Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Neurol., 27 May 2013
Sec. Epilepsy

Spike Dynamic and Epigenetic Malfunctions in Epilepsy: A Tale of Two Codes

  • 1Center for Brain and Cognition, University of California San Diego, San Diego, CA, USA
  • 2Medimark Corporation, Del Mar, CA, USA

Introduction

The cellular processes that underlie the genesis and spread of epileptic seizures are currently poorly understood. This paper explores two hypotheses that seek to fill in some of these gaps. The first hypothesis relates to the role of the claustrum in the genesis of both complex partial and grand mal seizures in light of new and unique clinical data, and in modulating and integrating synchronized gamma oscillations in corticoclaustral circuits. The second hypothesis explores the possible role of epigenetic transcription factors, microRNAs, and exosomes in the kindling phenomenon. The conclusion is that the brain may possess two coding systems – one based on synchronized spikes for its software, the other on epigenetic processes that may modulate its dynamic hardware.

Code 1: Synchronized Oscillations and Spike Timing

In a recent review, Zhang et al. (2011) state, “One of the most terrifying aspects of epilepsy is the sudden and apparently unpredictable transition of the brain into the pathological state of an epileptic seizure. The pathophysiology of the transition to seizure still remains mysterious.” Current ideas about the genesis of complex partial seizures are based on spreading cascades of interacting hypersynchronous oscillations between different functional networks. These are expressed in four recent reviews of this topic (Cavanna et al., 2011; Bertram, 2012; Blumfeld, 2012; Kramer and Cash, 2012). Particular attention is paid to the disruption of the network involving the thalamus and the frontoparietal association cortices that affects the level of awareness. Chipaux et al. (2013) have demonstrated with experiments in humans and rats that the disruption of conscious perception during an absence seizure is not due to an obliteration of information transfer in the thalamocortical system. The authors state, “Our findings thus dispute the widely accepted assumption that synchronized oscillations in thalamocortical loops disrupt conscious perception by filtering-out external sensory inputs and/or disallowing their allocation to the appropriate cortical assemblies.” Rather, they suggest that the loss of consciousness during absence seizures may be caused by a disruption of the normal information processing in large-scale brain networks. Stefan and Lopes da Silva (2013) argue that the concept of “local” and “generalized” epilepsies is now outmoded. They add that there is now compelling evidence, even in the case of Ideopathic Generalized Seizures and Childhood Absence Seizures, that seizures start in a well-defined brain area and spread at great speed to connected brain areas recruiting specific neuronal networks into typical oscillatory behavior. These accounts, however, consider only corticocortical and corticothalamic interactions; in none of these review papers is the claustrum even mentioned. Recent work, however, may throw some light on this mystery.

Koubeissi et al. (2012) have demonstrated that electrical stimulation of the claustrum, in a conscious patient with intractable complex partial epilepsy undergoing a preoperative investigation, produces a clinical state of immobility and complete lack of interaction with the environment, but retainment of some automatic movements, quite similar to cases of partial complex seizures. No such result was obtained when the nearby insula or white matter surrounding the claustrum was stimulated. Moreover, during the procedure, the EEG showed no evidence of ongoing seizure activity. This suggests that the stimulus had not simply triggered a seizure, and that the claustrum may somehow be involved in epileptogenesis. We therefore investigated how a recent hypothesis (Smythies et al., 2012), which suggests a specific functional role for the claustrum, might explain these observations.

This hypothesis states that the claustrum is involved in the integration of synchronized oscillations in the cerebral cortex. The mechanism suggested is as follows. Activation of two or more areas of the cortex will set up local areas of synchronized oscillations in those areas. Layer VI cortical neurons form feedback loops with selected claustral neurons according to the rule discovered by Pearson et al. (1982). This states that, if two cortical neurons (A and B) are paired by corticocortical connections, then the claustral neurons (C and D) that each cortical neuron projects (A–C and B–D) will themselves be anatomically connected. If other cortical neurons are not connected in this manner, than neither will the claustral neurons that they project to. In turn, the activated cortical neurons A and B will activate the interconnected claustral neurons C and D, which will then receive the synchronized oscillations. As claustral neurons are densely packed against one another, the interaction between C and D may be stronger than the interaction between A and B. In this way, the claustrum could amplify the synchronization. In an ever-changing environment, different corticoclaustral circuits would be activated sequentially, leading to competing intraclaustral oscillating systems. These would complete for dominance, with the winner activating the executive projection from the claustrum to the motor cortex, resulting in the appropriate behavior.

To adapt this hypothesis as a contribution to the explanation of epileptogenesis, we can develop the following scenario. Consider the situation where a local area of cortex develops, for any one of a number of reasons (Schevon et al., 2007), an epileptogenic focus. It is the tendency of such a focus to spread to local cortex where areas of local hypersynchrony will develop (Margineanu, 2010), further spreading to contralateral cortex via the corpus callosum to form a mirror focus, as well as to the claustrum. As a seizure develops, each of these foci will develop increasing excessive neuronal activity. In view of the compact nature of the claustrum, the effect of these excessive spikes may be magnified here also. At first, with only a few ictal pulses arriving, the claustrum might be able to maintain its normal function of integrating the synchronization of its inflow. However, as the ictal impulses steadily increase in number (Cymerbilt-Sabba and Schiller, 2012), a critical point would be reached in which the claustrum would no longer be able to maintain its normal function (because it was blocked by what is, in essence, excessive noise in the system), and a complex partial seizure would develop. This would be very similar to the effect produced by Koubeissi et al. (2012) using an electric current to disrupt claustral function. In this syndrome, other parts of the brain, which maintain such functions as standing posture and automatic movement, are still operative. However, should the ictal process continue, another threshold could be reached, at which point the seizure activity within the claustrum would trigger the activation of all claustral efferent neurons that project to nearly the entire cortex. This would send a massive number of spikes to the cortex, adding to the slower intercortical spread of ictal activity, thereby resulting in a grand mal seizure.

The similarity with “aclaustral rats,” following the intraclaustral injection of a specific toxin as described by Remedios (2012), is remarkable yet subtly different. The aclaustral rats could only make a few tentative, feeble, and abortive efforts to enter a wing of the eight-arm radial maze. However, in the study by Koubeissi et al. (2012), electrical stimulation seems to produce a more profound effect than chemical ablation. The patient was totally immobile and unresponsive. Our hypothesis (Smythies et al., 2012) might explain this in the following way. In the rats studied by Remedios (2012), their corticocortical pathway remains intact and thus could provide weak synchronization. However, 50 Hz electrical stimulation of the claustrum would disrupt not only intraclaustral synchronization, but might also traverse the claustrocortical projection and impact corticocortical synchronization, temporarily rendering the patient non-interactive with a profound lack of awareness – in essence, with “nothing.”

Thus, we suggest that a complex partial seizure results from a “blocked” claustrum, while a grand mal seizure is the result of a maximally activated claustrum.

Code 2: Epigenetics

The claustrum is also notable on account of its susceptibility to kindling (Sheerin et al., 2004). Kindling is a phenomenon in which repeated short periods of electrical stimulation of a part of the brain results in a permanent lowering of its seizure threshold. The nature of the permanent, and therefore presumably structural, neuronal changes that bring about kindling are at present unknown. However, there could be such alterations at a number of different levels.

1. There could be changes in such processes as the local molecular mechanisms that produce action potentials at the synapse, exert control of ion channels and uptake mechanisms, and alter of receptor processing by the endocytotic mechanisms.

2. Structural changes in neurons can also be brought about by epigenetic mechanisms (Smythies and Edelstein, 2013).

There is now considerable data to show that plastic changes in the brain brought about by learning involve new protein synthesis, which is guided by epigenetic processes including DNA methylation and demethylation, as well as histone methylation and acetylation. Furthermore, a number of microRNAs can modulate the function and structure of neurons (Impey et al., 2009; Barmack et al., 2010; Cohen et al., 2011; Hsu et al., 2012; Saba et al., 2012). MicroRNAs bind to specific mRNAs and so control new protein synthesis at the mRNA level. Lastly, it has been shown that neuronal synaptic activity induces changes in all these epigenetic activities. This is relevant to the fact that kindling is induced by electrical stimulation. For example, Cohen et al. (2011) have identified a developmentally- and activity-regulated microRNA (miR-485) that controls dendritic spine number and synapse formation in an activity-dependent homeostatic manner. Transcription of the microRNA miP335 is promoted by naturally evoked synaptic activity at the climbing fiber-Purkinje cell synapse in the mouse cerebellar flocculus (Barmack et al., 2010). Impey et al. (2009) report that neuronal activity regulates spine formation, in part, by increasing miR132 transcription. MicroR-181a activity in primary neurons, induced by dopamine signaling, is a negative post-transcriptional regulator of GluA2 expression (Saba et al., 2012). Thus, it is possible that excess activity in kindled neurons could activate microRNAs by epigenetic mechanisms. This might induce the permanent effects reported.

In most instances these effects have been investigated in the same neuron that produced the epigenetic factors, or in neurons to which these agents were applied from an external source. However, there is developing evidence that transcription factors, mRNAs, and microRNAs can be transmitted from one neuron to another across the synaptic cleft. These are carried by exosomes. Exosomes are small lipoprotein vesicles, derived from multivesicular bodies and the cellular endosome system, that cross the external plasma membrane to enter the perisynaptic space (Fauré et al., 2006; Smalheiser, 2007; Lachenal et al., 2011; Chivet et al., 2012). Here, they gain entry to the postsynaptic neuron, most likely via astrocytes. The endosome membrane fuses with the external membrane of the postsynaptic cell, thus releasing its payload into the postsynaptic neuron. The payloads carried by these exosomes include epigenetic signaling molecules, such as signaling proteins and various types of RNA including mRNAs and microRNAs (Smalheiser, 2007; Lee et al., 2012; O’Loughlin et al., 2012; Tetta et al., 2012; Vlassov et al., 2012). From the host cytoplasm a system of carrier molecules could deliver the cargo widespread throughout the postsynaptic neuron. This new information has profound consequences for neuroscience. In this way the presynaptic neuron can modulate the functional structure of the postsynaptic neuron. It has been suggested that this system in the brain could be involved in information processing (Smalheiser, 2007; Chivet et al., 2012) and to form an epigenetic code (Smythies and Edelstein, 2013).

Thus, the brain may contain not one but two signaling systems. The first is the familiar one carried by the complex and exact timing of axon spikes and local potentials. This contributes to the computations the brain carries out and may be viewed as “software.” This may be involved in complex partial seizures and grand mal. The second signaling system may be an epigenetic code expressed by the precise quantities of signaling proteins, mRNAs and microRNAs carried along the axon of the presynaptic neuron, and transmitted across the synapse to modulate the structure and function of the postsynaptic neuron. This process structures the actual computational “hardware” in the neuron so as to enable it to optimally process the particular software it receives. In this way, the different modalities of cerebral cortex may have different computational hardware modulated by Code 2. This process could be involved in the genesis of kindling via the pathologically increased activation of neurons, resulting in the delivery of excessive amounts of epigenetic factors. This over-activity would translate into structural changes in the recipient neurons that would contribute to the neurological basis of the kindling effect.

The brain may be a computer, but it appears to be a most remarkable one in that it can alter its own hardware to fit changes in the software at hand.

References

Barmack, N. H., Qian, Z., and Yakahnitsa, V. (2010). Climbing fibers induce microRNA transcription in cerebellar Purkinje cells. Neuroscience 17, 655–665.

CrossRef Full Text

Bertram, E. H. (2012). Neuronal circuits in epilepsy: do they matter? Exp. Neurol. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.expneurol.2012.01.028.

Blumfeld, H. (2012). Impaired consciousness in epilepsy. Lancet Neurol. 11, 814–826. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70188-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cavanna, A. E., Rickards, H., and Ali, F. (2011). What makes a simple partial seizure complex? Epilepsy Behav. 22, 651–658. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.10.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chipaux, M., Vercueil, L., Kaminska, A., Mahon, S., and Charpier, S. (2013). Persistence of cortical sensory processing during absence seizures in human and an animal model: evidence from EEG and intracellular recordings. PLoS ONE 8:e58180. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058180

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chivet, M., Hemming, F., Pernet-Gallay, K., Fraboulet, S., and Sadoul, R. (2012). Emerging role of neuronal exosomes in the central nervous system. Front. Physiol. 3:145. doi: 3389/fphys.2012.00145

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, J. E., Lee, P. R., Chen, S., and Fields, R. D. (2011). MicroRNA regulation of homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 11650–11655. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1017576108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cymerbilt-Sabba, A., and Schiller, Y. (2012). Development of hypersynchrony in the cortical network during chemoconvulsant-induced epileptic seizures in vivo. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 1718–1730. doi: 10.1152/jn.00327.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fauré, J., Lachenal, G., Court, M., Hirrlinger, J., Chatellard-Causse, C., Blot, B., et al. (2006). Exosomes are released by cultured cortical neurons. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 31, 642–648. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2005.12.003

CrossRef Full Text

Hsu, R., Scofield, C. M., de al Cruz, C. G., Jones-Davis, D. M., Blelloch, R., and Ullian, E. M. (2012). Loss of microRNAs in pyramidal neurons leads to specific changes in inhibitory synaptic transmission in the prefrontal cortex. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 50, 283–292. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2012.06.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Impey, S., Davarra, M., Lesiak, A., Fortin, D., Ando, H., Varlamova, O., et al. (2009). An activity-induced microRNA controls dendritic spine formation by reg An activity-induced microRNA controls dendritic spine formation by regulating Rac1-PAK signaling. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 43, 146–156.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Koubeissi, M. Z., Beltagy, A., Picard, F., and Edrees, M. (2012). “Alteration of consciousness due to electrical stimulation of the claustrum in the human brain,” in American Epilepsy Society Conference, San Diego. [Abstract 2.079].

Kramer, M. A., and Cash, S. S. (2012). Epilepsy as a disorder of cortical network organization. Neuroscientist 18, 360–372. doi: 10.1177/1073858411422754

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lachenal, G., Pernet-Gallay, G., Chivet, M., Hemming, F. J., Belly, A., Bodon, G., et al. (2011). Release of exosomes from differentiated neurons and its regulation by synaptic glutamatergic activity. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 46, 409–418. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2010.11.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lee, Y., Andaloussi, S., and Wood, M. J. (2012). Exosomes and microvesicles: extracellular vesicles for genetic information transfer and gene therapy. Hum. Mol. Genet. 21, R125–R134. doi: 10.1093/hmg/dds317

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Margineanu, D. G. (2010). Epileptic hypersynchrony revisited. Neuroreport 27, 963–967.

O’Loughlin, A. J., Woffindale, C. A., and Wood, M. J. (2012). Exosomes and the emerging field of exosome-based gene therapy. Curr. Gene Ther. 12, 262–274. doi:10.2174/156652312802083594

CrossRef Full Text

Pearson, R. C., Brodal, P., Gatter, K. C., and Powell, T. P. (1982). The organization of the connections between the cortex and the claustrum in the monkey. Brain Res. 234, 435–441. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90883-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Remedios, R. (2012). “The claustrum and the orchestra of cognitive control,” in Francis Crick Memorial Conference, Cambridge.

Saba, P. H., Störchel, A., Aksoy-Aksel, A., Kepura, F., Lippi, G., Plant, T. D., et al. (2012). Dopamine-regulated microRNA MiR-181a controls GluA2 surface expression in hippocampal neurons. Mol. Cell. Biol. 32, 619–632. doi: 10.1128/MCB.05896-11

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schevon, C. A., Cappell, J., Emerson, R., Isler, J., Grieve, P., Goodman, R., et al. (2007). Cortical abnormalities in epilepsy revealed by local EEG synchrony. Neuroimage 35, 140–148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sheerin, A. H., Nylen, K., Zhang, X., Saucier, D. M., and Corcoran, M. E. (2004). Further evidence for a role of the anterior claustrum in epileptogenesis. Neuroscience 125, 57–62.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smalheiser, N. R. (2007). Exosomal transfer of proteins and RNAs at synapses in the nervous system. Biol. Direct 2, 35. doi: 10.1186/1745-6150-2-35

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smythies, J., and Edelstein, L. (2013). Transsynaptic modality codes in the brain: possible involvement of synchronized spike timing, microRNAs, exosomes and epigenetic processes. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:126. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00126

CrossRef Full Text

Smythies, J., Edelstein, L., and Ramachandran, V. (2012). The functional neuroanatomy of the claustrum. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:53. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00053

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stefan, H., and Lopes da Silva, F. H. (2013). Epileptic neuronal networks: methods of identification and clinical relevance. Front. Neurol. 4:8. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2013.00008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tetta, C., Ghigo, E., Silengo, L., Deregibus, M. C., and Camussi, G. (2012). Extracellular vesicles as an emerging mechanism of cell-to-cell communication. Endocrine doi: 10.1007/s12020-012-9839-0 [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vlassov, A. V., Magdaleno, S., Setterquist, R., and Conrad, R. (2012). Exosomes: current knowledge of their composition, biological functions, and diagnostic and therapeutic potentials. Biochim. Biophys. Acta 1820, 940–948. doi: 10.1016/j.bbagen.2012.03.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhang, Z. J., Valiente, T. A., and Carlen, P. L. (2011). Transition to seizure: from “macro”- to “micro”-mysteries. Epilepsy Res. 97, 290–299. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2011.09.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Smythies J and Edelstein L (2013) Spike dynamic and epigenetic malfunctions in epilepsy: a tale of two codes. Front. Neurol. 4:63. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2013.00063

Received: 27 March 2013; Accepted: 13 May 2013;
Published online: 27 May 2013.

Edited by:

Ashok K. Shetty, Texas A&M Health Science Center, USA

Reviewed by:

Ashok K. Shetty, Texas A&M Health Science Center, USA
Marino M. Bianchin, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil
Dave Clarke, Dell Children’s Medical Center of Central Texas, USA

Copyright: © 2013 Smythies and Edelstein. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: jsmythies@ucsd.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.