In utero exposure to VPA between 350–800 mg/kg around E11–12.5 seems to be the critical time window for most autistic-like behavioral and anatomical abnormalities in rats and mice (Ingram et al., 2000; Schneider et al., 2001; Schneider and Przewlocki, 2005; Markram et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2011). However, a precise dose threshold for specific autistic-like and non-autistic features has not been well established in the model. Interestingly, lower doses were first used to introduce abnormalities associated with in utero VPA exposure in the mouse (v.o. 160–100 mg/kg, multidose; Chapman and Cutler, 1984), and later, to associate in utero VPA exposure in the rat with autism (i.p. 300 mg/kg, single dose at E11.5, E12, E12.5; Rodier et al., 1996). For non-explicit translational reasons, subsequent studies used higher VPA doses, and demonstrated a significant increase in autistic-like behaviors in a battery of tests. For comparison, the therapeutic doses in humans currently in use range from 200 to 3600 mg/day, while the threshold for increased risks varies from 800 mg to 1400 mg/day, depending on the morphological or neurodevelopmental measure (Roullet et al., 2013). For an average female of 65 kg, these doses are equivalent to 3–55 mg/kg, a range substantially lower than that used in the current animal models (350–800 mg/kg). Importantly, species differences in the pharmacokinetics of VPA must be taken into account when interpreting the relevance of the model. For instance, VPA bioavailability in humans (70–100%) is about twice of that in rodents (34–47%;Loscher, 1999, 2002; Haddad et al., 2009; Roullet et al., 2013). Furthermore, in utero toxicant exposure is also likely to vary across species, but to date, few studies have focused on the issue (Binkerd et al., 1988; Hendrickx et al., 1988). As a consequence, it is difficult today to estimate how the model doses compare to humans. Nonetheless, the literature today demonstrates that VPA exposure (at presumably very high doses) in rodents at a specific time in embryonic development (neural tube closure), is sufficient to induce neurodevelopmental and morphological features that resemble autism in humans. This is an important feature of the model, because it narrows down the neural systems that are primary targets for autism vulnerability, and provides experimentally controlled conditions for the study of the genetic interactions with environmental factors that can lead to specific features. A clearer translational value of mechanisms identified in the model can emerge from future studies comparing VPA pharmacokinetics across species, and the levels of VPA required for specific deficits.

Few attempts have been made to directly compare dose effects in rodents (Wagner et al., 2006; Frisch et al., 2009). We report here that VPA administered i.p. at 500 mg/kg on E12.5 is less toxic than at 600 mg/kg in terms of fetal survival. Then, we observed no further negative pregnancy outcomes of i.p. 500 mg/kg compared to controls in terms of dam body mass, nor on the number of pups born, for pregnancies that reached term with live pups. These results complement previously published data on different strains with different doses (Binkerd et al., 1988; Stanton et al., 2007; Kim et al., 2011, 2013), and support VPA dose-dependent toxicity for fetal survival. These results in the rat-model also compliment observations in humans on dose-dependent effects of VPA on negative pregnancy outcomes, where epileptic or bipolar patients in childbearing age are recommended to use the lowest possible dose of anti-epileptics, particularly valproate (Roullet et al., 2013). In parallel, the dose of 600 mg/kg is not a practical choice for studies of autism in the offspring, due to high rates of fetal mortality. With this in mind, the scope of the current study focused on the lower dose for subsequent measures.

Offspring Body Mass

There was no effect of VPA treatment on offspring body mass gain from postnatal week 2 well into adulthood (week 38). These results differ from the reduced body mass gain in VPA previously reported (Schneider and Przewlocki, 2005). This difference between the two studies may be explained by the use of 600 mg/kg males, housed five same sex animals per cage used by Schneider and Przewlocki, and difference in strain. For instance, the larger group housing may change social hierarchy and social anxiety or exercise levels, all of which could affect body weight in the Schneider and Przewlowski study but not in our case. It remains unclear if body weight observations in different versions of the VPA model are directly linked to autism-like symptoms or, if those effects are independent from autism and specific to a strain or dosage. Importantly, few studies have examined the body mass status of autistic patients (Emond et al., 2010), with inconsistent reports on the relationship with growth or obesity. While feeding symptoms and restricted diet are consistently reported to differ from controls, that is not case for energy intake which may (Evans et al., 2012) or may not (Emond et al., 2010; Kataoka et al., 2013) differ in ASD (Evans et al., 2012). Therefore, species-specific metabolic mechanisms and behavioral dietary choice or activity patters, (not modulated in standard laboratory environments for rodents, but variable for humans), all make the direct translation between the rodent model and the human body mass data currently difficult. Further research is needed on the VPA model on dietary choice and activity patterns, and their effect on growth and body mass, which may then help clarify inconsistencies in the literature.

Offspring Brain Mass

We report here a reduced absolute brain mass of VPA exposed animals in early postnatal life (P1, and P14); when body mass is controlled for, the effect of VPA treatment is most apparent at P14. In contrast, absolute microencephaly (on E14 and E18) followed by macroencephaly (on P2 and P7) has been recently described in offspring of Sprague Dawley dams injected s.c. with 400 mg/kg VPA on E13 (vaginal plug counted as E1), without control for body mass (Go et al., 2012). In another variant of the VPA-model (v.o. 800 mg/kg, on E13 counted from E1 vaginal plug; Mychasiuk et al., 2012) absolute brain mass at around P100 was shown to be decreased, and there is no reference to body size. Even though body mass is expected to become less associated with brain size in adults (no longer growing), the lack of systematic presentation of absolute brain mass and, its relationship with body mass at younger ages make the effects of VPA on brain mass debatable; additional difficulty for interpretation is due to the difference in VPA administration protocols, and the lack of studies on dose effects. Thorough presentation of brain and body mass could become an important issue, as the literature in humans and other animal models suggest that autism is accompanied by premature or accelerated neuronal growth early in cortical development, which is followed by normal or reduced sizes in adolescence and adulthood (Casanova, 2007; Chomiak et al., 2010; Courchesne et al., 2011; Chomiak and Hu, 2013). Thus, additional studies are needed to determine if altered brain size and underlying causes, are predictive of more severe autistic-like abnormalities in the model. This information will also be useful to further confirm the robustness of the model in view of different protocols for model generation currently in the literature.

Offspring Physical Malformations

We observed an increased occurrence of physical malformations in the tails of VPA-exposed offspring, compared to controls. These malformations indicate VPA toxicity for neural tube development. Similar results have been demonstrated in Wistar male rats exposed to 600 mg/kg VPA on E12 (Foley et al., 2012) and Sprague-Dawley male rats exposed to 600mg/kg VPA on E12 (Binkerd et al., 1988; Kim et al., 2013), further supporting at least some effects of VPA are independent of strain. These observations are also congruent with congenital malformations observed in humans exposed to VPA in utero, where the pattern of dysmorphic features are indicative of toxicity early in embryogenesis, around the time of neural tube closure (Rodier, 2002; Arndt et al., 2005; Tashiro et al., 2011). Birth defects in children with FVS include defects in facial features, neural tube associated, cleft pallet, cardiovascular, limb, and digits malformations, amongst others (Clayton-Smith and Donnai, 1995). In parallel, researchers have reported co-occurrence of autism and birth defects. Schendel and colleagues report 6.4% of autistic children present some form of birth defect, compared to 3.2% of children without autism (Schendel et al., 2009). Conversely, 0.43% of children presenting a birth defect presented autism, as compared to 0.22% among children without birth defects, which translates into 1.7 relative risk of presenting autism compared to children without birth defects. These were mainly isolated, and of central nervous system/eye, genitourinary, muscoskeletal, or cardiovascular nature; other defects included head/neck, respiratory, oral cleft, and gastrointestinal. Among the children with autism, male to female ratio of birth defects was approximately 7:1. Miles and colleagues report 20% of autistic patients with abnormal morphology, and 29% of these with abnormal brain morphology; they argue that full clinical morphological examination is highly sensitive to embryonic developmental insults, and thus has the power to better delineate patient subgroups (Miles and Hillman, 2000). Based on the congruence between the animal model and the human, it may be of interest to correlate birth defects with autism-like symptom severity in the models, as well as to investigate the mechanisms of VPA teratology. This knowledge may help delineate autism subgroups for clinical and genetic studies, and thus lead to better understanding of the endophenotypes more vulnerable to autistic features.

Offspring Chromodacryorrhea

We also report here noticeable, but statistically non-significant, chromodacryorrhea (“red tears”) in the VPA exposed rats, compared to none observed in controls. It remains possible however, that our sample size and analysis do not capture a true effect of VPA on rare biological contexts, and thus, that measuring chromodacryorrhea would remain indicative of vulnerable endophenotype. Chromodacryorrhea is observed due to accumulation of porphyrin after overproduction by the Harderian glands (HD) in the eye. It has been associated with physical stress (Harkness and Ridgway, 1980), joint pain (Kerins et al., 2003), bright light (Hugo et al., 1987), but not to bi-weekly or weekly home cage cleaning and human handling (Burn et al., 2006). Thus, chromodacryorrhea is used by animal caretakers as a non-invasive measure of general stress response (Burn et al., 2006; Castelhano-Carlos and Baumans, 2009). To our knowledge, no previous reports on the VPA rat demonstrated rates of chromodacryorrhea. HG secretion is related to several factors including hormonal functions, sexual differentiation, circadian rhythm, season, and age (dos Reis et al., 2005; Monteforte et al., 2009). Importantly, general stress is increased in human autism (Muris et al., 1998; Evans et al., 2005; MacNeil et al., 2009). Thus, as for physical malformations, it may be of interest to correlate the occurrence of chromodacryorrhea with autistic-like symptom severity in the model. Since stress and anxiety are frequently reported in humans (Evans et al., 2005), such studies will aid in the translational value of the VPA model.

Offspring Olfactory Discrimination Behavioral Test

We present the VPA (500 mg/kg) olfactory deficit at P10, where females take longer to reach the nest bedding as compared to controls, and both males and females show increased variability in this latency relative to controls. The observation at P10 may result from a deficit more pronounced at younger ages, which is then not fully recovered by P10 for some individuals. In agreement with this idea, Schneider and Przewlocki (Schneider and Przewlocki, 2005) show pup olfactory discrimination of nest bedding from clean bedding was delayed in the VPA group (600 mg/kg), which took longer time than controls to reach nest bedding at P9, but recovered at P10–11. Furthermore, delayed nest seeking behavior, recovered by P11, was also observed in the VPA mouse model (Roullet et al., 2010). These observations suggest that there is a nest-seeking deficit in the VPA-exposed animals, which may be partially overcome with age in certain individuals. These observations could at first be interpreted as recovery in the older animals from an initial social deficit; however, considering it has been previously shown that the adult animals exposed to VPA in utero also present social behavior deficits (Roullet et al., 2013), it seems unlikely that recovery from a primary social deficit is taking place. One alternative explanation to a primary social deficit is that VPA animals may suffer from primary olfactory deficit; in this case, the observed increased VPA latencies for nest seeking would be caused by weaker or absent stimulus identification. However, such a sensory deficit would be expected to reduce the overall number of animals that reach the nest in the VPA, because more animals would seek the neutral odor in error, while this is not observed. Alternatively, VPA could suffer from a primary deficit in motor execution of the behavior. However, a generalized motor deficit would be expected to affect the latency to reach any stimulus, and this is not the case, as we observed no effect of treatment on the latency to reach the neutral bedding. On the other hand, compensatory mechanisms, such as the preference for familiar odors, may come into play with development and then guide the older animals toward the nest. Taken together, these results support a deficit in the VPA to use social-odor information to return to their nest and mother, olfactory guided survival behaviors. In addition, they support that in utero exposure to VPA at 500 mg/kg at E12.5 is sufficient to induce a core autism-like symptom. Future studies in the animal models are needed to determine which compensatory mechanisms may come into play with age to mask social dysfunction in certain tasks, in certain individuals. Considering social deficit is a primary symptom for autism diagnosis, such studies will not only further validate the VPA model but cue into individual differences in the level of social dysfunction in humans.

As mentioned above, while the embryonic critical time for autism vulnerability seems to be E11–12.5, there is currently no indication in the literature on which exact VPA dose produces the most valid rodent model. Our results indicate that VPA administered to the mother with a single i.p. injection of 500 mg/kg on E12.5 (counted from E1vaginal plug detection) is sufficient to induce autistic like neurodevelopmental effects. We note that cross-laboratory interpretation must be made with care, due to inconsistency in reporting how the embryonic days were counted; it is customary to treat vaginal plug detection as E1 for mice and E0 for rats, but the latter is less consistent. For instance, work in rodents that have reported E12.5 as the critical period are not inconsistent to work reporting E11.5 because only the latter counted vaginal plug day as E0. None of the reviews attempting at translating the observation in the model to autism corrected for these inconsistencies when comparing studies (Markram et al., 2007; Markram and Markram, 2010; Roullet and Crawley, 2011; Kaffman and Krystal, 2012; Modi and Young, 2012; Roullet et al., 2013).

Taking into account the striking behavioral, and anatomical parallels to human autism, the VPA-rat model allows for systematic investigations of the biological events that lead to autism-like neuropathology, from the molecular to the behavioral level, guiding new and more specific hypothesis in humans (Markram and Markram, 2010). The first studies in this direction corroborate with the idea of autism as a minicolumn-pathy (Markram et al., 2007; Markram and Markram, 2010; Williams and Casanova, 2011). Interestingly, more recent results demonstrate the overlap between genetically based models and VPA epigenetic mechanisms, supporting a potential common pathophysiological mechanism in autism (Kolozsi et al., 2009; Go et al., 2012). We present here further support of the notion that in addition to sex, the brain mass, body mass, physical malformations, and reactivity to general stress may be important covariates in future investigations of the core autistic features, and to indicate how environmental factors interact with an individual's genome to constitute particularly vulnerable endophenotype for the development of autism.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Alliance for Autism Research, by the European Commission for the EUSynapse project (LSHM-CT-2005-019055) and the EU 6th Framework Programme (PROMEMORIA LSHM-CT-2005-512012).

References

Arndt, T. L., Stodgell, C. J., and Rodier, P. M. (2005). The teratology of autism. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 23, 189–199. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.11.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bambini-Junior, V., Rodrigues, L., Behr, G. A., Moreira, J. C., Riesgo, R., and Gottfried, C. (2011). Animal model of autism induced by prenatal exposure to valproate: behavioral changes and liver parameters. Brain Res. 1408, 8–16. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bandim, J. M., Ventura, L. O., Miller, M. T., Almeida, H. C., and Costa, A. E. (2003). Autism and mobius sequence: an exploratory study of children in northeastern brazil. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 61, 181–185. doi: 10.1590/S0004-282X2003000200004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bernier, R., Dawson, G., Panagiotides, H., and Webb, S. (2005). Individuals with autism spectrum disorder show normal responses to a fear potential startle paradigm. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 35, 575–583. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0002-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Betancur, C. (2011). Etiological heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorders: more than 100 genetic and genomic disorders and still counting. Brain Res. 1380, 42–77. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.11.078

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Binkerd, P. E., Rowland, J. M., Nau, H., and Hendrickx, A. G. (1988). Evaluation of valproic acid (vpa) developmental toxicity and pharmacokinetics in sprague-dawley rats. Fundam. Appl. Toxicol. 11, 485–493. doi: 10.1016/0272-0590(88)90112-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burn, C. C., Peters, A., Day, M. J., and Mason, G. J. (2006). Long-term effects of cage-cleaning frequency and bedding type on laboratory rat health, welfare, and handleability a cross-laboratory study. Lab. Anim. 40, 353–370. doi: 10.1258/002367706778476460

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Casanova, M. F. (2007). The neuropathology of autism. Brain Pathol. 17, 422–433. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-3639.2007.00100.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Castelhano-Carlos, M. J., and Baumans, V. (2009). The impact of light, noise, cage cleaning and in-house transport on welfare and stress of laboratory rats. Lab. Anim. 43, 311–327. doi: 10.1258/la.2009.0080098

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chapman, J. B., and Cutler, M. G. (1984). Sodium valproate: effects on social behaviour and physical development in the mouse. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 83, 390–396. doi: 10.1007/BF00428553

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chess, S. (1971). Autism in children with congenital rubella. J. Autism Child. Schizophr. 1, 33–47. doi: 10.1007/BF01537741

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chomiak, T., and Hu, B. (2013). Alterations of neocortical development and maturation in autism: insight from valproic acid exposure and animal models of autism. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 36, 57–66. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2012.08.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chomiak, T., Karnik, V., Block, E., and Hu, B. (2010). Altering the trajectory of early postnatal cortical development can lead to structural and behavioural features of autism. BMC Neurosci. 11:102. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-11-102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Christensen, J., Gronborg, T. K., Sorensen, M. J., Schendel, D., Parner, E. T., Pedersen, L. H., et al. (2013). Prenatal valproate exposure and risk of autism spectrum disorders and childhood autism. JAMA 309, 1696–1703. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.2270

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Clayton-Smith, J., and Donnai, D. (1995). Fetal valproate syndrome. J. Med. Genet. 32, 724–727. doi: 10.1136/jmg.32.9.724

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Courchesne, E., Campbell, K., and Solso, S. (2011). Brain growth across the life span in autism: age-specific changes in anatomical pathology. Brain Res. 1380, 138–145. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.09.101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

dos Reis, E. R., Nicola, E. M. D., and Nicola, J. H. (2005). Harderian gland of wistar rats revised as a protoporphyrin IX producer. Braz. J. Morphol. Sci. 22, 43–51.

Dufour-Rainfray, D., Vourc'h, P., Le Guisquet, A. M., Garreau, L., Ternant, D., Bodard, S., et al. (2010). Behavior and serotonergic disorders in rats exposed prenatally to valproate: a model for autism. Neurosci. Lett. 470, 55–59. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.12.054

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Emond, A., Emmett, P., Steer, C., and Golding, J. (2010). Feeding symptoms, dietary patterns, and growth in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 126, e337–e342. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-2391

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Evans, D. W., Canavera, K., Kleinpeter, F. L., Maccubbin, E., and Taga, K. (2005). The fears, phobias and anxieties of children with autism spectrum disorders and down syndrome: comparisons with developmentally and chronologically age matched children. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 36, 3–26. doi: 10.1007/s10578-004-3619-x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Evans, E. W., Must, A., Anderson, S. E., Curtin, C., Scampini, R., Maslin, M., et al. (2012). Dietary patterns and body mass index in children with autism and typically developing children. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 6, 399–405. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.06.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Foley, A. G., Gannon, S., Rombach-Mullan, N., Prendergast, A., Barry, C., Cassidy, A. W., et al. (2012). Class I histone deacetylase inhibition ameliorates social cognition and cell adhesion molecule plasticity deficits in a rodent model of autism spectrum disorder. Neuropharmacology 63, 750–760. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.05.042

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Folsom, T. D., and Fatemi, S. H. (2013). The involvement of reelin in neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuropharmacology 68, 122–135. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.08.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fombonne, E. (2009). Epidemiology of pervasive developmental disorders. Pediatr. Res. 65, 591–598. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31819e7203

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frisch, C., Husch, K., Angenstein, F., Kudin, A., Kunz, W., Elger, C. E., et al. (2009). Dose-dependent memory effects and cerebral volume changes after in utero exposure to valproate in the rat. Epilepsia 50, 1432–1441. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2008.01943.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gaigg, S. B., and Bowler, D. M. (2007). Differential fear conditioning in Asperger's syndrome: implications for an amygdala theory of autism. Neuropsychologia 45, 2125–2134. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.01.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gandal, M. J., Edgar, J. C., Ehrlichman, R. S., Mehta, M., Roberts, T. P., and Siegel, S. J. (2010). Validating gamma oscillations and delayed auditory responses as translational biomarkers of autism. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 1100–1106. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Go, H. S., Kim, K. C., Choi, C. S., Jeon, S. J., Kwon, K. J., Han, S. H., et al. (2012). Prenatal exposure to valproic acid increases the neural progenitor cell pool and induces macrocephaly in rat brain via a mechanism involving the gsk-3beta/beta-catenin pathway. Neuropharmacology 63, 1028–1041. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.07.028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gonzales, M. L., and LaSalle, J. M. (2010). The role of mecp2 in brain development and neurodevelopmental disorders. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 12, 127–134. doi: 10.1007/s11920-010-0097-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gregory, E. H., and Pfaff, D. W. (1971). Development of olfactory-guided behavior in infant rats. Physiol. Behav. 6, 573–576. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(71)90208-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haddad, P. M., Das, A., Ashfaq, M., and Wieck, A. (2009). A review of valproate in psychiatric practice. Expert Opin. Drug Metab. Toxicol. 5, 539–551. doi: 10.1517/17425250902911455

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hara, Y., Maeda, Y., Kataoka, S., Ago, Y., Takuma, K., and Matsuda, T. (2012). Effect of prenatal valproic acid exposure on cortical morphology in female mice. J. Pharmacol. Sci. 118, 543–546. doi: 10.1254/jphs.12025SC

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harkness, J. E., and Ridgway, M. D. (1980). Chromodacryorrhea in laboratory rats (rattus norvegicus): etiologic considerations. Lab. Anim. Sci. 30, 841–844.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hendrickx, A. G., Nau, H., Binkerd, P., Rowland, J. M., Rowland, J. R., Cukierski, M. J., et al. (1988). Valproic acid developmental toxicity and pharmacokinetics in the rhesus monkey: an interspecies comparison. Teratology 38, 329–345. doi: 10.1002/tera.1420380405

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hofvander, B., Delorme, R., Chaste, P., Nyden, A., Wentz, E., Stahlberg, O., et al. (2009). Psychiatric and psychosocial problems in adults with normal-intelligence autism spectrum disorders. BMC Psychiatry 9:35. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-9-35

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hugo, J., Krijt, J., Vokurka, M., and Janousek, V. (1987). Secretory response to light in rat harderian gland: possible photoprotective role of harderian porphyrin. Gen. Physiol. Biophys. 6, 401–404.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Ingram, J. L., Peckham, S. M., Tisdale, B., and Rodier, P. M. (2000). Prenatal exposure of rats to valproic acid reproduces the cerebellar anomalies associated with autism. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 22, 319–324. doi: 10.1016/S0892-0362(99)00083-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kaffman, A., and Krystal, J. H. (2012). New frontiers in animal research of psychiatric illness. Methods Mol. Biol. 829, 3–30. doi: 10.1007/978-1-61779-458-2_1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kalkman, H. O. (2012). A review of the evidence for the canonical wnt pathway in autism spectrum disorders. Mol. Autism 3:10. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-3-10

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv. Child 2, 217–250.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kataoka, S., Takuma, K., Hara, Y., Maeda, Y., Ago, Y., and Matsuda, T. (2013). Autism-like behaviours with transient histone hyperacetylation in mice treated prenatally with valproic acid. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 16, 91–103. doi: 10.1017/S1461145711001714

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kemper, T. L., and Bauman, M. (1998). Neuropathology of infantile autism. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 57, 645–652. doi: 10.1097/00005072-199807000-00001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kerins, C. A., Carlson, D. S., McIntosh, J. E., and Bellinger, L. L. (2003). Meal pattern changes associated with temporomandibular joint inflammation/pain in rats; analgesic effects. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 75, 181–189. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(03)00072-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, K. C., Kim, P., Go, H. S., Choi, C. S., Park, J. H., Kim, H. J., et al. (2013). Male-specific alteration in excitatory post-synaptic development and social interaction in pre-natal valproic acid exposure model of autism spectrum disorder. J. Neurochem. 124, 832–843. doi: 10.1111/jnc.12147

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, K. C., Kim, P., Go, H. S., Choi, C. S., Yang, S. I., Cheong, J. H., et al. (2011). The critical period of valproate exposure to induce autistic symptoms in sprague-dawley rats. Toxicol. Lett. 201, 137–142. doi: 10.1016/j.toxlet.2010.12.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kini, U. (2006). Fetal valproate syndrome: a review. Paediatr. Perinat. Drug Ther. 7, 123–130. doi: 10.1185/146300906X112908

CrossRef Full Text

Kolozsi, E., Mackenzie, R. N., Roullet, F. I., deCatanzaro, D., and Foster, J. A. (2009). Prenatal exposure to valproic acid leads to reduced expression of synaptic adhesion molecule neuroligin 3 in mice. Neuroscience 163, 1201–1210. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.07.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kozma, C. (2001). Valproic acid embryopathy: report of two siblings with further expansion of the phenotypic abnormalities and a review of the literature. Am. J. Med. Genet. 98, 168–175.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kuemerle, B., Gulden, F., Cherosky, N., Williams, E., and Herrup, K. (2007). The mouse engrailed genes: a window into autism. Behav. Brain Res. 176, 121–132. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.09.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lakatosova, S., and Ostatnikova, D. (2012). Reelin and its complex involvement in brain development and function. Int. J. Biochem. Cell Biol. 44, 1501–1504. doi: 10.1016/j.biocel.2012.06.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Loscher, W. (1999). Valproate: a reappraisal of its pharmacodynamic properties and mechanisms of action. Prog. Neurobiol. 58, 31–59. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00075-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Loscher, W. (2002). Basic pharmacology of valproate: a review after 35 years of clinical use for the treatment of epilepsy. CNS Drugs 16, 669–694. doi: 10.2165/00023210-200216100-00003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

MacNeil, B. M., Lopes, V. A., and Minnes, P. M. (2009). Anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 3, 1–21. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2008.06.001

CrossRef Full Text

Markram, H., Rinaldi, T., and Markram, K. (2007). The intense world syndrome - an alternative hypothesis for autism. Front. Neurosci. 1, 77–96. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.1.1.006.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Markram, K., and Markram, H. (2010). The intense world theory - a unifying theory of the neurobiology of autism. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:224. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00224

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Markram, K., Rinaldi, T., La Mendola, D., Sandi, C., and Markram, H. (2008). Abnormal fear conditioning and amygdala processing in an animal model of autism. Neuropsychopharmacology 33, 901–912. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301453

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matsunami, N., Hadley, D., Hensel, C. H., Christensen, G. B., Kim, C., Frackelton, E., et al. (2013). Identification of rare recurrent copy number variants in high-risk autism families and their prevalence in a large asd population. PLoS ONE 8:e52239. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052239

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McAlonan, G. M., Daly, E., Kumari, V., Critchley, H. D., van Amelsvoort, T., Suckling, J., et al. (2002). Brain anatomy and sensorimotor gating in Asperger's syndrome. Brain 125, 1594–1606. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf150

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mehta, M. V., Gandal, M. J., and Siegel, S. J. (2011). Mglur5-antagonist mediated reversal of elevated stereotyped, repetitive behaviors in the vpa model of autism. PLoS ONE 6:e26077. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026077

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miles, J. H., and Hillman, R. E. (2000). Value of a clinical morphology examination in autism. Am. J. Med. Genet. 91, 245–253.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Modi, M. E., and Young, L. J. (2012). The oxytocin system in drug discovery for autism: animal models and novel therapeutic strategies. Horm. Behav. 61, 340–350. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moldin, S. O., and Rubenstein, J. L. R. (2006). Understanding Autism: from Basic Neuroscience to Treatment, (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press Taylor & Francis Group). doi: 10.1201/9781420004205

CrossRef Full Text

Monteforte, R., Santillo, A., Di Giovanni, M., D'Aniello, A., Di Maro, A., and Chieffi Baccari, G. (2009). D-Aspartate affects secretory activity in rat harderian gland: molecular mechanism and functional significance. Amino Acids 37, 653–664.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Moore, S. J., Turnpenny, P., Quinn, A., Glover, S., Lloyd, D. J., Montgomery, T., et al. (2000). A clinical study of 57 children with fetal anticonvulsant syndromes. J. Med. Genet. 37, 489–497. doi: 10.1136/jmg.37.7.489

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muris, P., Steerneman, P., Merckelbach, H., Holdrinet, I., and Meesters, C. (1998). Comorbid anxiety symptoms in children with pervasive developmental disorders. J. Anxiety Disord. 12, 387–393. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(98)00022-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mychasiuk, R., Richards, S., Nakahashi, A., Kolb, B., and Gibb, R. (2012). Effects of rat prenatal exposure to valproic acid on behaviour and neuro-anatomy. Dev. Neurosci. 34, 268–276. doi: 10.1159/000341786

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nanson, J. L. (1992). Autism in fetal alcohol syndrome: a report of six cases. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 16, 558–565. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1992.tb01417.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Narita, M., Oyabu, A., Imura, Y., Kamada, N., Yokoyama, T., Tano, K., et al. (2010). Nonexploratory movement and behavioral alterations in a thalidomide or valproic acid-induced autism model rat. Neurosci. Res. 66, 2–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2009.09.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nau, H., Kuhnz, W., Egger, H. J., Rating, D., and Helge, H. (1982). Anticonvulsants during pregnancy and lactation. Transplacental, maternal and neonatal pharmacokinetics. Clin. Pharmacokinet. 7, 508–543. doi: 10.2165/00003088-198207060-00003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ornoy, A. (2009). Valproic acid in pregnancy: how much are we endangering the embryo and fetus? Reprod. Toxicol. 28, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.reprotox.2009.02.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Perry, W., Minassian, A., Lopez, B., Maron, L., and Lincoln, A. (2007). Sensorimotor gating deficits in adults with autism. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 482–486. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rasalam, A. D., Hailey, H., Williams, J. H., Moore, S. J., Turnpenny, P. D., Lloyd, D. J., et al. (2005). Characteristics of fetal anticonvulsant syndrome associated autistic disorder. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 47, 551–555. doi: 10.1017/S0012162205001076

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rinaldi, T., Kulangara, K., Antoniello, K., and Markram, H. (2007). Elevated nmda receptor levels and enhanced postsynaptic long-term potentiation induced by prenatal exposure to valproic acid. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 13501–13506. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0704391104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rinaldi, T., Perrodin, C., and Markram, H. (2008a). Hyper-connectivity and hyper-plasticity in the medial prefrontal cortex in the valproic acid animal model of autism. Front. Neural Circuits 2:4. doi: 10.3389/neuro.04.004.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rinaldi, T., Silberberg, G., and Markram, H. (2008b). Hyperconnectivity of local neocortical microcircuitry induced by prenatal exposure to valproic acid. Cereb. Cortex 18, 763–770. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm117

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rodier, P. M. (2002). Converging evidence for brain stem injury in autism. Dev. Psychopathol. 14, 537–557. doi: 10.1017/S0954579402003085

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rodier, P. M. (2004). 2003 warkany lecture: autism as a birth defect. Birth Defects Res. A Clin. Mol. Teratol. 70, 1–6. doi: 10.1002/bdra.10152

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rodier, P. M., Ingram, J. L., Tisdale, B., and Croog, V. J. (1997). Linking etiologies in humans and animal models: studies of autism. Reprod. Toxicol. 11, 417–422. doi: 10.1016/S0890-6238(97)80001-U

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rodier, P. M., Ingram, J. L., Tisdale, B., Nelson, S., and Romano, J. (1996). Embryological origin for autism: developmental anomalies of the cranial nerve motor nuclei. J. Comp. Neurol. 370, 247–261.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Roullet, F. I., and Crawley, J. N. (2011). Mouse models of autism: testing hypotheses about molecular mechanisms. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 187–212. doi: 10.1007/7854_2010_113

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roullet, F. I., Lai, J. K., and Foster, J. A. (2013). In utero exposure to valproic acid and autism–a current review of clinical and animal studies. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 36, 47–56. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2013.01.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roullet, F. I., Wollaston, L., Decatanzaro, D., and Foster, J. A. (2010). Behavioral and molecular changes in the mouse in response to prenatal exposure to the anti-epileptic drug valproic acid. Neuroscience 170, 514–522. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.06.069

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schendel, D. E., Autry, A., Wines, R., and Moore, C. (2009). The co-occurrence of autism and birth defects: prevalence and risk in a population-based cohort. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 51, 779–786. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03310.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schneider, T., Labuz, D., and Przewlocki, R. (2001). Nociceptive changes in rats after prenatal exposure to valproic acid. Pol. J. Pharmacol. 53, 531–534.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Schneider, T., and Przewlocki, R. (2005). Behavioral alterations in rats prenatally exposed to valproic acid: animal model of autism. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 80–89. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300518

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schneider, T., Turczak, J., and Przewlocki, R. (2006). Environmental enrichment reverses behavioral alterations in rats prenatally exposed to valproic acid: issues for a therapeutic approach in autism. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 36–46.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Schneider, T., Ziolkowska, B., Gieryk, A., Tyminska, A., and Przewlocki, R. (2007). Prenatal exposure to valproic acid disturbs the enkephalinergic system functioning, basal hedonic tone, and emotional responses in an animal model of autism. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 193, 547–555. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0795-y

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Settipani, C. A., Puleo, C. M., Conner, B. T., and Kendall, P. C. (2012). Characteristics and anxiety symptom presentation associated with autism spectrum traits in youth with anxiety disorders. J. Anxiety Disord. 26, 459–467. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.01.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Silva, G. T., Le Bé, J.-V., Riachi, I., Rinaldi, T., Markram, K., and Markram, H. (2009). Enhanced long term microcircuit plasticity in the valproic acid animal model of autism. Front. Synaptic Neurosci. 1:1. doi: 10.3389/neuro.19.001.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stanton, M. E., Peloso, E., Brown, K. L., and Rodier, P. (2007). Discrimination learning and reversal of the conditioned eyeblink reflex in a rodent model of autism. Behav. Brain Res. 176, 133–140. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.10.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stromland, K., Nordin, V., Miller, M., Akerstrom, B., and Gillberg, C. (1994). Autism in thalidomide embryopathy: a population study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 36, 351–356. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1994.tb11856.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sui, L., and Chen, M. (2012). Prenatal exposure to valproic acid enhances synaptic plasticity in the medial prefrontal cortex and fear memories. Brain Res. Bull. 87, 556–563. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.01.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sullivan, P. F., Daly, M. J., and O'Donovan, M. (2012). Genetic architectures of psychiatric disorders: the emerging picture and its implications. Nat. Rev. Genet. 13, 537–551. doi: 10.1038/nrg3240

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tashiro, Y., Oyabu, A., Imura, Y., Uchida, A., Narita, N., and Narita, M. (2011). Morphological abnormalities of embryonic cranial nerves after in utero exposure to valproic acid: implications for the pathogenesis of autism with multiple developmental anomalies. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 29, 359–364. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2011.03.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Steensel, F. J., Bogels, S. M., and Perrin, S. (2011). Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 14, 302–317.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Vorhees, C. V. (1987). Behavioral teratogenicity of valproic acid: selective effects on behavior after prenatal exposure to rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 92, 173–179. doi: 10.1007/BF00177911

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wagner, G. C., Reuhl, K. R., Cheh, M., McRae, P., and Halladay, A. K. (2006). A new neurobehavioral model of autism in mice: pre- and postnatal exposure to sodium valproate. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 779–793. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0117-y

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weintraub, K. (2011). The prevalence puzzle: autism counts. Nature 479, 22–24. doi: 10.1038/479022a

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Williams, E. L., and Casanova, M. F. (2011). Above genetics: lessons from cerebral development in autism. Transl. Neurosci. 2, 106–120. doi: 10.2478/s13380-011-0016-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Xu, J. Y., Xia, Q. Q., and Xia, J. (2012). A review on the current neuroligin mouse models. Sheng Li Xue Bao 64, 550–562.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Zahir, F. R., and Brown, C. J. (2011). Epigenetic impacts on neurodevelopment: pathophysiological mechanisms and genetic modes of action. Pediatr. Res. 69, 92R–100R. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e318213565e

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhang, Y., Sun, Y., Wang, F., Wang, Z., Peng, Y., and Li, R. (2012). Downregulating the canonical wnt/beta-catenin signaling pathway attenuates the susceptibility to autism-like phenotypes by decreasing oxidative stress. Neurochem. Res. 37, 1409–1419. doi: 10.1007/s11064-012-0724-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: autism, valproic acid, VPA, animal model, rat, teratogen, development

Citation: Favre MR, Barkat TR, LaMendola D, Khazen G, Markram H and Markram K (2013) General developmental health in the VPA-rat model of autism. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:88. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00088

Received: 01 May 2013; Accepted: 01 July 2013;
Published online: 24 July 2013.

Edited by:

Nuno Sousa, University of Minho, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Edward S. Brodkin, Perelman School of Medicine at University of Pennsylvania, USA
Jane A. Foster, McMaster University, Canada

Copyright © 2013 Favre, Barkat, LaMendola, Khazen, Markram and Markram. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Kamila Markram, Brain Mind Institute-SV-Laboratory of Neural Microcircuits, École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, Station 15, 1015 Lausanne, Switzerland e-mail: kamila.markram@epfl.ch

Download