Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Neural Circuits, 12 January 2017
Volume 10 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fncir.2016.00115

Breathing as a Fundamental Rhythm of Brain Function

Detlef H. Heck1* Samuel S. McAfee1 Yu Liu1 Abbas Babajani-Feremi1,2 Roozbeh Rezaie2 Walter J. Freeman3 James W. Wheless2 Andrew C. Papanicolaou1,2 Miklós Ruszinkó4 Yury Sokolov5 Robert Kozma5,6
  • 1Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, University of Tennessee Health Science Center, Memphis, TN, USA
  • 2Department of Pediatrics, Division of Pediatric Neurology, University of Tennessee Health Science Center and Le Bonheur Children’s Hospital Neuroscience Institute, Memphis, TN, USA
  • 3Department of Molecular and Cell Biology, Division of Neurobiology, University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, USA
  • 4Rényi Institute of Mathematics, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary
  • 5Department of Mathematical Sciences, University of Memphis, Memphis, TN, USA
  • 6Department Computer Sciences, University of Massachusetts Amherst, Amherst, MA, USA

Ongoing fluctuations of neuronal activity have long been considered intrinsic noise that introduces unavoidable and unwanted variability into neuronal processing, which the brain eliminates by averaging across population activity (Georgopoulos et al., 1986; Lee et al., 1988; Shadlen and Newsome, 1994; Maynard et al., 1999). It is now understood, that the seemingly random fluctuations of cortical activity form highly structured patterns, including oscillations at various frequencies, that modulate evoked neuronal responses (Arieli et al., 1996; Poulet and Petersen, 2008; He, 2013) and affect sensory perception (Linkenkaer-Hansen et al., 2004; Boly et al., 2007; Sadaghiani et al., 2009; Vinnik et al., 2012; Palva et al., 2013). Ongoing cortical activity is driven by proprioceptive and interoceptive inputs. In addition, it is partially intrinsically generated in which case it may be related to mental processes (Fox and Raichle, 2007; Deco et al., 2011). Here we argue that respiration, via multiple sensory pathways, contributes a rhythmic component to the ongoing cortical activity. We suggest that this rhythmic activity modulates the temporal organization of cortical neurodynamics, thereby linking higher cortical functions to the process of breathing.

We have recently shown that respiration-locked olfactory bulb activity in awake, head restrained mice causes respiration-locked delta oscillations and gamma power modulations in the somatosensory cortex (Ito et al., 2014). This unexpected direct ability of respiration-locked sensory activity to modulate oscillatory neuronal activity in the neocortex led us to consider the potential wider implications of a link between breathing and brain activity, particularly with respect to the possibility that respiration influences cortical neuronal activity underlying cognitive function.

Based on our own experimental findings, results from our modeling studies using a simple graph theory model and a review of the literature, we argue that respiration, via multiple sensory pathways, provides a subtle but continuous rhythmic modulation of cortical neuronal activity that modulates sensory, motor, emotional and cognitive processes. Specifically, we hypothesize that: (1) respiration causes respiration-locked oscillations that are synchronized across large areas of neocortex at the species-specific respiratory rhythm; (2) that increases in the power of gamma oscillations (40–100 Hz) occur preferably during certain phases of (i.e., are phase-locked to) the respiratory cycle. Both hypotheses are supported by solid experimental results in the somatosensory barrel cortex in awake mice (Ito et al., 2014; Figure 1) and by our modeling studies (see below). Additional results, published in abstract form, support the possibility that respiration-locked oscillations are also present in several other areas of mouse neocortex (Liu et al., 2015), including the visual cortex (McAfee et al., 2016).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. From Ito et al. (2014): respiratory modulation of the power of gamma frequency oscillations in mouse whisker barrel cortex. Phase–amplitude coupling between respiration-locked delta and gamma band oscillations in the barrel cortical local field potential (LFP) activity of an awake intact and an awake bulbectomized mouse, followed by population statistics. (A) Respiratory activity (top trace), amplitude of gamma band oscillations (middle trace) and delta oscillations (light green bottom trace) and its phase (dark green bottom trace) in an intact mouse. Gamma oscillation (75 Hz) amplitude peaks rhythmically phase locked to the delta cycle. (B) Gamma oscillation amplitude as a function of delta phase (red). The solid and dotted black lines indicate the mean and the 2.5 and 97.5 percentile boundaries of the surrogate amplitude distribution estimated from 1000 phase-randomized surrogates. Gamma amplitude modulation is significant at phase 0 of the delta cycle. (C,D) Same as (A,B), respectively, but for a bulbectomized mouse. After removal of the olfactory bulb, the amplitude modulation of the gamma band oscillations is no longer phase locked to respiration.

A third prediction is supported by preliminary experimental findings on sudden changes in synchronization patterns of neural activity published in abstract form (Kozma et al., 2015). We predict that (3) the timing of the sudden changes in the network activity, i.e., fast transitions between synchronized and de-synchronized network states, are phase-locked to the respiratory rhythm. These transitions can be detected as jumps in the analytic phase of oscillatory population activity using a Hilbert transform based analysis of local field potential (LFP) or electroencephalographic (EEG) activity (Freeman and Rogers, 2002; Freeman et al., 2006; Freeman, 2015). Graph theoretical arguments provide a modeling framework to describe the experimentally observed sudden changes as “phase transitions” (Puljic and Kozma, 2008; Kozma and Puljic, 2015), a term we will use throughout this article.

As we lay out in more detail below, gamma oscillations are forms of cortical activity widely linked to cognitive and other higher cortical functions. Our hypotheses predict that a consciously controlled change in respiratory behavior will cause a change in cognitive and emotional states, which is a common observation in yogic breathing (Jella and Shannahoff-Khalsa, 1993; Stancák and Kuna, 1994; Brown and Gerbarg, 2005) and stress reducing respiratory exercises such as combat tactical breathing employed by military and special forces (Grossman and Christensen, 2011). A second key prediction is that respiration-locked modulation of cortical gamma activity and phase transition timing directly links respiratory behavior to higher cortical processes, including cognitive and limbic functions, sensory perception and motor control. The respiration-locked modulation of neocortical activity we propose here would thus provide a neuronal mechanism and causal link between respiration and pain perception (Arsenault et al., 2013; Iwabe et al., 2014), motor control (Ebert et al., 2002; Rassler and Raabe, 2003; Li and Laskin, 2006; Iwamoto et al., 2010; Cao et al., 2012; Krupnik et al., 2015), attention (Gallego et al., 1991; Krupnik et al., 2015) and emotion (Benson et al., 1974; Arch and Craske, 2006; Homma and Masaoka, 2008).

Oscillations of neocortical activity in the gamma (30–100 Hz) frequency range, have been strongly implicated in affective and cognitive brain functions such as attention (Fries et al., 2001; Laufs et al., 2003; Tallon-Baudry, 2004), sensory perception (Engel et al., 2001; Tallon-Baudry, 2003; Gould et al., 2012), decision making (Kay and Beshel, 2010; Siegel et al., 2011; Gould et al., 2012; van Vugt et al., 2012; Wyart et al., 2012; Nácher et al., 2013), problem solving (Sheth et al., 2009), memory formation (Marshall et al., 2006; Tort et al., 2009; Chauvette et al., 2012) and language processing (Crone et al., 2001; Towle et al., 2008; Babajani-Feremi et al., 2014).

Sudden changes in network synchronization are characteristic features of cortical activity that have been widely linked to cognitive processes (Kozma and Freeman, 2016). Detailed analysis of rabbit and human intracranial electrocorticography (ECoG) signals revealed discontinuities in the analytic phase determined by Hilbert analysis (Freeman and Rogers, 2002; Freeman et al., 2006; Freeman, 2015). Experiments with rabbits trained with a classical conditioning paradigm showed that discontinuities of the analytic phase have cognitive relevance (Freeman, 2004; Kozma and Freeman, 2008). Namely, after delivering the conditioned stimulus, the occurrence of the phase discontinuity correlates with the stimulus, suggesting that these discontinuities can be viewed as markers of the cognitive activity (stimulus classification) performed by the rabbits.

Schölvinck et al. (2015) observed that variability of neuronal responses in the primary visual cortex to repeated identical stimuli was caused by large scale network activity, which was more variable when the network was in a synchronized state vs. an asynchronous state. Recently, Tan et al. (2014) also showed that visual stimulation shifted the activity states of the macaque primary visual cortex from synchronous to asynchronous activity. These findings are fundamentally in line with our hypothesis that the timing of such phase transitions is linked to the rhythmic sensory stimulation caused by respiration. We have obtained preliminary supporting evidence for phase-locking between respiration and phase discontinuities in human cortical activity from an analysis of ECoG signals from a human subject. We interpreted the results as phase transitions in cortical population activity between synchronized and de-synchronized states; see Kozma et al. (2015).

A small group of researchers have envisioned the possibility of respiration influencing large-scale brain activity via the olfactory system. Freeman and colleagues performed pioneering studies on the influence of respiration through olfaction on the rat brain (Eeckman and Freeman, 1990; Kay and Freeman, 1998). Effects of theta-modulation of saccadic signals have been described as visual sniffing (Kozma and Freeman, 2001). Fontanini and Bower (2006) speculated that olfactory bulb respiration-locked oscillations in rodents may propagate through the entire cortex. However, none of these earlier studies anticipated that respiration could modulate the power of gamma oscillations or considered a respiratory influence on the timing of phase transitions in cortical population activity as a mechanism that directly links respiratory behavior and cognitive brain processes.

Respiration creates both conscious and unconscious streams of rhythmic sensory inputs to the brain. Consciously accessible sensations of normal, unobstructed breathing include odor perception, the mechanical and thermal sensation of air flowing through nose, mouth and upper airways, and the proprioception of movements of the chest and abdomen. Unconscious sensory signals caused by respiration include interoceptive signals from the lungs, diaphragm and internal organs, which represent the mechanical consequences of respiratory movements, and the chemosensitive signals from the cardiovascular system, which represent breath-by-breath fluctuations of CO2 and O2 levels in the blood. The sensations and brain activity patterns associated with hunger-for-air (Liotti et al., 2001; Macey et al., 2005) are not considered here, as they represent an emergency response not related to normal, unobstructed breathing.

There are also a number of indirect ways cortical areas receive respiration-locked sensory input. Eye movements, for example, have been shown to be transiently phase-locked to respiration during sleep (Rittweger and Pöpel, 1998) as well as in the awake state (Rassler and Raabe, 2003). Recently, Ito et al. (2013) reported saccade related changes in the power of neuronal oscillatory activity in four frequency bands, including gamma, in primates that were freely viewing their environment. This suggests that the retinal flow associated with eye movements causes a modulation of power in visual cortical oscillations that is partially correlated with respiration. Another indirect respiration-locked sensory input comes from the auditory cortex, which receives rhythmic auditory input related to respiration caused by the sound of air flowing through the nose or mouth. Finally, neurons in the brain stem project broadly to thalamic nuclei (Carstens et al., 1990; Krout et al., 2002). These projections likely provide respiration-locked input to the thalamus (Chen et al., 1992), introducing a non-sensory respiratory rhythm to the thalamo-cortical network.

While there are many sources of respiration-locked activity, the olfactory system deserves special attention, because early mammals relied strongly on their olfactory sense and had proportionately large olfactory bulbs (Rowe et al., 2011). Furthermore, neuronal oscillations, particularly gamma oscillations, are a universal element of odor processing in animals as far removed from joint evolutionary ancestors as mammals and insects are (Kay, 2015). Even though in primates the olfactory sense lost the prime importance it has for most other mammals in favor of vision (Gilad et al., 2004). EEG studies comparing nasal and oral breathing of room air found that nasal breathing elicited significantly different patterns of EEG activity than mouth breathing (Servít et al., 1977; Lorig et al., 1988). This is in line with our findings of nasal air flow in mice driving delta oscillations and gamma power modulations in a non-olfactory area of neocortex (Ito et al., 2014) and suggests that the olfactory bulb activation exerts similar influence on human cortical activity.

The detection and analysis of respiration locked cortical activity requires the simultaneous measurement of respiration and brain activity. Such simultaneous measurements are not commonly performed. A notable exception is a recent study of the effects of sleep disordered breathing (SDB) in children on cortical oscillatory activity (Immanuel et al., 2014). Immanuel et al. (2014) showed that the average power of the EEG signal decreased during inspiration and increased during expiration, in a frequency band and sleep stage dependent manner, in both healthy subjects and subjects suffering from SDB. This study did, however, not evaluate phase-locking between EEG oscillations and the respiratory cycle.

Respiration related sensory activity during unobstructed breathing mainly reaches three areas of the cortex: (1) the olfactory cortex and surrounding areas receive olfactory bulb input; (2) the somatosensory cortex receives inputs from mechanoreceptors of chest, the abdominal skin and muscles that are stretched and moved by respiration; and (3) the insular cortex receives input from chemoreceptors and mechanoreceptors in the lungs, diaphragm and internal organs. Our recordings of olfactory bulb dependent respiration-locked oscillations in the mouse somatosensory cortex suggest that respiration-locked activity propagates from primary sensory areas to parts of the cortex that do not receive direct respiration related sensory inputs. A likely mode of propagation is through the cortico-cortical network itself, possibly involving also cortico-thalamic connections. However, the anatomy of axonal connections within the parabulbar and limbic areas suggest a number of subcortical regions and neuromodulator systems may also be influenced by respiration-driven sensory input. For example, widely projecting serotonergic and cholinergic neurons within the rat basal forebrain have been shown to rhythmically discharge in phase with respiration (Manns et al., 2003; Mason et al., 2007), with olfactory bulb respiration-locked activity as a likely driving force (Linster and Hasselmo, 2000). Stimulation of cholinergic neurons in particular is associated with increased neocortical gamma oscillations (Cape and Jones, 2000) a mechanism that might contribute to the respiration-locked modulation of gamma power in mouse somatosensory whisker barrel cortex (Ito et al., 2014). However, as we argue below, respiration-locked gamma power modulation may result from intrinsic properties of the cortical network itself.

The link between respiration-locked cortical oscillations and respiration-related sensory inputs to the cortex is straightforward: respiration-locked rhythmic inputs drive cortical neurons to fire rhythmically at the same frequency. Experiments in anesthetized rodents show that respiration-locked oscillations in the piriform cortex are driven this way by respiration-locked activity of olfactory bulb afferents (Fontanini and Bower, 2005; Uchida et al., 2014), which also drive respiration-locked activity in the hippocampus of mice, both under anesthesia (Yanovsky et al., 2014) and while awake and walking on a tread mill (Nguyen Chi et al., 2016). However, the mechanisms behind respiration-locked modulations of gamma power, which we observed in the mouse somatosensory cortex (Figure 1), are less obvious.

To investigate the processes leading to respiration locked increases in the power of gamma oscillations we used a simple graph theory model inspired by cortical network architecture, with a biologically appropriate balance of excitatory and inhibitory neurons and mix of short- and long-range connections. Expanding on previous work (Reijneveld et al., 2007; Turova and Villa, 2007; Gallos et al., 2012; Janson et al., 2016), we define a geometric graph, which is the combination of a regular 2-dimensional square lattice with N × N vertices, and a few additional long edges between some lattice points. The additional long edges, or “shortcuts” are selected randomly according to probability p = c/(N × d), where d is the Euclidian distance between the lattice points, and c is a constant (Janson et al., 2015). Note that this model defines a scale-free distribution of the shortcuts with power exponent 1. The expected number of long edges per node has been shown to be LAMBDA = 2c * ln(2). Next, we define an activation process on the random lattice graph, and the activation of a node at time t + 1 is denoted as Av(t + 1). Note that some of the nodes are excitatory (E), while others are inhibitory (I). In the present model, we select 25% of the nodes as inhibitory and the rest are excitatory. The update rule is defined by the so-called “k-majority”, i.e., a node becomes active at time t + 1, if more than k of its neighbors have been active at time t, while it will be inactive in the opposite case. Note that inhibitory nodes have inverse effects on excitatory nodes; namely, the activity of inhibitory nodes are subtracted from the total activation when the k-majority rule is tested (for details see Janson et al., 2015).

Our model has several parameters; the number of shortcuts (LAMBDA); the ratio of excitatory nodes (OMEGA), and threshold parameter (k). In a regular square lattice without shortcuts, the majority rule is given by k = 2. In the results shown here, we select k = 2 and k = 3 for E and I nodes, respectively. Figure 2A shows that depending on the choice of LAMBDA and OMEGA, various dynamical regimes can be modeled, such as limit cycle, non-zero fixed point (following a dampened oscillation), and zero fixed point.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Results of calculations using graph theory models of coupled excitatory-inhibitory populations; the following parameter values are used: proportion of excitatory units OMEGA = 0.75, expected number of long axonal connections (shortcuts) is LAMBDA = 0.0017. (A) Phase diagram with parameter regions with the dominance of limit cycle oscillations (purple), nonzero fixed point (light green) and zero-fixed point (blue) regimes; the yellow circle corresponds to parameter settings used in (B) plot at the edge of the limit cycle regime, close the fixed point regime. (B) Illustration of the phase-locked amplitude modulation of the gamma oscillations (of excitatory population) in response to periodic input (respiration) perturbations of increasing amplitude (RA); (Ba) RA = 0.001; (Bb) RA = 0.02; (Bc) RA = 0.03; (Bd) shape of the respiratory sinusoid signal. The amplitude modulation of the inherent high-frequency oscillation (around 60 Hz) is locked to the respiratory cycle, so that the high-frequency component has increased magnitude during the increasing segment of the input signal from its minimum value.

In order to simulate respiratory effects, we introduce a sinusoidal input with magnitude (RA). In this model we select parameters OMEGA = 0.75 and LAMBDA = 0.0017; this parameter choice is illustrated by yellow circle in Figure 2A. Examples of our simulations with varying magnitudes of perturbation are shown in Figure 2B. With very weak perturbation (RA = 0.001) we observe strong oscillations dominated by a periodic (gamma) component, see Figure 2Ba. As the magnitude of the input perturbation increases, we reach a condition when the high-frequency (gamma) component is constrained to the time segment of increasing perturbation. This shows that the graph theory model can reproduce the respiration-locked modulation of gamma power, i.e., the gamma power increases at the inhalation stage for a suitably selected input signal.

This suggests that the physiological properties of cortical network itself may be sufficient to explain the modulation of gamma power in phase with respiration-locked sensory activity. This is not to say that other factors, such as cortico-thalamic interactions or the action of neuromodulators have no role, but future research will have to determine the nature of their involvement.

Each of these forms of cortical activity appears to have different functions. Oscillatory rhythms that are phase-locked to respiration may help to synchronize large portions of the cortical network and create a temporal alignment for slower processes. The calming effect of controlled, slow and deep breathing could be due to this respiration-locked synchronization of activity across large areas of cortex, an EEG activity pattern commonly observed during meditative states (Dillbeck and Bronson, 1981; Gaylord et al., 1989). Additional evidence of respiration-locked synchronization of cortical oscillatory activity comes from a study of EEG activity during meditation with forced alternate nostril breathing, which caused an increase in interhemispheric beta coherence (Stancák and Kuna, 1994).

Few studies have evaluated cognitive processing as a function of respiratory phase. However, interactions between respiration and non-respiratory functions have been documented in humans and rodents. In humans, for example, phase-locking with respiration has been observed for visual signal detection (Flexman et al., 1974) eye movements (Rittweger and Pöpel, 1998; Rassler and Raabe, 2003), the temporal grouping of pianistic finger movements (Ebert et al., 2002), reaction time to visual (Li et al., 2012) and auditory (Gallego et al., 1991) stimuli, and grip-force (Li and Laskin, 2006). Rassler et al. (1996) reported that response latency, tracking-precision and movement duration of finger movements made to track a visual target showed significant respiratory-phase-dependent differences and that the respiratory-phase-dependence differed between finger flexion and extension movements (Rassler, 2000). In mice, movements of the mystacial whiskers are phase-locked to respiration (Cao et al., 2012; Moore et al., 2013).

Respiration has also been implicated in the modulation of pain perception. Pain-studies in humans showed that pain perception is reduced during inspiration (Arsenault et al., 2013) and that focused slow breathing reduces the perceived severity of pain (Zautra et al., 2010). Other clinical studies have shown that the strength of cortico-spinal communication assessed with transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is modulated in phase with respiration (Li and Rymer, 2011). We suggest that these interactions between respiration and sensory motor processes are mostly caused by respiration-locked fluctuations of ongoing neuronal activity in motor and sensory cortical areas.

In summary, we propose that ongoing neuronal activity of the neocortex is rhythmically modulated by respiration-locked sensory inputs. We predict three emergent patterns of cortical activity that are phase-locked to respiration and are synchronized across large areas of neocortex: (1) neuronal oscillations following the respiratory rhythm; (2) increases in gamma power phase locked to breathing; and (3) the timing of phase transitions in large scale network activity phase locked to respiration. Gamma oscillation power and phase transition timing are strongly implicated in cognitive function, directly linking breathing to cognitive processes. Our findings and hypotheses provide a new perspective of the function of respiration beyond the life-supporting exchange of gases towards a link between the states of the body and mind. This new physiological role of respiration calls for experimental designs to incorporate respiratory information and for future investigations of the interactions between respiration and cognitive, sensory and motor processes.

Author Contributions

DHH developed the original research concept, participated in data collection and analysis and wrote the manuscript. SSM participated in data collection and analysis and contributed to writing the manuscript. YL and AB-F contributed to the design of the research, to data analysis and to the writing of the manuscript. RR contributed to data collection, analysis and writing of the manuscript. WJF contributed to the design of the research, especially the modeling aspect, and to the writing of the manuscript (WJF passed away before the completion of this manuscript and is included as a posthumous author). JWW participated in the development of experimental designs, coordinated ECoG data collection and contributed to the writing of the manuscript. ACP contributed to the design of the research, to data analysis and to the writing of the manuscript. MR, YS and RK performed the modeling portion of the study. RK and MR wrote the modeling portion of the manuscript and contributed to the writing of the overall manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from the UTHSC College of Medicine iRISE Pilot Program to DHH, ACP and JWW and by support from the Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, University Tennessee Health Science Center to DHH. The contribution by RK has been supported in part by NSF CRCNS grant NSF-DMS-13-11165, and by DARPA MTO Superior Artificial Intelligence initiative. SSM was supported by the UTHSC Neuroscience Institute.

References

Arch, J. J., and Craske, M. G. (2006). Mechanisms of mindfulness: emotion regulation following a focused breathing induction. Behav. Res. Ther. 44, 1849–1858. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arieli, A., Sterkin, A., Grinvald, A., and Aertsen, A. (1996). Dynamics of ongoing activity: explanation of the large variability in evoked cortical responses. Science 273, 1868–1871. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5283.1868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arsenault, M., Ladouceur, A., Lehmann, A., Rainville, P., and Piché, M. (2013). Pain modulation induced by respiration: phase and frequency effects. Neuroscience 252, 501–511. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.07.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babajani-Feremi, A., Rezaie, R., Narayana, S., Choudhri, A. F., Fulton, S. P., Boop, F. A., et al. (2014). Variation in the topography of the speech production cortex verified by cortical stimulation and high gamma activity. Neuroreport 25, 1411–1417. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benson, H., Beary, J. F., and Carol, M. P. (1974). The relaxation response. Psychiatry 37, 37–46. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1974.11023785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boly, M., Balteau, E., Schnakers, C., Degueldre, C., Moonen, G., Luxen, A., et al. (2007). Baseline brain activity fluctuations predict somatosensory perception in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 12187–12192. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0611404104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, R. P., and Gerbarg, P. L. (2005). Sudarshan Kriya yogic breathing in the treatment of stress, anxiety and depression: part I-neurophysiologic model. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 11, 189–201. doi: 10.1089/acm.2005.11.189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, Y., Roy, S., Sachdev, R. N., and Heck, D. H. (2012). Dynamic correlation between whisking and breathing rhythms in mice. J. Neurosci. 32, 1653–1659. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4395-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cape, E. G., and Jones, B. E. (2000). Effects of glutamate agonist versus procaine microinjections into the basal forebrain cholinergic cell area upon gamma and theta EEG activity and sleep-wake state. Eur. J. Neurosci. 12, 2166–2184. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00099.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carstens, E., Leah, J., Lechner, J., and Zimmermann, M. (1990). Demonstration of extensive brainstem projections to medial and lateral thalamus and hypothalamus in the rat. Neuroscience 35, 609–626. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(90)90333-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chauvette, S., Seigneur, J., and Timofeev, I. (2012). Sleep oscillations in the thalamocortical system induce long-term neuronal plasticity. Neuron 75, 1105–1113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Z., Eldridge, F. L., and Wagner, P. G. (1992). Respiratory-associated thalamic activity is related to level of respiratory drive. Respir. Physiol. 90, 99–113. doi: 10.1016/0034-5687(92)90137-l

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crone, N. E., Hao, L., Hart, J. Jr., Boatman, D., Lesser, R. P., Irizarry, R., et al. (2001). Electrocorticographic gamma activity during word production in spoken and sign language. Neurology 57, 2045–2053. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.11.2045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deco, G., Jirsa, V. K., and McIntosh, A. R. (2011). Emerging concepts for the dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 43–56. doi: 10.1038/nrn2961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dillbeck, M. C., and Bronson, E. C. (1981). Short-term longitudinal effects of the transcendental meditation technique on EEG power and coherence. Int. J. Neurosci. 14, 147–151. doi: 10.3109/00207458108985827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebert, D., Hefter, H., Binkofski, F., and Freund, H. J. (2002). Coordination between breathing and mental grouping of pianistic finger movements. Percept. Mot. Skills 95, 339–353. doi: 10.2466/pms.95.6.339-353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eeckman, F. H., and Freeman, W. J. (1990). Correlations between unit firing and EEG in the rat olfactory system. Brain Res. 528, 238–244. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(90)91663-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, A. K., Fries, P., and Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 704–716. doi: 10.1038/35094565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flexman, J. E., Demaree, R. G., and Simpson, D. D. (1974). Respiratory phase and visual signal detection. Percept. Psychophys. 16, 337–339. doi: 10.3758/bf03203952

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fontanini, A., and Bower, J. M. (2005). Variable coupling between olfactory system activity and respiration in ketamine/xylazine anesthetized rats. J. Neurophysiol. 93, 3573–3581. doi: 10.1152/jn.01320.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fontanini, A., and Bower, J. M. (2006). Slow-waves in the olfactory system: an olfactory perspective on cortical rhythms. Trends Neurosci. 29, 429–437. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., and Raichle, M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 700–711. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, W. J. (2004). Origin, structure, and role of background EEG activity. Part 2. Analytic phase. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 2089–2107. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.02.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, W. J. (2015). Mechanism and significance of global coherence in scalp EEG. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 31, 199–205. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, W. J., Holmes, M. D., West, G. A., and Vanhatalo, S. (2006). Fine spatiotemporal structure of phase in human intracranial EEG. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 1228–1243. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, W. J., and Rogers, L. J. (2002). Fine temporal resolution of analytic phase reveals episodic synchronization by state transitions in gamma EEGs. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 937–945.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fries, P., Reynolds, J. H., Rorie, A. E., and Desimone, R. (2001). Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science 291, 1560–1563. doi: 10.1126/science.291.5508.1560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallego, J., Perruchet, P., and Camus, J. F. (1991). Assessing attentional control of breathing by reaction time. Psychophysiology 28, 217–224. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb00413.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallos, L. K., Makse, H. A., and Sigman, M. (2012). A small world of weak ties provides optimal global integration of self-similar modules in functional brain networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 109, 2825–2830. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1106612109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaylord, C., Orme-Johnson, D., and Travis, F. (1989). The effects of the transcendental mediation technique and progressive muscle relaxation on EEG coherence, stress reactivity and mental health in black adults. Int. J. Neurosci. 46, 77–86. doi: 10.3109/00207458908991618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgopoulos, A. P., Schwartz, A. B., and Kettner, R. E. (1986). Neuronal population coding of movement direction. Science 233, 1416–1419. doi: 10.1126/science.3749885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilad, Y., Przeworski, M., and Lancet, D. (2004). Loss of olfactory receptor genes coincides with the acquisition of full trichromatic vision in primates. PLoS Biol. 2:E5. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gould, I. C., Nobre, A. C., Wyart, V., and Rushworth, M. F. (2012). Effects of decision variables and intraparietal stimulation on sensorimotor oscillatory activity in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 32, 13805–13818. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2200-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossman, D., and Christensen, L. W. (2011). On Combat: The Psychology and Physiology of Deadly Conflict in War and Peace. Millstadt, IL: Human Factor Research Group, Inc.

Google Scholar

He, B. J. (2013). Spontaneous and task-evoked brain activity negatively interact. J. Neurosci. 33, 4672–4682. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2922-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Homma, I., and Masaoka, Y. (2008). Breathing rhythms and emotions. Exp. Physiol. 93, 1011–1021. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.2008.042424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Immanuel, S. A., Pamula, Y., Kohler, M., Martin, J., Kennedy, D., Saint, D. A., et al. (2014). Respiratory cycle-related electroencephalographic changes during sleep in healthy children and in children with sleep disordered breathing. Sleep 37, 1353–1361. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, J., Maldonado, P., and Grün, S. (2013). Cross-frequency interaction of the eye-movement related LFP signals in V1 of freely viewing monkeys. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:1. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, J., Roy, S., Liu, Y., Cao, Y., Fletcher, M., Boughter, J. D., et al. (2014). Whisker barrel cortex delta oscillations and gamma power in the awake mouse are linked to respiration. Nat. Commun. 5:3572. doi: 10.1038/ncomms4572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iwabe, T., Ozaki, I., and Hashizume, A. (2014). The respiratory cycle modulates brain potentials, sympathetic activity and subjective pain sensation induced by noxious stimulation. Neurosci. Res. 84, 47–59. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2014.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iwamoto, E., Taito, S., Kawae, T., Sekikawa, K., Takahashi, M., and Inamizu, T. (2010). The neural influence on the occurrence of locomotor-respiratory coordination. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 173, 23–28. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2010.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janson, S. R., Kozma, R., Ruszinkó, M., and Sokolov, Y. (2015). Activation process on a long-range percolation graph with power law long edge distribution. Part I: phase transition without inhibition. arxiv 1507.07997.

Google Scholar

Janson, S., Kozma, R., Ruszinkó, M., and Sokolov, Y. (2016). Bootstrap percolation on a random graph coupled with a lattice. arXiv 1507.07997v2.

Google Scholar

Jella, S. A., and Shannahoff-Khalsa, D. S. (1993). The effects of unilateral forced nostril breathing on cognitive performance. Int. J. Neurosci. 73, 61–68. doi: 10.3109/00207459308987211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kay, L. M. (2015). Olfactory system oscillations across phyla. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 31, 141–147. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kay, L. M., and Beshel, J. (2010). A beta oscillation network in the rat olfactory system during a 2-alternative choice odor discrimination task. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 829–839. doi: 10.1152/jn.00166.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kay, L. M., and Freeman, W. J. (1998). Bidirectional processing in the olfactory-limbic axis during olfactory behavior. Behav. Neurosci. 112, 541–553. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.112.3.541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kozma, R., and Freeman, W. J. (2001). “Analysis of visual theta rhythm-experimental and theoretical evidence of visual sniffing,” in Proceedings of the International Joint Conference on Neural Networks (Washington, DC), 1118–1121.

Google Scholar

Kozma, R., and Freeman, W. J. (2008). Intermittent spatio-temporal desynchronization and sequenced synchrony in ECoG signals. Chaos 18:037131. doi: 10.1063/1.2979694

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kozma, R., and Freeman, W. J. (2016). Cognitive Phase Transitions in the Cerebral Cortex-Enhancing the Neuron Doctrine by Modeling Neural Fields. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.

Google Scholar

Kozma, R., Heck, D. H., Liu, Y., McAfee, S., Rezaie, R., Babajani-Feremi, A., et al. (2015). Hilbert analysis of the relation between respiration and LFP/ECoG. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 479.02.

Kozma, R., and Puljic, M. (2015). Random graph theory and neuropercolation for modeling brain oscillations at criticality. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 31, 181–188. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krout, K. E., Belzer, R. E., and Loewy, A. D. (2002). Brainstem projections to midline and intralaminar thalamic nuclei of the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 448, 53–101. doi: 10.1002/cne.10236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krupnik, V., Nietzold, I., Bartsch, B., and Rassler, B. (2015). The effect of motor-respiratory coordination on the precision of tracking movements: influence of attention, task complexity and training. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 115, 2543–2556. doi: 10.1007/s00421-015-3250-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laufs, H., Krakow, K., Sterzer, P., Eger, E., Beyerle, A., Salek-Haddadi, A., et al. (2003). Electroencephalographic signatures of attentional and cognitive default modes in spontaneous brain activity fluctuations at rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 100, 11053–11058. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1831638100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, C., Rohrer, W. H., and Sparks, D. L. (1988). Population coding of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus. Nature 332, 357–360. doi: 10.1038/332357a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Li, S., and Laskin, J. J. (2006). Influences of ventilation on maximal isometric force of the finger flexors. Muscle Nerve 34, 651–655. doi: 10.1002/mus.20592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S., Park, W. H., and Borg, A. (2012). Phase-dependent respiratory-motor interactions in reaction time tasks during rhythmic voluntary breathing. Motor Control 16, 493–505. doi: 10.1123/mcj.16.4.493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S., and Rymer, W. Z. (2011). Voluntary breathing influences corticospinal excitability of nonrespiratory finger muscles. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 512–521. doi: 10.1152/jn.00946.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linkenkaer-Hansen, K., Nikulin, V. V., Palva, S., Ilmoniemi, R. J., and Palva, J. M. (2004). Prestimulus oscillations enhance psychophysical performance in humans. J. Neurosci. 24, 10186–10190. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2584-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linster, C., and Hasselmo, M. E. (2000). Neural activity in the horizontal limb of the diagonal band of broca can be modulated by electrical stimulation of the olfactory bulb and cortex in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 282, 157–160. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)00885-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liotti, M., Brannan, S., Egan, G., Shade, R., Madden, L., Abplanalp, B., et al. (2001). Brain responses associated with consciousness of breathlessness (air hunger). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 98, 2035–2040. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.4.2035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Y., McAfee, S., Rezaie, R., Babajani-Feremi, A., Kozma, R., Papanicolaou, A. C., et al. (2015). Respiratory modulation of brain activity. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 479.03.

Lorig, T. S., Schwartz, G. E., Herman, K. B., and Lane, R. D. (1988). Brain and odor: II. EEG activity during nose and mouth breathing. Psychobiology 16, 285–287.

Google Scholar

Macey, P. M., Woo, M. A., Macey, K. E., Keens, T. G., Saeed, M. M., Alger, J. R., et al. (2005). Hypoxia reveals posterior thalamic, cerebellar, midbrain, and limbic deficits in congenital central hypoventilation syndrome. J. Appl. Physiol. 98, 958–969. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00969.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manns, I. D., Alonso, A., and Jones, B. E. (2003). Rhythmically discharging basal forebrain units comprise cholinergic, GABAergic, and putative glutamatergic cells. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 1057–1066. doi: 10.1152/jn.00938.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, L., Helgadóttir, H., Mölle, M., and Born, J. (2006). Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature 444, 610–613. doi: 10.1038/nature05278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mason, P., Gao, K., and Genzen, J. R. (2007). Serotonergic raphe magnus cell discharge reflects ongoing autonomic and respiratory activities. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 1919–1927. doi: 10.1152/jn.00813.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maynard, E. M., Hatsopoulos, N. G., Ojakangas, C. L., Acuna, B. D., Sanes, J. N., Normann, R. A., et al. (1999). Neuronal interactions improve cortical population coding of movement direction. J. Neurosci. 19, 8083–8093.

PubMed Abstract

McAfee, S., Liu, Y., and Heck, D. H. (2016). Respiration modulates neuronal activity in mouse visual cortex. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 331.13.

Moore, J. D., Deschênes, M., Furuta, T., Huber, D., Smear, M. C., Demers, M., et al. (2013). Hierarchy of orofacial rhythms revealed through whisking and breathing. Nature 497, 205–210. doi: 10.1038/nature12076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nácher, V., Ledberg, A., Deco, G., and Romo, R. (2013). Coherent delta-band oscillations between cortical areas correlate with decision making. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 110, 15085–15090. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1314681110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nguyen Chi, V., Müller, C., Wolfenstetter, T., Yanovsky, Y., Draguhn, A., Tort, A. B., et al. (2016). Hippocampal respiration-driven rhythm distinct from theta oscillations in awake mice. J. Neurosci. 36, 162–177. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2848-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palva, J. M., Zhigalov, A., Hirvonen, J., Korhonen, O., Linkenkaer-Hansen, K., and Palva, S. (2013). Neuronal long-range temporal correlations and avalanche dynamics are correlated with behavioral scaling laws. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 110, 3585–3590. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216855110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poulet, J. F., and Petersen, C. C. (2008). Internal brain state regulates membrane potential synchrony in barrel cortex of behaving mice. Nature 454, 881–885. doi: 10.1038/nature07150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puljic, M., and Kozma, R. (2008). Narrow-band oscillations in probabilistic cellular automata. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlin. Soft Matter Phys. 78:026214. doi: 10.1103/physreve.78.026214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rassler, B. (2000). Mutual nervous influences between breathing and precision finger movements. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 81, 479–485. doi: 10.1007/s004210050071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rassler, B., Ebert, D., Waurick, S., and Junghans, R. (1996). Coordination between breathing and finger tracking in man. J. Mot. Behav. 28, 48–56. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1996.9941732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rassler, B., and Raabe, J. (2003). Co-ordination of breathing with rhythmic head and eye movements and with passive turnings of the body. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 90, 125–130. doi: 10.1007/s00421-003-0876-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reijneveld, J. C., Ponten, S. C., Berendse, H. W., and Stam, C. J. (2007). The application of graph theoretical analysis to complex networks in the brain. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2317–2331. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rittweger, J., and Pöpel, A. (1998). Respiratory-like periodicities in slow eye movements during sleep onset. Clin. Physiol. 18, 471–478. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2281.1998.00128.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, T. B., Macrini, T. E., and Luo, Z. X. (2011). Fossil evidence on origin of the mammalian brain. Science 332, 955–957. doi: 10.1126/science.1203117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sadaghiani, S., Hesselmann, G., and Kleinschmidt, A. (2009). Distributed and antagonistic contributions of ongoing activity fluctuations to auditory stimulus detection. J. Neurosci. 29, 13410–13417. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2592-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schölvinck, M. L., Saleem, A. B., Benucci, A., Harris, K. D., and Carandini, M. (2015). Cortical state determines global variability and correlations in visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 35, 170–178. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4994-13.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Servít, Z., Kristof, M., and Kolínová, M. (1977). Activation of epileptic electrographic phenomena in the human EEG by nasal air flow. Physiol. Bohemoslov. 26, 499–506.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Shadlen, M. N., and Newsome, W. T. (1994). Noise, neural codes and cortical organization. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 4, 569–579. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(94)90059-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheth, B. R., Sandkühler, S., and Bhattacharya, J. (2009). Posterior Beta and anterior gamma oscillations predict cognitive insight. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1269–1279. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, M., Engel, A. K., and Donner, T. H. (2011). Cortical network dynamics of perceptual decision-making in the human brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:21. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stancák, A. Jr. and Kuna, M. (1994). EEG changes during forced alternate nostril breathing. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 18, 75–79. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(84)90017-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tallon-Baudry, C. (2003). Oscillatory synchrony and human visual cognition. J. Physiol. Paris 97, 355–363. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2003.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tallon-Baudry, C. (2004). Attention and awareness in synchrony. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 523–525. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, A. Y., Chen, Y., Scholl, B., Seidemann, E., and Priebe, N. J. (2014). Sensory stimulation shifts visual cortex from synchronous to asynchronous states. Nature 509, 226–229. doi: 10.1038/nature13159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tort, A. B., Komorowski, R. W., Manns, J. R., Kopell, N. J., and Eichenbaum, H. (2009). Theta-gamma coupling increases during the learning of item-context associations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 20942–20947. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911331106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Towle, V. L., Yoon, H. A., Castelle, M., Edgar, J. C., Biassou, N. M., Frim, D. M., et al. (2008). ECoG gamma activity during a language task: differentiating expressive and receptive speech areas. Brain 131, 2013–2027. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turova, T. S., and Villa, A. E. P. (2007). On a phase diagram for random neural networks with embedded spike timing dependent plasticity. BioSystems. 89, 280–286. doi: 10.1016/j.biosystems.2006.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uchida, N., Poo, C., and Haddad, R. (2014). Coding and transformations in the olfactory system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 37, 363–385. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-013941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Vugt, M. K., Simen, P., Nystrom, L. E., Holmes, P., and Cohen, J. D. (2012). EEG oscillations reveal neural correlates of evidence accumulation. Front. Neurosci. 6:106. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vinnik, E., Itskov, P. M., and Balaban, E. (2012). β- and γ-band EEG power predicts illusory auditory continuity perception. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 2717–2724. doi: 10.1152/jn.00196.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wyart, V., de Gardelle, V., Scholl, J., and Summerfield, C. (2012). Rhythmic fluctuations in evidence accumulation during decision making in the human brain. Neuron 76, 847–858. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yanovsky, Y., Ciatipis, M., Draguhn, A., Tort, A. B. L., and Brankačk, J. (2014). Slow oscillations in the mouse hippocampus entrained by nasal respiration. J. Neurosci. 34, 5949–5964. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5287-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zautra, A. J., Fasman, R., Davis, M. C., and Craig, A. D. (2010). The effects of slow breathing on affective responses to pain stimuli: an experimental study. Pain 149, 12–18. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mind-body, cortical oscillations, respiration, embodied cognition, phase transitions, phase amplitude coupling, proprioception, graph theory

Citation: Heck DH, McAfee SS, Liu Y, Babajani-Feremi A, Rezaie R, Freeman WJ, Wheless JW, Papanicolaou AC, Ruszinkó M, Sokolov Y and Kozma R (2017) Breathing as a Fundamental Rhythm of Brain Function. Front. Neural Circuits 10:115. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2016.00115

Received: 19 September 2016; Accepted: 26 December 2016;
Published: 12 January 2017.

Edited by:

Alexey Semyanov, University of Nizhny Novgorod, Russia

Reviewed by:

Beate Rassler, Leipzig University, Germany
Leslie M. Kay, University of Chicago, USA

Copyright © 2017 Heck, McAfee, Liu, Babajani-Feremi, Rezaie, Freeman, Wheless, Papanicolaou, Ruszinkó, Sokolov and Kozma. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Detlef H. Heck, dheck@uthsc.edu

Download