Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 16 October 2015
Sec. Motivation and Reward
Volume 9 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00271

Developmental role of acetylcholinesterase in impulse control in zebrafish

  • 1School of Biological and Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK
  • 2School of Health Sciences and Social Work, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, UK
  • 3Centre for Clinical and Diagnostic Oral Sciences, Institute of Dentistry, Barts and The London School of Medicine and Dentistry, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK

Cellular and molecular processes that mediate individual variability in impulsivity, a key behavioral component of many neuropsychiatric disorders, are poorly understood. Zebrafish heterozygous for a nonsense mutation in ache (achesb55/+) showed lower levels of impulsivity in a 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT) than wild type and ache+∕+. Assessment of expression of cholinergic (nAChR), serotonergic (5-HT), and dopamine (DR) receptor mRNA in both adult and larval (9 dpf) achesb55/+ revealed significant downregulation of chrna2, chrna5, and drd2 mRNA in achesb55/+ larvae, but no differences in adults. Acute exposure to cholinergic agonist/antagonists had no effect on impulsivity, supporting the hypothesis that behavioral effects observed in adults were due to lasting impact of developmental alterations in cholinergic and dopaminergic signaling. This shows the cross-species role of cholinergic signaling during brain development in impulsivity, and suggests zebrafish may be a useful model for the role of cholinergic pathways as a target for therapeutic advances in addiction medicine.

Introduction

The identification of endophenotypes, as quantifiable, core components of complex behavioral traits and disease phenotypes makes genetic analysis of the pathogenesis of neuropsychiatric disease more tractable in both humans and model organisms (Burmeister et al., 2008). One such potential endophenotype is impulsivity (Urcelay and Dalley, 2012). Impulsivity not only is the hallmark symptom of a number of neuropsychiatric disorders (ADHD, addiction) but, in the case of addiction, has been shown to predict patterns of relapse and compulsive drug seeking in rats (Belin et al., 2008).

Despite the well-established role in a number of neuropsychiatric disorders, the cellular, and molecular mechanisms that underlie impulsivity are not well-understood. The cholinergic system, in particular cholinergic projections from the PFC, has long been implicated in sustained attention (Sarter et al., 2001). For example, IgG-saporin lesions of cholinergic neurons in the basal forebrain reduce sustained attention (Mcgaughy and Sarter, 1998), while systemic administration of the nAChR agonist nicotine improves performance accuracy and reduces omissions on the 5-CSRTT (Blondel et al., 2000; Hahn and Stolerman, 2002; Young et al., 2004). In addition, infusions of scopolamine (mAchR antagonist) into the medial pre-frontal cortex (mPFC), and systemic mecamylamine (nAchR antagonist) reduce response accuracy (Robbins et al., 1998). The effects of chronic elevation of ACh, however, are less clear, although Grottick and Higgins (2000) found that improved performance accuracy is apparent with chronic nicotine exposure. The effects of genetic alteration of ACh activity have not previously been tested, particularly with respect to premature responding on the 5-CSRTT.

Notwithstanding their small size, low housing costs and prolific breeding, there now exists a number of genetic tools for zebrafish research, including N-ethyl-N-nitrosourea (ENU) mutagenized lines, extensive sperm libraries, and a number of GFP/RFP lines. Despite anatomical differences between the fish and their mammalian counterparts, key neurochemical pathways are well-conserved between the species (Guo, 2004); for example, the ascending and descending midbrain catecholeminergic pathways (Guo et al., 1999).

Here, we tested the performance of ache (the gene that codes for acetylcholinesterase; AChE) deficient (achesb55/+) zebrafish, for performance characteristics on the 5-CSRTT, a task designed to test aspects of impulse control through examination of anticipatory responding. achesb55 contain a point mutation close to the catalytic site of the enzyme resulting in a replacement of serine 226 by an asparagine. Serine 226 is conserved in all ache gene family members, and is important for catalytic activity (Behra et al., 2002). Chronic alterations in cholinergic signaling with the AChE inhibitor chlorpyrifos has previously been demonstrated to increase impulsivity, make cholinergic signaling an interesting target for inquiry into the molecular mechanisms underlying impulse control (Middlemore-Risher et al., 2010; Cardona et al., 2011; Oca et al., 2012). Zebrafish have previously been shown to respond well on the 5-CSRTT (Parker et al., 2012, 2013, 2014).

Materials and Methods

Ethics Statement

All experimental procedures, including drug dosing, and behavioral testing, were carried out under the Animals (Scientific procedures) Act (1984). The procedures carried out conformed both to local ethical guidelines and to the terms of a project license from the UK Home Office. In addition, all experiments were approved by the Queen Mary Animals Welfare and Ethical Review Board.

Subjects

Twenty-nine (n = 10 achesb55/+ (Ninkovic et al., 2006), n = 19 Tubingen wild-type), adult zebrafish (age = 6 months; mixed sex) were selected for the first part of the study (5-s fixed interval PSI), and 12 adult zebrafish (age = 5 months; mixed sex; n = 5 achesb55/+; n = 7 ache+∕+) were selected for the second part (Variable PSI). All were sourced initially from the Sanger Institute (Cambridge, UK), and bred and reared in the aquarium facility at Queen Mary University of London according to standard protocols (Westerfield, 1993). During the entire experimental period, fish were fed artemia/bloodworm mix during testing trials, and this was supplemented with flake food/artemia in the evenings and at weekends.

Apparatus

Figure 1 displays the 5-CSRTT tanks used in the study. The shell of the testing tanks was constructed from opaque acrylic, as were the central gates. The lights were LEDs (magazine light green, stimulus aperture lights yellow). The reinforcer used was artemia liquidized with bloodworm, suspended in aquarium-treated water (R-O water with added salts). The food was delivered via a plastic syringe fitted with a 1 mm diameter rubber catheter tube, which was driven by a linear stepper motor (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Five-choice serial reaction time task testing unit and the constituent parts. (A) The pneumatic gate mechanism. (B) The 5-choice apparatus viewed from the perspective of the camera. (C) The stimulus light area. The stimuli were 5 white LEDs. (D) Food was delivered via activation of a linear stepper motor driving the plunger of a 1.5 ml plastic syringe. The food (liquidized bloodworm and brine-shrimp) was delivered to the fish through 1 mm latex catheter tubing. (E) The food delivery area and magazine. This comprised a green LED to act as a stimulus to signal food availability. Adapted from Parker et al. (2013).

General Procedure

The main procedure is an extension and modification of the commonly used rodent 5-CSRTT, and has been described in detail elsewhere (Parker et al., 2012, 2013, 2014).

Pre-training

Prior to commencing training, all subjects were habituated to the test room for 1 week to acclimate to the conditions. All pre-training, training and testing was carried out Monday–Friday (0800–1800), with the exception of the final stage (Stage 8, see Table 1), which was also carried out Saturday and Sunday. Training was divided into eight distinct stages (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Procedure for pre-training and training during 5-CSRTT.

During stages 1–3 (pre-training) data were collected and examined to ensure that all animals were receiving food during training. Any that did not perform the task (e.g., froze in the tank or did not approach the lights; n < 2 on any given session) had their food supplemented immediately after the session. During acclimation (Stage 1), fish were placed individually into the test tanks for 30-mins. During this all lights were illuminated and the gate was open. Immediately after acclimation, the fish were trained to enter the food magazine (Stage 2). During this stage, the gate remained closed at all times. The magazine light was illuminated for 30-s intervals, during which entry to the magazine resulted in the light turning off, and a small delivery (~20 μl) of artemia/bloodworm mix. In Stage 3 the fish were trained to approach the response apertures. Here, the gate opened to reveal all of the response apertures illuminated, and entry to any one of the apertures was conditionally reinforced with illumination of the magazine light. Subsequent entry to the food magazine was reinforced with artemia/bloodworm mix. During Stage 3 (response aperture orientation) only fish that completed 20 or more correct trials were taken forward to 5-CSRTT training.

Five-choice Serial Reaction Time Task: Phase 1

After a 2-min habituation period, the magazine light was illuminated, and entry to the food magazine initiated the trial sequence after an inter-trial interval (ITI) of 20-s1. This ITI always followed food delivery, and allowed the fish time to consume the reinforcer ration. After 20-s, the gate was raised, and one of the stimulus apertures was illuminated after a pre-stimulus interval (PSI). Entry to the correct aperture during the stimulus illumination, or during a brief pause thereafter (limited hold; LH), were conditionally reinforced by illumination of the magazine light, and the trial ended when the fish collected the food. All training sessions lasted 30-mins. For the first 4 weeks (Stage 4) the fish were trained with 30-s stimulus duration, a PSI of 1-s and a 1-s limited hold period. At all times during training and testing, the magazine light remained illuminated for 30-s following a correct response, after which magazine entry was not reinforced. During the second stage of 5-CSRTT training (Stage 5) the stimulus duration was reduced to 10-s, the PSI was increased to 5-s and limited hold remained at 1-s. The criterion for moving from each stage to the next was that the fish had reached a steady-state response, operationalized as completing >20 trials per session over 5-consecutive sessions. Any fish not meeting this criterion were excluded from the subsequent stage.

Long PSI Stage

There were three long PSI sessions, during which the PSI was increased to 7-s. All other test parameters remained the same as during Stage 5 (stimulus duration = 10-s, limited hold = 1-s). The three long PSI sessions were interspersed by two baseline sessions (Stage 5; PSI = 5-s, stimulus duration = 10-s, limited hold = 1-s). During the long PSI sessions, the length of the session was increased to 35 min. The criterion for a fish progressing to the long PSI phase of the experiment was that they reached steady state responding, again, operationally defined as having completed five sessions of >20 trials prior to testing. Any fish that did not meet this criterion were excluded from the testing phase.

Five-choice Serial Reaction Time Task: Phase 2

For the second phase of the experiment, we trained a group of experimentally naïve fish (n = 5 achesb55/+; n = 7 ache+∕+) in an identical manner to that described above for stages 1–4. For Stage 5, we introduced 5-s variable interval (VI) PSI. All other timings were the same as in Phase 1, Stage 5 (stimulus duration = 10-s, limited hold = 1-s). There was no Long-PSI stage in Phase 2.

Acute Exposure to AChe Antagonist, and nAChR and mAChR Agonists

Trained fish (wild-type from Phase 1) were selected for the drug administration phase. The exposure schedule was organized according to a full crossover design, with each fish receiving each of the drugs over a 1-week period. Fish were initially re-trained (2-weeks) in the absence of drug to establish steady-state baseline performance (>20 reinforced trials/session, for 5 sessions). The 5-CSRTT was as before in Stage 5 (see above: stimulus-duration = 10-s, PSI = 5-s, LH = 1-s), except that in this phase we employed a variable interval (VI) 5-s PSI. During the first experiment, there was no difference between the strains during the long PSI trials, but there was a difference during the earlier stages of training. Therefore, we chose to increase the complexity of the task by using a VI-PSI during the entire training period. Immediately prior to training, fish were immersed in a pre-treatment tank (1 L) either in the drug solution or in aquarium-treated H2O for 20-mins. Drugs (nicotine: 1.54 μM [Sigma-Aldrich, UK]; pilocarpine [Sigma-Aldrich, UK]: 8.64 μM; donepezil [Sigma-Aldrich, UK]: 2.63 μM) were dissolved in aquarium-treated H2O. Doses of donepezil, nicotine and pilocarpine were selected based on previous work on attention/impulsivity (Day et al., 2007; Brembs, 2009; Cardona et al., 2011). The dose of donepezil was also based on an initial assessment of brain levels of ACh and AChE following drug administration to determine a dose that best reflected the ACh and AChE levels in achesb55∕+ (Ninkovic et al., 2006).

Brain activity of AChE and brain levels of ACh were assessed in wild-type fish exposed to 2.63 μM donepezil or aquarium-treated H2O for 20 mins using a fluorescence-based approach (George et al., 1961). Following exposure to drug fish were placed in a recovery tank for 5-min, and then killed by immersion in ice water. Brains were immediately removed, weighed and homogenized in ice-cold Tris-HCl (pH 8). Samples were then centrifuged (20-min at 13,000 rpm) and AChE and ACh was assessed from the resulting supernatant using Amplex Red Acetylcholine/Acetylcholinesterase assay kit (Molecular Probes, Invitrogen Detection Technologies, Paisley, UK) according to manufacturer's instructions. Briefly, AChE converts ACh into choline, which is then oxidized by choline oxidase to betaine and H2O2. Brain activity of AChE and brain levels of ACh were measured using 10-acetyl-3, 7-dihydroxyphenoxazine, a flourogenic probe for H2O2. All ACh and AChE samples were examined in duplicate against standards and fluorescence was measured on a fluorescence microplate reader (FLUOstar OPTIMA, BMG LABTECH, Cary, NC). Following exposure to 2.63 μM donepezil, the levels of ACh were found to be higher in the drug group [11.8 nM/g vs. 7.1 nM/g; t(8) = 2.81, P = 0.02], which was directly comparable to levels seen in the achesb55/+ thus validating the dose used (Ninkovic et al., 2006).

The exposure schedule was as follows: Week 1: drug A, Week 2: recovery (no drug), Week 3: drug B, Week 4: recovery, Week 5: drug C. As stated, each fish was tested in the presence of each of the three drugs, the order of which was counterbalanced across weeks.

Gene Expression Changes in achesb55/+

We collected embryos from 4 × achesb55∕+ in-crosses. All homozygous individuals were removed at 72 hpf (easily identifiable by morphological features and lack of motor activity) leaving petri dishes with ~2/3 heterozygous individuals. We also collected embryos from 4 × ache+∕+ in-crosses for comparison. Reference genes used were β-actin, ef1α, and rpl13α based on previous findings findings (Tang et al., 2007). Target genes used are listed in Table 2. All embryos were manually sorted to ensure all were at the same developmental stage over the first 72 hpf, and grown to 9 dpf in petri dishes (~40/dish) in an incubator (28°C). At 9 dpf embryos were terminally anesthetized in MS-222, and placed in RNAlater until assay (4°C). Eight batches of n = 3 embryos per strain (achesb55∕+ and ache+∕+) were lysed in 200 μl Lysis buffer with 2 μl Proteinase K for 30–45 min (55°C). mRNA was isolated using 40 μl Dynabeads® Oligo(dT)25 according to manufacturer's instructions. Ten adult (6 months) brains (n = 5 achesb55/+; n = 5 ache+∕+) were homogenized in 400 μl Lysis buffer with 4 μl Proteinase K for 30-min (55°C). mRNA was isolated using 80 μl Dynabeads® Oligo(dT)25 according to manufacturer's instructions. All qPCR reactions were carried out in triplicate. 1 μl of cDNA and 1.5 μl each of forward and reverse primers (see Table 2) were added to 5 μl SYBR® Green PCR Master mix and run in a 384-well plate format (Roche Diagnostics). Method reported in full elsewhere (Gemenetzidis et al., 2010) (Teh et al., 2013).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Primer pairs for all reference and target genes examined in quantitative real-time PCR analysis.

Data Analysis

5-CSRTT data were fitted to general linear models (fit by REML), with time (5-CSRTT phases 1–5) and strain (either achesb55/+ vs. ache+∕+ or achesb55/+ vs. wild-type) as fixed effects. In the drug administration phase, drug (four-levels, nicotine, pilocarpine, donepezil, and control) was added as a fixed factor, with ID and day as random effects. In each case, the dependent measure was calculated from performance in the 5-CSRTT:

• Correct; calculated as: correct(correct+incorrect)

• Omissions; calculated as: omissions(correct+incorrect+omissions)

• Premature; calculated as: premature(correct+incorrect+omissions+premature)

Post-hoc Tukey tests were carried out to examine main effects and interactions of 5-CSRTT data.

Finally, to test the difference between levels of mRNA expression in larvae and adult achesb55/+ and ache+∕+ siblings, we carried out a series of Mann–Whitney U-tests, with strain (achesb55/+ vs. ache+∕+) as the independent variable and target gene expression, relative to reference genes, as the dependent variables. For mRNA expression data, P values were estimated following Bonferroni correction for multiple comparison. Effect sizes for all differences in expression were also calculated using the Grissom and Kim (2012) method. Descriptive statistics are reported as mean ± SEM unless otherwise stated. A type-1 error rate of α = 0.05 was adopted for all statistical tests. All data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics v.21 for Macintosh.

Results

achesb55/+ Show Higher Levels of Responding During Pre-training

The achesb55/+ heterozygotes were selected by systematic in-crosses, the mutation being homozygous-lethal. There was a main effect for day, F(4, 35) = 3.42, P < 0.02. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons revealed that there was a significant increase after Day 1 (Ps ≤ 0.05), but no change thereafter (Ps > 0.6). There was also a significant main effect for strain, F(1, 85) = 5.61, P < 0.01, with the achesb55/+ making significantly more response than the wild-type (Figure 2A). There was no day × strain interaction (F < 1). Of the original 39 fish, 3 of the achesb55/+ (30%) and 8 of the 19 wild-type (42%) failed to meet criteria (i.e., < 20 reinforcers were received).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Five-choice serial reaction time task data. (A) ache sb55∕+ receive more reinforcers in the stimulus-light training session that TU wild-type fish; (B) achesb55/+ perform a lower proportion of anticipatory responses during 5-CSRTT training than TU wild-type; (C) ache sb55/+ perform a lower proportion of anticipatory responses in 5-CSRTT than ache +∕+; (D) 1.54 uM nicotine increases proportion of correct responses during 5-CSRTT in TU wild-type fish. Note: *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01.

achesb55/+ Show Lower Levels of Premature Responding in Long Fixed-interval and variable-interval PSIs

The rates of correct responses, omissions and premature responding were comparable with our previously published work with zebrafish (Parker et al., 2012, 2013, 2014). There was a significant main effect of phase for correct responses, F(4, 24) = 23.61, P < 0.01. Post-hoc tests revealed that the proportion of correct responses increased after phase 1 (phase 1 < phases 3, 4 and long-PSI, Ps < 0.01, but not phase 2, P = 0.06) and phase 2 (phase 2 < phases 3, 4 and long-PSI, Ps > 0.01), but there was no difference between phases 3, 4 and long-PSI (Ps > 0.14). There was no main effect of strain (achesb55/+ = 0.52±0.02, wild-type = 0.52±0.02), F < 1, nor a significant phase × strain interaction, F < 1.

The rates of premature responding were comparable with our previous studies (Parker et al., 2012, 2013, 2014). There was a significant effect of phase, F(4, 20) = 37.17, P < 0.01. Post-hoc test revealed that phase 1 < phases 2, 3, 4 and long-PSI (Ps < 0.01), phase 2 < phases 3, 4 and long-PSI (Ps < 0.01), phase 3 = phase 4 (P = 0.3), and subjects performed more premature responses in the long-PSI phase than phases 3 and 4 (Ps < 0.05). There was also a significant main effect of strain (Figure 2B), F(1, 28) = 5.07, P = 0.03, with the achesb55/+ performing a lower proportion of premature responses than the wild-type. There was no significant phase × strain interaction, F(4, 20) = 2.11, P = 0.12.

Rates of omissions were again comparable with our previous study (Parker et al., 2012, 2013, 2014). There were significant main effects of phase, F(4, 27) = 22.02, P < 0.01. Post-hoc tests revealed that phase 1 < phases 2, 3, 4 and long-PSI (Ps < 0.01), and phase 2 > phases 3 and 4 (Ps < 0.04), but not long-PSI (P = 0.3). Phase 3 was not significantly different from phase 4 (P = 0.14) but was significantly lower than long-PSI (P < 0.03). There was no significant effect of strain (achesb55/+ = 0.32±0.02, wild-type = 0.31 ± 0.01), F < 1, nor was there a significant phase × strain interaction, F(4, 27) = 1.85, P = 0.14.

There was a significant effect of phase on the latency to approach the stimulus for correct responses, F(4, 23) = 26.91, P < 0.01, with subjects taking longer to approach the stimulus in Phase 1 (12.69 ± 0.77 s) than in phases 2 (4.51 ± 0.27 s), 3 (5.31 ± 0.21 s), 4 (5.45 ± 0.19 s) or the long PSI phase (6.0 ± 0.18 s). There was no significant effect of strain, F < 1, nor was there a phase × strain interaction, F(4, 23) = 1.18, P = 0.35.

The number of trials completed in each session during 5-CSRTT training changed significantly according to phase, F(4, 30) = 7.96, P < 0.01, characterized as fish completing the most trials in phase 3 (40.76 ± 1.23 trials; P < 0.01), and fewer trials in the long-PSI phase (31.06 ± 1.29 trials) than in phase 4 (34.89 ± 1.29 trials; P < 0.01). There was no main effect of strain, F < 1 nor a phase × strain interaction, F < 1.

Finally, we carried out a replication with achesb55/+ heterozygotes and ache+∕+ wild-type siblings. First, fish were trained for 20 sessions (1 s fixed interval PSI), and finally with six, 5 s variable-interval (VI) PSI trials included. achesb55/+ showed a significantly lower proportion of premature responses during the VI-PSI trials, F(1, 18) = 10.48, P = 0.03 (Figure 2C). There were no differences in correct responses (achesb55/+ = 0.66±0.03; ache+∕+ = 0.61±0.02; P = 0.13), nor omissions (achesb55/+ = 0.34±0.05; ache+∕+ = 0.24±0.03; P = 0.1).

Acute Manipulation of Cholinergic Activity Increases Performance Accuracy but has No Effect on Anticipatory Responding in Adult Wild-type Zebrafish

Figure 2D shows the results of drug administration on 5-CSRTT performance in wild-type fish. There was a significant main effect of drug on correct responses, F(3, 75) = 4.01, P = 0.01. Post-hoc pairwise comparisons (α-adjusted for multiple tests) revealed that there was a significant increase from control in correct responses during the nicotine (P = 0.02) but not pilocarpine (P = 0.19) or donepezil (P = 0.85). There were no differences between nicotine and donepezil (P = 0.07), nicotine and pilocarpine (P = 0.68) or pilocarpine and donepezil (P = 0.53). There were no differences between the drugs' effects in terms of premature response rates (control = 0.126 ± 0.02; nicotine = 0.104 ± 0.03; pilocarpine = 0.103 ± 0.03; donepezil = 0.13 ± 0.03; F < 1), nor in terms of omissions (control = 0.08 ± 0.03; nicotine = 0.1 ± 0.04; pilocarpine = 0.1 ± 0.04; donepezil = 0.13 ± 0.04; F(3, 79) = 1.22, P = 0.3). There were no differences in the total number of trials completed in each session (control = 21.4 ± 0.52; nicotine = 19.2 ± 0.94; pilocarpine = 21.7 ± 0.94; donepezil = 21.4 ± 0.94; F(3, 80) = 1.77, P = 0.16). Finally, there was no effect of drug on approach latency (control = 8.6 ± 1.3; nicotine = 8.8 ± 1.5; pilocarpine = 9.1 ± 1.5; donepezil = 9.1 ± 1.5; F < 1).

achesb55/+ have Down Regulation of chrna2, chrna5, and drd2 mRNA at 9 dpf, but no Detectable Differences in Adult Expression

Finally, to help understand the mechanisms by which developmental reduction in AChE affected the observed reduction in anticipatory responding, we characterized the gene expression profile of achesb55/+ focussing on neural circuits known to be involved in impulse control. Table 3 summarizes the differences in mRNA expression for achesb55/+ heterozygotes vs. ache+∕+ wild-type siblings. We found that in the achesb55/+ heterozygotes, there was robust downregulation in chrna2, chrna5, and drd2 mRNA, the genes that code for the alpha-2, alpha-5 receptor subunits (nAChRa2, nAChRa5), and the dopamine d2 receptor subunit (DRD2), respectively. In the adults, there was no difference in expression of any of the genes we observed.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. mRNA expression for achesb55∕+ vs. ache+∕+ at 9 dpf and 6 months of age.

Discussion

The aim of this experiment was to test the hypothesis that developmental alterations in cholinergic signaling affect impulse control using a zebrafish model of the commonly used 5-CSRTT with a strain heterozygous for a missense mutation in ache (achesb55/+). We found that achesb55/+ showed a lower proportion of premature responding than achesb55/+ siblings and wild-type zebrafish. There were no significant differences in either the number of correct responses, latency to respond, number of trials or the number of omissions, although the achesb55/+ appeared to learn faster, collecting more reinforcers during pre-training. Acute reductions of AChE (donepezil) had no significant effects on premature responding, or other 5-CSRTT parameters, and acute administration of a nAChR agonist significantly increased performance accuracy, while having no effect on premature responding. Finally, achesb55/+ have a down regulation of chrna2, chrna5, and drd2 mRNA expression at 9 dpf, but no difference in expression in any of the genes we examined in adulthood. Previous studies have shown that high levels of AChE inhibition during development (e.g., with the organophosphate weedkiller chlorpyrifous Middlemore-Risher et al., 2010; Cardona et al., 2011; Oca et al., 2012) increase impulsivity in later life. Collectively, these data suggest the intriguing theory that variation in AChE during development may follow a J-curve with respect to its effects on impulse control, potentially through downstream effects on cholinergic and dopaminergic pathways.

Lesion, neuropsychological, and pharmacological studies have demonstrated that cortical cholinergic projections to mid-brain regions are strongly implicated in sustained attention and in general top-down cognitive control (Sarter et al., 2001). In particular, during 5-CSRTT performance rats display elevated ACh release from the medial pre-frontal cortex (mPFC), and phasic increases in ACh release when a visual distracter was introduced to increase task complexity (Passetti et al., 2000). We did not see any differences in the number of correct responses in our version of the task, but more of the achesb55/+ met criteria to move to the 5-CSRTT stage of training, and of those that met criteria, overall performance in terms of reinforcers gained was significantly greater than the wild-type. This finding replicates assessment of this strain's learning previously demonstrated in a T-maze task (Ninkovic et al., 2006). During this initial training stage, despite the strain difference, there was no day × strain interaction, suggesting that achesb55/+ learnt at the same rate. It may be that the achesb55/+ were more motivated to perform, or habituated faster than the wild-type. This effect was transient, however, disappearing once training started on the 5-CSRTT. We did, however, find evidence for the role of nAChR in task performance, with acute exposure to nicotine (nAChR agonist) increasing the proportion of correct responses in the task. This supports previous data from rodents (Blondel et al., 2000; Hahn and Stolerman, 2002; Young et al., 2004).

A potential mechanism for the observed differences in premature responding may relate to the role of nAChR during early brain development and patterning. nAChR subtypes, in particular α4, α5, α7, β2, and β4, are found early in brain development, and have been suggested to play a role in modulating and mediating early patterning, dendritic outgrowth and synaptogenesis (Hellström-Lindahl et al., 1998). It is possible therefore that reduction in AChE levels, as is characteristic of the achesb55/+ heterozygotes, during early brain development alter the distribution of nAChRs thus causing differences in patterning and dendritic morphology. Indeed, in zebrafish, AChE enzymatic activity has been shown to be important for both axon outgrowth and synapse stability, albeit within the neuromuscular projections of the nervous system (Behra et al., 2002; Downes and Granato, 2004).

Chronic reductions of AChE in adult rats with donepezil increases expression of α4 and α7 nAChR (Kume et al., 2005), and ACh-modulated reductions in impulsive action in the 3-CSRTT are mediated by α4 nAChR (Tsutsui-Kimura et al., 2010). Although we did not observe differences either in chrna4 or chrna7 here, we did observe robust down regulation of chrna2 and chrna5 mRNA expression in the achesb55∕+ heterozygotes at 9 dpf, but no differences in adulthood. CHRNA2 and CHRNA5 variants have been shown to predict impulsive responding in response-inhibition in humans (Rigbi et al., 2008), and transgenic mice overexpressing the Chrna3, Chrna5, Chrnb4 gene cluster show a reduction in impulsivity (Viñals et al., 2012). However, the differences in behavior observed in the achesb55/+ heterozygotes demonstrate haploinsufficiency of the AChE gene, and thus has implications for the impact of AChE mutations within the human population. Although we are yet to understand the mechanism, this may inform our exploration of potential targets for therapeutics in the future.

The functional properties of nAChRs on catecholaminergic (in particular, dopaminergic) axonal terminals alter during development, highlighting their role in the development of the dopamine system (Azam et al., 2007). It is clear that over-activation of nAChR during early development, e.g., from maternal smoking during pregnancy, can result in an increased risk for impulse control disorders (Button et al., 2007). In addition, as discussed above, excessive inhibition of AChE during development, resulting from exposure to the organophosphate insecticide chlorpyrifos, results in higher impulsivity (Middlemore-Risher et al., 2010; Cardona et al., 2011; Oca et al., 2012). Although this shows a clear link between developmental effects of cholinergic-system disruption and impulsivity, it is not clear at this stage the mechanisms by which subtle alterations, such as are seen with achesb55/+, subsequently reduces impulsivity. It is possible that this reflects species-specific differences in patterning during early brain ontogeny, although this seems unlikely based on documented similarities between fish and mammalian cholinergic system development (Xie et al., 2000; Behra et al., 2002).

During development, AChE is transiently involved with aspects of neural patterning and hodological development. For example, during cortical synaptogenesis and development of thalamo-cortical pathways, AChE activity is recorded in various brain regions (Button et al., 2007). The cholinergic system interacts with mid-brain dopamine activity in a number of ways. First, the nucleus accumbens (NAc) is densely innervated by cholinergic projection neurons (Meredith et al., 1989; Woolf, 1991). Second, cholinergic receptors [both muscarinic (mAChR) and nicotinic (nAChR)] are found on ventral tegmental area (VTA) dopamine neurons, suggesting dopaminergic control of cholinergic activity (Clarke and Pert, 1985). Third, mesolimbic cholinergic projection neurons are abundant with dopamine receptors, suggesting cholinergic mediation of dopamine activity (Gronier et al., 2000), creating a feedback loop. Rats characterized as high trait impulsivity based on baseline performance on the 5-CSRTT show a greater tendency for elevated cocaine self-administration (Dalley et al., 2007), increased compulsive cocaine seeking (Belin et al., 2008) and increased relapse to compulsive cocaine seeking following punishment-induced abstinence (Economidou et al., 2009). In addition, high impulsive rats show a reduction in DRD2/DRD3 in the ventral striatum, suggesting a potential biomarker for the addiction phenotype (Dalley et al., 2007). Interestingly, achesb55/+ have previously been characterized as showing a decrease in conditioned place preference (CPP) for amphetamine (Ninkovic et al., 2006). It is well-established, through the therapeutic efficacy of dopamine agonists such as methylphenidate in reducing impulsivity in ADHD patients (Barkley, 1997), that impulsivity is, at least in part, related to a reduction in availability of dopamine (Li et al., 2006). It is possible that genetic impairment of AChE in achesb55/+ – which results in higher levels of circulating ACh and desensitization of AChRs (Ninkovic et al., 2006) – may act to stabilize dopamine activity (Zhou et al., 2001), thus decreasing impulsive responding. However, although we observed downregulation in drd2 mRNA in 9 dpf achesb55/+ embryos, there was no significant differences in the adults. This requires further exploration in order to elucidate the mechanism.

In rodents, low levels of premature responding in the 5-CSRTT are predictive of animals that show resistance to developing compulsive drug seeking (Belin et al., 2008) and relapse following abstinence (Economidou et al., 2009), and this has been interpreted as these animals showing low levels of trait impulsivity affecting top-down cognitive control (Dalley et al., 2011). The neural circuits of impulsivity are currently not well-understood (Brown et al., 2006; Chang et al., 2012), but these findings suggest that zebrafish, an established genetic model system, offer a means for exploration of this.

Gaining a better understanding of the etiology of psychiatric disease is currently a priority area of research (Campbell, 2010), and with current advances in neuroimaging and huge increases in genetic sequencing power this aim is beginning to be realized. For example, genome-wide association studies (GWAS) are making progress in this regard (Sullivan, 2010), but are limited by uncontrollable factors such as environmental influences and heterogeneity of diseases (Burmeister et al., 2008). Animal models have proved useful in terms of identifying molecular mechanisms of many psychiatric diseases, as symptoms consistent with DSM-IV (APA, 2000) diagnoses of psychiatric disorder have been characterized in many models (Gould and Gottesman, 2006). A better understanding of the molecular mechanisms will be helpful in tailoring treatment options for patients, but also for early identification of at-risk individuals to allow preventative measures to be adopted in the early stages of the disorder (Uhl et al., 2008). Progress in identifying molecular mechanisms, however, has remained slow. This study shows more evidence that zebrafish may be very useful in expediting this process.

In conclusion, this study has found that alterations in Ache reduce premature responding in zebrafish on the 5-CSRTT. This effect appears to relate specifically to developmental effects of reduced AChE, as acute exposure to an AChE antagonist had no effect on premature responding in the task. Molecular analyses suggest that the route of action may be through cholinergic interactions with midbrain dopamine systems during development. This study opens the door for potential large-scale forward genetic population screening of mutagenized lines of zebrafish to identify novel alleles for phenotypes such as impulsivity, which is crucial in the search for novel therapeutics and individualized medicine (Jain et al., 2011).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The Associate Editor Allan V. Kalueff declares that, despite of having collaborated with the authors Matthew O. Parker and Caroline H. Brennan, the review process was handled objectively. The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research was funded by a project grant from the National Centre for the Replacement, Reduction, and Refinement of Animals in Research (NC3Rs; UK; Grant G1000053) and by the Medical Research Council (MRC; UK; Grant G1000403). CB is a Royal Society Industrial Research Fellow (UK). We would also like to acknowledge the contributions of Dennis Ife, Jun Ma, and Chris Straw of the School of Engineering and Materials Science at Queen Mary University of London for building and engineering the automated testing arena, Dr. Fabrizio Smeraldi (Electronic Engineering and Computer Science), and Mahesh Pancholi (School of Biological and Chemical Sciences) for writing the visual tracking programme.

Footnotes

1. ^Note that in the rodent version of the 5-CSRTT, there is no gate lifted, and thus the pause prior to the stimulus presentation is an inter-trial interval. In our version of the task, the trial is initiated by the opening of the gate, and we refer to this as pre-stimulus interval (PSI).

References

APA (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.

Azam, L., Chen, Y., and Leslie, F. M. (2007). Developmental regulation of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors within midbrain dopamine neurons. Neuroscience 144, 1347. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.11.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol. Bull. 121, 65.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Behra, M., Cousin, X., Bertrand, C., Vonesch, J.-L., Biellmann, D., Chatonnet, A., et al. (2002). Acetylcholinesterase is required for neuronal and muscular development in the zebrafish embryo. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 111–118. doi: 10.1038/nn788

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belin, D., Mar, A. C., Dalley, J. W., Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (2008). High impulsivity predicts the switch to compulsive cocaine-taking. Science 320, 1352–1355. doi: 10.1126/science.1158136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blondel, A., Sanger, D. J., and Moser, P. C. (2000). Characterisation of the effects of nicotine in the five-choice serial reaction time task in rats: antagonist studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 149, 293–305. doi: 10.1007/s002130000378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brembs, B. (2009). Mushroom Bodies Regulate Habit Formation in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 19, 1351–1355. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, S. M., Manuck, S. B., Flory, J. D., and Hariri, A. R. (2006). Neural basis of individual differences in impulsivity: contributions of corticolimbic circuits for behavioral arousal and control. Emotion 6:239. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burmeister, M., Mcinnis, M. G., and Zöllner, S. (2008). Psychiatric genetics: progress amid controversy. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9, 527–540. doi: 10.1038/nrg2381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Button, T. M. M., Maughan, B., and Mcguffin, P. (2007). The relationship of maternal smoking to psychological problems in the offspring. Early Hum. Dev. 83, 727. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, P. (2010). A decade for psychiatric disorders. Nature 463:9. doi: 10.1038/463009a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Cardona, D., López-Crespo, G., Sánchez-Amate, M., Flores, P., and Sánchez-Santed, F. (2011). Impulsivity as long-term sequelae after chlorpyrifos intoxication: time course and individual differences. Neurotox. Res. 19, 128–137. doi: 10.1007/s12640-009-9149-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, S. W. C., Barack, D. L., and Platt, M. L. (2012). Mechanistic classification of neural circuit dysfunctions: insights from neuroeconomics research in animals. Biol Psychiatry 72, 101–106. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, P., and Pert, A. (1985). Autoradiographic evidence for nicotine receptors on nigrostriatal and mesolimbic dopaminergic neurons. Brain Res. 348, 355–358.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dalley, J. W., Fryer, T. D., Brichard, L., Robinson, E. S., Theobald, D. E., Laane, K., et al. (2007). Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Science 315, 1267–1270.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dalley, J. W., Everitt, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2011). Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 69, 680–694.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Day, M., Pan, J. B., Buckley, M. J., Cronin, E., Hollingsworth, P. R., Hirst, W. D., et al. (2007). Differential effects of ciproxifan and nicotine on impulsivity and attention measures in the 5-choice serial reaction time test. Biochem. Pharmacol. 73, 1123–1134.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Downes, G. B., and Granato, M. (2004). Acetylcholinesterase function is dispensable for sensory neurite growth but is critical for neuromuscular synapse stability. Dev. Biol. 270, 232–245. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2004.02.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Economidou, D., Pelloux, Y., Robbins, T. W., Dalley, J. W., and Everitt, B. J. (2009). High impulsivity predicts relapse to cocaine-seeking after punishment-induced abstinence. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 851–856. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gemenetzidis, E., Elena-Costea, D., Parkinson, E. K., Waseem, A., Wan, H., and Teh, M.-T. (2010). Induction of human epithelial stem/progenitor expansion by FOXM1. Cancer Res. 70, 9515–9526. doi: 10.1158/0008-5472.CAN-10-2173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

George, L., Ellman, K., and Valentino, A. Jr (1961). A new and rapid colorimetric determination of acetylcholinesterase activity. Biochem. Pharmacol. 7, 88–95. doi: 10.1016/0006-2952(61)90145-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gould, T. D., and Gottesman, I. I. (2006). Psychiatric endophenotypes and the development of valid animal models. Genes, Brain and Behavior 5, 113–119. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00186.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grissom, R. J., and Kim, J. J. (2012). Effect sizes for Research. Routledge: Univariate and multivariate applications.

Google Scholar

Gronier, B., Perry, K. W., and Rasmussen, K. (2000). Activation of the mesocorticolimbic dopaminergic system by stimulation of muscarinic cholinergic receptors in the ventral tegmental area. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 147, 347–355. doi: 10.1007/s002130050002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grottick, A. J., and Higgins, G. A. (2000). Effect of subtype selective nicotinic compounds on attention as assessed by the five-choice serial reaction time task. Behav. Brain Res. 117, 197–208. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00305-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, S. (2004). Linking genes to brain, behavior and neurological diseases: what can we learn from zebrafish? Genes Brain Behav. 3, 63–74. doi: 10.1046/j.1601-183X.2003.00053.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, S., Wilson, S. W., Cooke, S., Chitnis, A. B., Driever, W., and Rosenthal, A. (1999). Mutations in the zebrafish unmask shared regulatory pathways controlling the development of catecholaminergic neurons. Dev. Biol. 208, 473–487.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hahn, B., and Stolerman, I. (2002). Nicotine-induced attentional enhancement in rats: effects of chronic exposure to nicotine. Neuropsychopharmacology 27, 712–722. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00348-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hellström-Lindahl, E., Gorbounova, O., Seiger, A., Mousavi, M., and Nordberg, A. (1998). Regional distribution of nicotinic receptors during prenatal development of human brain and spinal cord. Dev. Brain Res. 108, 147–160.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jain, R. A., Wolman, M. A., Schmidt, L. A., Burgess, H. A., and Granato, M. (2011). Molecular-genetic mapping of zebrafish mutants with variable phenotypic penetrance. PLoS ONE 6:e26510. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kume, T., Sugimoto, M., Takada, Y., Yamaguchi, T., Yonezawa, A., Katsuki, H., et al. (2005). Up-regulation of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors by central-type acetylcholinesterase inhibitors in rat cortical neurons. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 527, 77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.10.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, D., Sham, P. C., Owen, M. J., and He, L. (2006). Meta-analysis shows significant association between dopamine system genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Hum. Mol. Genet. 15, 2276–2284. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddl152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mcgaughy, J., and Sarter, M. (1998). Sustained attention performance in rats with intracortical infusions of 192 IgG-saporin-induced cortical cholinergic deafferentation: effects of Physostigmine and FG 7142. Behav. Neurosci. 112, 1519. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.112.6.1519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meredith, G. E., Blank, B., and Groenewegen, H. J. (1989). The distribution and compartmental organization of the cholinergic neurons in nucleus accumbens of the rat. Neuroscience 31, 327–345.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Middlemore-Risher, M., Buccafusco, J., and Terry Jr A. (2010). Repeated exposures to low-level chlorpyrifos results in impairments in sustained attention and increased impulsivity in rats. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 32, 415–424. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2010.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ninkovic, J., Folchert, A., Makhankov, Y. V., Neuhauss, S. C., Sillaber, I., Straehle, U., et al. (2006). Genetic identification of AChE as a positive modulator of addiction to the psychostimulant D-amphetamine in zebrafish. J. Neurobiol. 66, 463–475. doi: 10.1002/neu.20231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oca, L. M. D., Moreno, M., Cardona, D., Campa, L., Suñol, C., Galofré, M., et al. (2012). Long term compulsivity on the 5-choice serial reaction time task after acute Chlorpyrifos exposure. Toxicol. Lett. 216, 73–85. doi: 10.1016/j.toxlet.2012.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, M., Millington, M., Combe, F., and Brennan, C. (2012). Development and implementation of a three-choice serial reaction time task for zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 227, 73–80. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.10.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, M. O., Brock, A. J., Sudwarts, A., and Brennan, C. H. (2014). Atomoxetine reduces anticipatory responding in a 5-choice serial reaction time task for adult zebrafish. Psychopharmacology 231, 2671–2679. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3439-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, M., Ife, D., Ma, J., Pancholi, M., Smeraldi, F., Straw, C., et al. (2013). Development and automation of a test of impulse control in zebrafish. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:65. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Passetti, F., Dalley, J., O'connell, M., Everitt, B., and Robbins, T. (2000). Increased acetylcholine release in the rat medial prefrontal cortex during performance of a visual attentional task. Eur. J. Neurosci. 12, 3051–3058. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00183.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rigbi, A., Kanyas, K., Yakir, A., Greenbaum, L., Pollak, Y., Ben−Asher, E., et al. (2008). Why do young women smoke? V. Role of direct and interactive effects of nicotinic cholinergic receptor gene variation on neurocognitive function. Genes Brain Behav. 7, 164–172. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00329.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, T. W., Granon, S., Muir, J., Durantou, F., Harrison, A., and Everitt, B. (1998). Neural Systems underlying arousal and attention: implications for drug abusea. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 846, 222–237. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09740.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarter, M., Givens, B., and Bruno, J. P. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain Res. Rev. 35, 146–160. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00044-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, P. F. (2010). The psychiatric GWAS consortium: big science comes to psychiatry. Neuron 68, 182–186. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, R., Dodd, A., Lai, D., Mcnabb, W. C., and Love, D. R. (2007). Validation of zebrafish (Danio rerio) reference genes for quantitative real-time RT-PCR normalization. Acta Biochim. Biophys. Sin. (Shanghai). 39, 384–390. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-7270.2007.00283.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teh, M. T., Hutchison, I. L., Costea, D. E., Neppelberg, E., Liavaag, P. G., Purdie, K., et al. (2013). Exploiting FOXM1−orchestrated molecular network for early squamous cell carcinoma diagnosis and prognosis. Int. J. Cancer 132, 2095–2106. doi: 10.1002/ijc.27886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsutsui-Kimura, I., Ohmura, Y., Izumi, T., Yamaguchi, T., Yoshida, T., and Yoshioka, M. (2010). Endogenous acetylcholine modulates impulsive action via α4β2 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 641, 148–153. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2010.05.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uhl, G. R., Drgon, T., Johnson, C., Fatusin, O. O., Liu, Q. R., Contoreggi, C., et al. (2008). “Higher order” addiction molecular genetics: convergent data from genome-wide association in humans and mice. Biochem. Pharmacol. 75, 98–111. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2007.06.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urcelay, G. P., and Dalley, J. W. (2012). Linking ADHD, impulsivity, and drug abuse: a neuropsychological perspective. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 173–197. doi: 10.1007/7854_2011_119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viñals, X., Molas, S., Gallego, X., Fernández-Montes, R. D., Robledo, P., Dierssen, M., et al. (2012). Overexpression of α3/α5/β4 nicotinic receptor subunits modifies impulsive-like behavior. Drug Alcohol Depend. 122, 247–252. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.09.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westerfield, M. (1993). The Zebrafish Book: A Guide for the Laboratory use of Zebrafish (Brachydanio rerio). Eugene, OR: M. Westerfield.

Woolf, N. J. (1991). Cholinergic systems in mammalian brain and spinal cord. Prog. Neurobiol. 37, 475.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Xie, W., Stribley, J. A., Chatonnet, A., Wilder, P. J., Rizzino, A., Mccomb, R. D., et al. (2000). Postnatal developmental delay and supersensitivity to organophosphate in gene-targeted mice lacking acetylcholinesterase. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 293, 896–902.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Young, J. W., Finlayson, K., Spratt, C., Marston, H. M., Crawford, N., Kelly, J. S., et al. (2004). Nicotine improves sustained attention in mice: evidence for involvement of the Œ±7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 891–900. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, F., Liang, Y., and Dani, J. A. (2001). Endogenous nicotinic cholinergic activity regulates dopamine release in the striatum. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 1224–1229. doi: 10.1038/nn769

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: 5-choice serial reaction time, impulsivity, acetylcholinesterase, Dopamine D2 receptor, zebrafish

Citation: Parker MO, Brock AJ, Sudwarts A, Teh M-T, Combe FJ and Brennan CH (2015) Developmental role of acetylcholinesterase in impulse control in zebrafish. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:271. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00271

Received: 29 July 2015; Accepted: 22 September 2015;
Published: 16 October 2015.

Edited by:

Allan V. Kalueff, ZENEREI Institute, USA & Guangdong Ocean University, China & Saint Petersburg State University, Russia

Reviewed by:

Juan Dominguez, University of Texas at Austin, USA
Denis Broock Rosemberg, Universidade Federal de Santa Maria, Brazil

Copyright © 2015 Parker, Brock, Sudwarts, Teh, Combe and Brennan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Caroline H. Brennan, School of Biological and Chemical Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, Mile End Rd, London E1 4NS, UK, c.h.brennan@qmul.ac.uk

Present Address: Fraser J. Combe, School of Science and the Environment, Manchester Metropolitan University, Manchester, UK

Download