Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 04 November 2015
Sec. Neurotrauma

Altered Amygdala Connectivity in Individuals with Chronic Traumatic Brain Injury and Comorbid Depressive Symptoms

  • 1Center for BrainHealth®, School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas, Dallas, TX, USA
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, TX, USA

Depression is one of the most common psychiatric conditions in individuals with chronic traumatic brain injury (TBI). Though depression has detrimental effects in TBI and network dysfunction is a “hallmark” of TBI and depression, there have not been any prior investigations of connectivity-based neuroimaging biomarkers for comorbid depression in TBI. We utilized resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging to identify altered amygdala connectivity in individuals with chronic TBI (8 years post-injury on average) exhibiting comorbid depressive symptoms (N = 31), relative to chronic TBI individuals having minimal depressive symptoms (N = 23). Connectivity analysis of these participant sub-groups revealed that the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group showed relative increases in amygdala connectivity primarily in the regions that are part of the salience, somatomotor, dorsal attention, and visual networks (pvoxel < 0.01, pcluster < 0.025). Relative increases in amygdala connectivity in the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group were also observed within areas of the limbic–cortical mood-regulating circuit (the left dorsomedial and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortices and thalamus) and the brainstem. Further analysis revealed that spatially dissociable patterns of correlation between amygdala connectivity and symptom severity according to subtypes (Cognitive and Affective) of depressive symptoms (pvoxel < 0.01, pcluster < 0.025). Taken together, these results suggest that amygdala connectivity may be a potentially effective neuroimaging biomarker for comorbid depressive symptoms in chronic TBI.

Introduction

Depression is one of the most common psychiatric conditions among individuals with traumatic brain injury (TBI) (15). For example, a recent study with large sample size (N = 559) showed that 53.1% met criteria for major depressive disorder during the first year following the occurrence of a TBI (6). The comorbidity of depression in TBI is associated with poorer cognitive function (79), reduced health-related quality of life (6, 10, 11), greater functional disability (3, 12), increased suicide attempts (13), greater sexual dysfunction (14), less social and recreational activity (15), and poorer recovery (16). Given the significant effects of comorbid depression on individuals with TBI, it is important to better understand the underlying neural mechanisms of depression in the context of TBI.

Structural and functional connectivity utilizing advanced neuroimaging techniques, such as diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), revealed that network dysfunction is a hallmark of TBI, particularly mild TBI [see Sharp et al. (17) for a review]. For example, resting-state functional connectivity MRI has been used to identify alterations in the default mode network (DMN) (18, 19), executive network (19), inter-hemispheric connectivity (20), thalamic connectivity (21), small-worldness (22, 23), and modular organization (24) of individuals with TBI. The primary injury mechanism responsible for such network dysfunction following TBI is diffuse axonal injury (DAI) (25). Since DAI occurs at multiple white matter pathways connecting distributed regions across the brain, the effects of DAI are complex. Furthermore, DAI may interact with pathology associated with complications arising late in life after TBI such as Alzheimer’s disease or chronic traumatic encephalopathy (17). Thus, connectivity-based assessments of individuals with TBI may continue to provide valuable information for us to better understand the complex nature of clinical outcomes following TBI.

Depression is currently viewed as a system-level disorder that affects (1) integrated pathways linking limbic, cortical, and subcortical brain regions (26) and (2) neurotransmitter activity related to these regions (27). Since depression is fundamentally a mood disorder, investigating neural processes occurring over extended periods of time (minutes or hours) rather than brief periods of time (such as individual trials lasting only seconds in a task-based fMRI session) may be more relevant to our understanding of depression (28). Thus, the research community has increasingly relied upon resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) to investigate neural function in individuals diagnosed with depression [see Dutta et al. (29), Hamilton et al. (28), Northoff et al. (30), and Wang et al. (31) for a review]. For example, previous resting-state fMRI studies of depression have revealed increased DMN connectivity in the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex and thalamus (32), reduced frontoparietal control network (FPCN) (33) connectivity (34), reduced salience network (SN) (35) connectivity involving the anterior insula (36), and elevated connectivity of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC) to the DMN, cognitive control, and affective networks (ANs) (37) in individuals with depression. These reductions and increases in connectivity related to depression provide a complex picture at the present time. The directionality of altered connectivity may depend upon a variety of factors, including brain regions involved, age, and comorbid conditions.

Given the central role of the amygdala in both bottom-up and top-down emotional processes (38), the amygdala and its connecting regions have been widely studied in the depression literature (3948). From a network perspective, the amygdala has connections with the cortical–striatal–pallidal–thalamic circuit, which is often considered to be the core neural system in mood disorders (49). More specifically, the amygdala connects (1) the medial prefrontal cortex, a part of the DMN (50), and (2) anterior insula and hypothalamus, both of which are parts of the SN (35). Since alterations in the DMN and SN of individuals TBI have been previously reported (18, 19, 51), it is important to investigate whether amygdala connectivity can be a potential neuroimaging biomarker for comorbid depression among individuals at the chronic phase of a TBI.

Though two separate lines of research in (1) TBI without depression and (2) depression without TBI have both demonstrated marked network dysfunction of those individuals, there have been no functional or structural connectivity studies in individuals with both TBI and depression. As such, most of the studies of neuroimaging biomarkers for comorbid depression among individuals with TBI have been limited to regional assessments of brain structure and function, including regional gray matter volume (8, 5255), white mater integrity (5658), presence of microbleeds (31), and regional brain activity (40, 41, 5961). Thus, connectivity-based studies in TBI with comorbid depressive symptoms can contribute to this body of literature and are well suited to capturing the larger-scale network interactions associated with depressive symptoms in this population.

Depression is a psychological construct comprised of several factors (62). Previous investigations of the neural correlates of depression severity in TBI (41, 52, 54, 56) did not specifically characterize the neural correlates in accordance with subtypes of depressive symptoms [see Strain et al. (58) for an exception]. Given the heterogeneity of both depressive symptoms and TBI, several studies attempted to identify the underlying factor structure of depressive symptoms among TBI individuals (6365). Thus, identifying neural correlates of these underlying factors will be useful for assessment, diagnosis, and characterization of often heterogeneous TBI population (66).

Here, we utilized resting-state fMRI to identify altered amygdala connectivity within individuals with chronic TBI and comorbid depressive symptoms. Based on previous findings reporting abnormal amygdala connectivity in depression and aberrant connectivity in TBI, we hypothesized that the amygdala connectivity of TBI individuals with comorbid depressive symptoms would be altered, relative to TBI individuals exhibiting minimal depressive symptoms. We also predicted that amygdala connectivity would be characterized by abnormal spatial patterns associated with different subtypes of depressive symptoms according to the Buckley categories of items from the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), which classifies depression into separable constructs representing cognitive, somatic, and emotional symptoms (62, 67).

Materials and Methods

Participants

The data used for this analysis are part of an ongoing study (68). We analyzed 54 chronic TBI individuals who ranged from lower moderate disability to lower good recovery [age 20–60; >6 months post-injury; 5–7 on the extended Glasgow outcome scale (GOS-E)] (69), who completed MRI scans and whose MRI scans passed quality assurance (QA) procedures described below. We recruited these participants from the Dallas–Ft. Worth community and screened by a phone interview before inclusion in the study. The primary causes of TBIs are blasts, blunt force trauma, falls, athletic impacts, vehicle accidents, or combinations thereof. Note that, at several years post-injury time, it was not feasible to obtain all participants’ clinical information on initial injury characteristics such as Glasgow coma scale (GCS) (70) or the duration of loss of consciousness (LOC) from the inpatient, acute-care facilities where they were hospitalized multiple years ago (see the limitation section regarding limited clinical information on initial injury). Therefore, injury severity and the duration of LOC at the time of injury were estimated utilizing the Ohio State University TBI identification (OSU TBI-ID) method (71). The OSU TBI-ID method is a structured interview developed to incorporate recommendations from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) for the detection of TBI history. The OSU TBI-ID first elicits recall of all possible head or neck injuries receiving medical attention, or that should have received medical attention. The elicitation method subsequently focuses on any injuries involving a blow to the head or neck, fall, blast exposure, or vehicle accident that can cause an injury to the brain. For these injuries, the occurrence and nature of altered consciousness and treatment received are probed. Using the OSU TBI-ID, various pieces of information on injury history are available, including injury severity, the number of injuries, worst injury, age at injury, and time since the most recent injury. Bogner and Corrigan (72) and Corrigan and Bogner (71) showed that the OSU TBI-ID method had good inter-rater reliability and test–retest reliability. Previous studies (71, 72) demonstrated the validity of summary indices driven from the OSU TBI-ID to predict TBI-related cognitive and behavioral deficits. Note that the OSU TBI-ID method estimates initial injury severity based on the duration of LOC and the CDC guidelines for the conceptual definition and identification of TBI (73, 74). Specifically, the TBI participants whose duration of LOC <30 min, <24 h, or >24 h were considered to be probable mild, probable moderate, or probable severe TBI, respectively. The participants included both civilians and veterans (see Table 1 for demographics). No participants had a history of any significant, clinically diagnosed neurological or psychiatric comorbidities. Participants also had no history of depressive symptoms prior to their TBI or TBIs. We also confirmed that all participants’ brains were free of visible focal lesions, contusions, mass shifting, or extreme cortical thinning on structural MRI scans. This confirmation should rule out potential effects of such macro structural injuries on fMRI preprocessing steps, including registration and subsequent functional connectivity analyses. All participants provided written informed consent. This study was conducted in compliance with the declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Texas at Dallas and University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics.

Assessment of Depressive Symptoms

Depressive symptom severity was quantified using the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) (75). In the development of the BDI test, Beck et al. (75) showed that the BDI-II had excellent internal consistency for the 500 psychiatric outpatients (α = 0.92) and the 120 college students (α = 0.93) and a robust 1-week test–retest correlation (r = 0.93). They also reported highly convergent and discriminant validity of the BDI-II with respect to clinically rated depression and anxiety such as the Revised Hamilton Psychiatric Rating Scale for Depression (76) and the Revised Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (77). According to suggested total BDI score guidelines for the diagnosis of major depression (75), we subdivided the TBI participants into two groups: a TBI with minimal depressive symptom group (N = 23; 0–13) and a TBI with mild to severe depressive symptom group (N = 31; 14–63). Due to a previous report detailing altered amygdala connectivity in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (78, 79), we also measured PTSD symptom severity of the TBI participants enrolled in this study using the PTSD Check List Stressor-specific (PCL-S) (80) for the forth edition of the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV).

Neuropsychological Assessments

We administered neuropsychological tests on the participants to characterize the TBI sub-groups in a variety of domains. These tests include similarities, matrix reasoning, and full scale intelligent quotient-2 (FSIQ-2) from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) for estimated current IQ (81), FSIQ from the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) for estimated premorbid IQ (82), digit span forward and backward from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition (WAIS-III) for working memory (83), color-word, verbal fluency, card sorting, trail making from the Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System (D–KEFS) for inhibitory control, switching, verbal fluency, processing speed and problem solving (84), immediate recall and delayed recall from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Fourth Edition (WMS-IV) for memory and recall (85), verbal problem solving assessment (S. B. Chapman, unpublished data), and visual selective learning task adapted from Hanten et al. (86). We also assessed satisfaction with life scale (87) for the participants to measure global cognitive judgments of their life satisfaction.

MRI Data Acquisition

The participants underwent MRI scanning on a Philips Achieva 3 T scanner (Philips Medical Systems, Netherlands) at the Advanced Imaging Research Center at the University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center. In each imaging session, one or two 416-s runs of rsfMRI were acquired using a standard 32-channel head coil with T2*-weighted image sequence (repetition time (TR)/echo time (TE) = 2000/30 ms; flip angle (FA) = 80°; field of view (FOV) = 22.0 cm × 22.0 cm; matrix = 64 × 64; 37 slices, 4.0 mm thick). During rsfMRI acquisition, the participants were asked to remain still with their eyes closed. For rsfMRI alignment, we obtained one high-resolution T1-weighted image of the whole brain (TR/TE = 8.2/3.8 ms; FA = 12°; FOV = 25.6 cm × 25.6 cm; matrix = 256 × 256; 160 slices, 1.0 mm thick) for each participant using the same head coil.

MRI Preprocessing

Resting-state fMRI data were preprocessed with standard methods using a modified version of a shell script generated by http://afni_proc.py 1 from AFNI (88). Each subject’s whole-brain structural images were first skull-stripped and registered (affine transform with 12 parameters) to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space (89). For each rsfMRI run, the initial four time points were discarded to allow T1 magnetization saturation. Standard preprocessing methods were then applied, including despiking, slice timing correction, motion correction, coregistration to the structural images in the MNI space using a single affine transform with spatial resampling (4 mm isotropic), normalization to whole-brain mode of 1000, band-pass filtering (0.009 < f < 0.08 Hz), and linear regression. At the motion correction stage, the six rigid body motion profiles were obtained for the linear regression. In the linear regression, the rsfMRI time series were third order detrended, and several sources of signal fluctuation unlikely to be of neuronal origin were regressed out as nuisance variables: (1) six parameters for the rigid body head motion acquired from the motion correction (90), (2) the signal averaged over the lateral ventricles (91), (3) the signal averaged over a region centered in the deep cerebral white matter (91), and (4) the first temporal derivatives of aforementioned parameters. Note that we did not apply global signal regression in this procedure since global signal regression could arguably generate difficulties in interpreting group comparisons (9296). After the linear regression, motion “scrubbing” (97) was performed with a framewise displacement (FD) of 0.5 mm and a standardized DVARS2 of 1.8 to prevent potential motion artifacts (9799). A standardized DVARS of 1.8 corresponds to the median plus 1.5 times interquartile range of the standardized DVARS data across all frames and runs. The remaining rsfMRI signals were spatially blurred with 6-mm full-width-at-half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. For participants on whom two runs of rsfMRI scans were acquired, the two preprocessed rsfMRI runs were temporally concatenated. To account for the differences in total number of frames (subsequently different degrees of freedom for correlation coefficients) after motion scrubbing across rsfMRI scans, all remaining frames were trimmed to the minimum length (121 frames; 242 s) across all rsfMRI scans as suggested in Power et al. (100).

Quality Assurance

We visually inspected all structural MRI scans to ensure that subjects had no significant brain atrophy. In rsfMRI preprocessing, the quality of the preprocessed data was visually inspected at each step. After motion “scrubbing,” we confirmed that the total time duration of remaining frames after the “scrubbing” exceeded 4 min, the minimum length required to reliably estimate functional connectivity (101).

Whole-Brain Seed-Based Connectivity Analysis

For each participant, amygdala connectivity was identified by seeding the center of mass (L: −24, −1, −16; R: 26, 1, −18) of the amygdala labels in the automated anatomical label (AAL) atlas (102) with 5-mm radius spheres, respectively. Pearson correlation maps were then Fisher’s Z-transformed to ensure the normality of correlations, followed by conversions to z-scores (i.e., normal distributions with zero mean and unit variance). To identify patterns of group differences in amygdala connectivity, we performed the general linear model (GLM) analysis at each of the voxels with regressors of group memberships, age, PCL-S scores, age by PCL-S score interactions, and FD after censoring and trimming. Specifically, connectivity strength for the participants at each of the voxels was modeled as follows:

y=β1+β2ID+β3a+β4ICp+β5IDp  +β6ICap+β7IDap+β8f+e

where vectors a, p, and f are age, PCL-S scores, and FD after censoring and trimming for the participants, IC and ID are group indicator vectors for the controls and depressive symptoms group, and e is a normally distributed vector with zero mean, respectively. The age values were included in this model because of reports of significant age effects on amygdala connectivity in healthy individuals (103), and the PCL-S scores were included to account for potential effects of PTSD on amygdala connectivity (78, 79). FD after censoring and trimming was included to account for potential effects of trends in higher FD in TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group. To construct covariate regressors, the age and FD values were centered at the global mean over the two groups after confirming no statistically significant group differences whereas the PCL-S scores were within-group centered to interpret the between-group differences in amygdala connectivity at their respective average PCL-S scores. The reason for applying within-group centering to the PCL-S scores was due to reported comorbidities between depression and PTSD in TBI (104) and statistically significant group differences in the PCL-S scores for our groups (see Table 1 and limitations relevant to comorbidity between depression and PTSD in TBI). Statistically significant group differences in amygdala connectivity at the whole-brain level were identified at pvoxel < 0.01, correcting for multiple comparisons across voxels by cluster size using AFNI’s AlphaSim and across the number of amygdala seeds by additional Bonferroni correction at pcluster < 0.05/2 (20 voxels; 1,280 mm3).

Conjunction Analysis

To identify regions showing consistent group differences in both left and right amygdala connectivity, we performed a conjunction analysis. This was performed in accordance with the conjunction inference procedure described by Nichols et al. (105). We took minimum statistics over the group comparison maps for left and right amygdala connectivity at each of the voxels then thresholded the minimum statistic map over the whole brain at pvoxel < 0.01 and pcluster < 0.025. We identified peak foci of the conjunction map as follows. First, we generated local peaks within the given cluster with >8 mm apart. Next, we selected a local peak closest to the center of mass for the cluster as a focus of the cluster. To elucidate spatial patterns of altered amygdala connectivity at the large-scale network level, we also overlaid the conjunction map onto network-based parcelation maps of the cerebral cortex and cerebellum (106, 107).

Correlation Analysis

We performed a correlation analysis to identify spatial patterns of amygdala connectivity associated with sub-factors of depressive symptoms within the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group. We first obtained participants’ BDI-II sub-factor scores using the Buckley three-factor model (67). The Buckley three-factor model decomposes the total BDI-II scores into cognitive, affective, and somatic symptoms (see Table 2). The cognitive factor includes items regarding sadness, pessimism, past failure, guilty feelings, punishment feelings, self-dislike, self-criticalness, suicidal ideation, and worthlessness. The affective factor includes items probing loss of pleasure, crying, loss of interest, and indecisiveness. The somatic factor includes the agitation, loss of energy, sleep disturbance, irritability, appetite disturbance, concentration difficulty, fatigue, and loss of sexual interest test items. The Buckley factor model was originally proposed for treatment-seeking substance abuser (67). The Buckley factor model has been reported to provide a better characterization of depressive symptom severity of psychiatric patients over alternative models (108) and served as the best model for veterans with polytrauma (63). Regions with statistically significant correlation between amygdala connectivity and each of the Buckley BDI-II sub-scores were then identified at pvoxel < 0.01, correcting for multiple comparisons by cluster size using AFNI’s AlphaSim and the number of amygdala seeds (Bonferroni) at pcluster < 0.05/2 (20 voxels; 1,280 mm3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Buckley BDI-II factor structure.

Data-Driven Connectivity Analysis Over 268 Putative Functional Nodes

To identify (1) if the amygdala is the most important driver of observed group differences in connectivity strength and the patterns of correlation with the Buckley BDI factor scores or, alternatively, (2) if these effects can be better represented via other components of the distributed network, we assessed group differences in connectivity strength and correlation coefficients over 268 putative functional nodes. As in Cao et al. (109), the 268 nodes were obtained by combining 264 nodes reported by Power et al. (110), the hippocampi (L: −30, −13, −12; R: 30, −4, −12) from Bilek et al. (111) and the amygdalae. For the defined regions, we constructed a Fisher’s Z-transformed connectivity matrix followed by a Z-score (zero mean and unit variance) conversion. For group comparisons, we performed the GLM analysis at each pair of the normalized (Z-scored) connectivity matrix elements. Subsequently, we obtained average group differences in connectivity strength for a node (seed) by taking average absolute values of Z-statistics for connections between the given node and the other 267 nodes. For correlations between connectivity and the Buckley BDI factor scores, we obtained Fisher’s Z-transformed and normalized (Z-scored) correlation coefficients for each pair of nodes. Subsequently, we calculated average correlations between BDI factors scores and connectivity strength with a node by taking average absolutes values of normalized correlation coefficients for connections between the given node and the other 267 nodes.

Statistical Analyses

All statistical analyses were carried out in MATLAB R2013a. First, we performed the Shapiro–Wilk test at α = 0.05 to assess the normality of age, years of education, post-injury time, PCL-S total scores, BDI-II total scores, Buckley BDI factor scores, percentage of motion-censored volumes, and average FD after motion censoring and trimming within each of the groups. Post-injury time and percentage of motion-censored volumes did not pass Shapiro–Wilk normality test. Thus, the Wilcoxon rank-sum test was used to compare these measures between the groups. Two sample t-tests were used to compare age, years of education, PCL-S total scores, BDI-II total, and Buckley BDI-II factor scores between the groups. The Fisher’s exact test was used to compare the gender distributions and proportion of civilians and veterans between the groups. The likelihood ratio chi-square test was used to compare the distribution of primary cause of injury between the groups. We performed linear regression analysis on neuropsychological measures with age and education covariates.

In these statistical tests, 95% confidence intervals were also obtained as follows: mean of group differences for the t-test, median of group differences for the Wilcoxon rank-sum test, odds ratio for the Fisher’s exact test, and group contrasts for the regression analysis. Effect sizes were also obtained for each type of test: Hedge’s g for the t-test, W for the first group for the Wilcoxon rank-sum test, odds ratio for the Fisher’s exact test, Cramer’s V for the chi-square test, and ηp2 for the linear regression analysis. To identify if group differences in amygdala connectivity in the GLM analysis have sufficient power, we obtained observed power at each of the voxels and thresholded at 0.8.

Control Analyses

Somatomotor Connectivity

To ensure data quality of resting-state functional connectivity of the TBI sub-groups, we obtained the somatomotor connectivity by seeding at somatomotor cortices (L: −41, −18, 59; R: 46, −19, 54) with a 5-mm radius sphere. The seed locations were obtained after converting Talairach coordinates reported by Fox et al. (112) to MNI coordinates using “tal2icbm” program3 (113).

Group Comparisons with Healthy Individuals

To confirm that the TBI participants had residual TBI-related deficits in neuropsychological behavior, we compared neuropsychological assessment results of each of the TBI sub-groups with those of 17 healthy individuals whose MRI scans were acquired from the same MRI scanner and same imaging parameters as those of the TBI sub-groups (see Table S1 in Supplementary Material for demographics of the healthy individuals). In addition, we obtained connectivity in the three groups by seeding the posterior cingulate cortex [PCC; L: −7, −55, 27; R: 8, −48, 31; Power et al. (110)] and anterior prefrontal cortex [aPFC; L: −36, 57, 9; R: 34, 52, 10; Vincent et al. (33)] with a 5-mm radius sphere, respectively, which are parts of the DMN (50) and FPCN (33). We expected that DMN and FPCN would be likely to show alterations in the TBI sub-groups relative to the healthy group (18, 19, 114). Furthermore, to observe amygdala connectivity of the TBI sub-groups in the context of healthy individuals, we performed the GLM analysis with the three groups without PCL-S score covariates. Note that, in these group comparisons of the neuropsychological test performance and connectivity measures, we excluded older TBI participants to match their age with the healthy individuals at α = 0.05 as there were statistically significant group differences in age with the full TBI samples at α = 0.05 (see Table S1 in Supplementary Material for the sample sizes of the age-matched TBI sub-groups). We further confirmed that there were no statistically significant effects of age on any of the neuropsychological assessment scores or any of the connectivity measures for the age-matched TBI sub-groups and the healthy group even though there were still trends that the age-matched TBI sub-groups were older than the healthy group (see Table S1 in Supplementary Material).

Assessment of Amygdala Connectivity in Civilians Versus Veterans Within the TBI-Plus-Depressive Symptoms Group

To identify if mixed veterans and civilians within the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group systematically affected the correlation analysis results, we further subdivided the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group into civilians and veterans, then we compared their respective amygdala connectivity at peak locations within the regions showing statistically significant correlations between amygdala connectivity and the BDI sub-scores.

Assessment of the Effects of Comorbid PTSD Symptom Severity on Amygdala Connectivity

To assess the effects of comorbid PTSD symptom severity on group comparison results for amygdala connectivity in the GLM analysis, we also obtained color maps for these covariates at pvoxel < 0.01 and pcluster < 0.025.

Assessment of the Effects of Estimated Injury Severity on Depressive Symptom Severity and Amygdala Connectivity

To identify if there were systematic effects of estimated injury severity on our findings, we assessed the BDI-II total scores and amygdala connectivity of the TBI sub-groups according to estimated injury severity, and we repeated group analyses of amygdala connectivity with probable mild TBI participants only (N = 21 for the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group and N = 17 for the TBI-only group). Furthermore, we performed group analyses of amygdala connectivity by excluding probable mild TBI participants while matching the sample sizes for each of the newly formed TBI sub-groups with those of the sub-groups of participants with mild TBI only. The goal of this analysis was to test whether the probable moderate and severe participants contributed more to the amygdala connectivity differences than the probable mild TBI participants. To achieve this goal, we utilized a resampling method, conceptually similar to the resampling procedure described in Han and Talavage (115). Specifically, we resampled the TBI sub-groups by excluding 10 (out of 21) and 6 (out of 17) probable mild TBI participants from the original TBI-plus-depressive symptoms and TBI-only groups, respectively, in a pseudo-random fashion. Then, we performed group analyses of amygdala connectivity in the resampled TBI sub-groups. We repeated this resampling procedure 5,000 times. Subsequently, we defined the magnitude of overall group differences in amygdala connectivity by taking average absolute values of Z-statistics for the group comparison test over the whole brain. Finally, to determine if the magnitude of overall group differences in amygdala connectivity of the probable mild TBI participants only groups was “significantly” different than those of the resampled TBI sub-group (i.e., higher proportions of probable moderate and severe TBI participants than the original TBI groups), we assessed whether the magnitude of overall group differences in amygdala connectivity of the probable mild TBI sub-groups fell outside the intervals of 2.5–97.5th percentile (similar to 95% confidence intervals for Z-statistics) for group differences, obtained from 5,000 resampled groups.

Visualization

The thresholded volumetric statistical results for group differences in amygdala connectivity and correlation analysis were surface-projected onto the cortical surface of the population-averaged landmark- and surface-based (PALS-B12) atlas (116) using a multi-fiducial mapping that avoids the biases of choosing a cortical surface from a single-individual as an atlas target, implemented in Caret Software (117).

Results

Demographics

The TBI participants were in the long-term chronic phase of TBI (approximately 8 years post-injury time on average). There were no statistically significant group differences in age, education, gender, proportion of civilians and veterans, post-injury time, distributions of estimated injury severity or primary injury types, percentage of motion-censored volumes, or gender (Table 1). The TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group had statistically significant higher scores on the PCL-S and Buckley BDI factors over the TBI-only group. The finding of higher PTSD symptom severity in the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group is consistent with findings reported by Hibbard et al. (4) and Levin et al. (104). We also observed a trend in which the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group had higher FD after censoring and trimming than the TBI-only group. Thus, FD after censoring and trimming was included as a covariate in all subsequent analyses.

Neuropsychological Measures

There were no statistically significant group differences in estimated premorbid IQ, but group differences in current IQ showed marginal statistical significance (Table 3). The TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group showed poorer performance in immediate and delayed recall, consistent with previously reported on memory deficits among depressive TBI individuals (9, 60). Group analyses of neuropsychological measures also revealed group differences in category fluency scores of the verbal fluency test. Relatively poor performance in these neuropsychological measures of the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group indicates that there were adverse effects of depressive symptoms likely influencing cognitive function among individuals with chronic TBI. We also observed relatively lower satisfaction with life scale in the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group, suggesting the presence of adverse effects of depressive symptoms on global cognitive judgments of life satisfaction among individuals with chronic TBI.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Neuropsychological assessment results.

Group Differences in Amygdala Connectivity

Group analysis revealed enhanced bilateral amygdala connectivity for the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group relative to the TBI-only group across multiple regions except left amygdala connectivity with the left superior parietal lobule (SPL), right insula and right thalamus and right amygdala connectivity with the right thalamus at pvoxel < 0.01 and pcluster < 0.025 (Figure 1). Spatial patterns of relatively enhanced amygdala connectivity of the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group were fairly consistent over both amygdala connectivities though right amygdala connectivity showed stronger increases in the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group over the TBI-only group. Conjunction analysis results (Figure 2) highlighted brain regions with consistently increased bilateral amygdala connectivity of the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group over the TBI-only group. The regions showing increased connectivity for both left and right amygdala include the bilateral posterior midcingulate cortex, marginal sulcus, paracentral sulcus, precentral gyrus; the left SPL; the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), central sulcus, postcentral gyrus, and insula. Local peak coordinates of these regions and their Z-scores were listed in Table 4. Group comparison maps, including a network-based parcelation (Figure 3), revealed that alterations in amygdala connectivity of the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group primarily occurred in the areas originally reported by Biswal et al. (118) and later labeled the somatomotor network (SMN) (119), the dorsal attention network (DAN) (112, 120), SN (35), and visual network (VN) (121). Relative increases in amygdala connectivity of the TBI group with depressive symptoms also occurred within areas of the limbic–cortical mood-regulating circuit (45), including the left DMPFC and right DLPFC and right thalamus (Figures 13).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Group comparison maps of amygdala connectivity.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Conjunction map of left and right amygdala connectivity (A) and foci of local peaks (B).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Regions showing statistically significant group differences in both left and right amygdala connectivity (TBI-plus-depressive symptoms > TBI-only; pvoxel < 0.01; pcluster < 0.025).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Group comparison maps overlaid on network-based parcelation (106, 107). (A) Left amygdala connectivity, (B) right amygdala connectivity, (C) network-based parcellation. Note that we renamed the ventral attention network (VAN) in Buckner et al. (106) and Yeo et al. (107) as the salience network (SN) since (1) the VAN in Buckner et al. (106) and Yeo et al. (107) is an aggregate of multiple networks, including the SN and (2) most of the corresponding regions in the conjunction map fell onto the SN (35).

Association of Amygdala Connectivity with the Buckley BDI Factors of the TBI-Plus-Depressive Symptoms Group

Voxel-wise correlation analysis of amygdala connectivity with each of the Buckley BDI factors (i.e., cognitive, affective, and somatic) exhibited dissociable spatial patterns over the whole brain within the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group (Figure 4). Overall, only cognitive and affective factors were associated with amygdala connectivity among the TBI individuals with depressive symptoms (pvoxel < 0.01; pcluster < 0.025), and such statistically significant correlations occurred in the regions that are part of the DMN, DAN, SN, SMN, FPCN, and VN. Specifically, the cognitive factor was negatively correlated with right amygdala connectivity in the bilateral aPFC and anterior medial prefrontal cortices (amPFC); left superior central sulcus. The affective factor was negatively correlated with left amygdala connectivity in the bilateral lingual gyri, subcentral cortices, superior temporal cortices, middle temporal complexes, marginal sulci and dorsal anterior cingulate cortices; left insula and SPL; right precentral sulcus and DLPFC. Negative association between the affective factor and left amygdala connectivity was also occurred in the cerebellar lobule VI Vermis and bilateral cerebellar lobules VI (hemisphere). Scatter plots for BDI factors versus amygdala connectivity controlled for age, PCL-S, and FD at selected nine local peak coordinates in Table 5 confirmed that statistically significant correlations did not erroneously occur by outliers (Figure 5).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Correlation maps of amygdala connectivity and the Buckley BDI factors within the TBI group with depressive symptoms.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Selected regions from the maps for correlations between amygdala connectivity and the Buckley BDI factors (pvoxel < 0.01; pcluster < 0.025).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Scatter plots for correlation between amygdala connectivity and the Buckley BDI factors within the TBI group with depressive symptoms at each of the selected nine local peaks in Figure 4. (A–E) The Affective factor versus left amygdala connectivity in the cerebellar lobule VI vermis (A), left insula (B), right subcentral gyrus (C), right precentral sulcus (D), and left superior parietal lobule (E), respectively. (F–I) The Cognitive factor versus right amygdala connectivity in the right anterior prefrontal cortex (F), left anterior medial prefrontal cortex (G), left superior central sulcus (H), and left anterior prefrontal cortex (I), respectively.

Connectivity Over 268 Putative Functional Nodes

Bar graphs for average group differences in connectivity and correlations between connectivity and BDI sub-factors over the rest of the putative functional nodes were obtained by seeding each of the 268 putative functional nodes. This analysis revealed that the amygdala seed connectivity showed pronounced average group differences relative to other seeds (Figure 6). However, the right amygdala seed was not an important driver of correlations with the BDI cognitive factor and correlations with the BDI affective factor can be better represented via other seeds (e.g., the left precuneus).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Bar graphs for average group differences (A) and average correlation between connectivity strength and the Buckley BDI sub-scores (B-D) over 268 putative functional nodes. Only top 20 nodes for each of the measures were shown. See Cao et al. (109) for abbreviations for node names.

Power Analysis Results

The power analyses demonstrated that our findings have sufficiently reached both statistical significance and statistical power (Figures 7 and 8). The effect size maps for group differences in amygdala connectivity (Figure 7) revealed that the patterns of voxels whose group differences in amygdala connectivity accounted for more than 10% of total variance were similar to those of statistical significance at pvoxel < 0.01 and pcluster < 0.025 (Figure 1). Again, the patterns of observed power values at >0.8 for group comparisons of left and right amygdala connectivity (Figure 8) were similar to those of statistical significance at pvoxel < 0.01 and pcluster < 0.025 (Figure 1).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Effect size maps for group differences in amygdala connectivity.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Observed power for group differences in amygdala connectivity.

Control Analysis Results

Somatomotor Connectivity

Somatomotor connectivity maps for the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms and TBI-only groups (Figure S1 in Supplementary Material) showed that both groups had strong somatomotor connectivity in the bilateral somatomotor cortices and supplementary motor cortex, which was similar to somatomotor connectivity of healthy individuals (118, 119).

Group Comparisons with Healthy Individuals

Relative to the healthy individuals, the age-matched TBI individuals had higher depressive symptom severity (Table S1 in Supplementary Material), lower satisfaction with life scale scores and poorer performance on multiple neuropsychological assessments (Table S2 in Supplementary Material). Group comparisons of PCC and aPFC connectivity measures with those of healthy individuals revealed that both the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms and TBI-only groups showed relative reductions in PCC and aPFC connectivity (Figure S2 in Supplementary Material). There were no statistically significant group differences in PCC or aPFC connectivity between the TBI sub-groups at pvoxel < 0.01 and pcluster < 0.025. Group comparisons of amygdala connectivity in the age-matched TBI sub-groups with the healthy group demonstrated that both groups showed relative reductions in amygdala connectivity with more pronounced reductions in the TBI-only group (Figure S3 in Supplementary Material). Note that, in these group comparisons with the healthy group, there were no statistically significant effects of age on any of the assessed neuropsychological test scores at p < 0.05 or any of the obtained connectivity measures at pvoxel < 0.01 and pcluster < 0.025.

Amygdala Connectivity in Civilians Versus Veterans Within the TBI-Plus-Depressive Symptoms Group

Civilian versus veteran group comparisons of amygdala connectivity were adjusted to control for age, PCL-S, and FD (Figure S4 in Supplementary Material) and did not show statistically significant differences between civilians and veterans within the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group at p < 0.05 in each of the selected nine regions in Figure 4.

Effects of Comorbid PTSD Symptom Severity on Amygdala Connectivity

Statistically significant (pvoxel < 0.01 and pcluster < 0.025) effects of PTSD-related covariates occurred by the PCL-S scores and age by PCL-S interaction in the TBI-plus-depressive symptom group for right amygdala connectivity (Figure S5 in Supplementary Material). However, the spatial extent of these PTSD-related effects was small compared to the group differences in amygdala connectivity demonstrated in Figures 13.

Effects of Estimated Injury Severity on Depressive Symptom Severity and Amygdala Connectivity

There were no systematic effects of estimated injury severity on the TBI participants’ BDI-II total scores and amygdala connectivity at selected four regions from Table 4 (Figure S6 in Supplementary Material). Group comparison maps for amygdala connectivity of the TBI participants with probable mild TBI only were essentially similar to results for the full TBI participants at pvoxel < 0.01 and pcluster < 0.025 (Figure S7A in Supplementary Material). Again, group comparison maps for amygdala connectivity of the resampled TBI sub-groups, whose overall group differences magnitude in amygdala connectivity corresponded to the median value among those of the entire resampled pool, essentially replicated the patterns of amygdala connectivity differences for the full TBI participants at pvoxel < 0.01 and pcluster < 0.025 (Figure S7B in Supplementary Material). The magnitude of overall group differences in amygdala connectivity of the probable mild TBI sub-groups did not fall outside the 2.5–97.5th percentile intervals of group differences obtained from 5,000 resampled groups, suggesting that there were no “significant” effects of estimated injury severity on the amount of overall group differences in amygdala connectivity.

Discussion

In summary, we confirmed that depressive symptoms altered amygdala connectivity in the individuals with TBI over multiple brain regions and networks. Relative increases in amygdala connectivity within the TBI group with depressive symptoms primarily occurred in the regions that are part of the SN, SMN, DAN, and VN. Alterations in amygdala connectivity also occurred within the areas of the limbic–cortical mood-regulating circuit (45) that includes the DMPFC, DLPFC, and thalamus. Amygdala connectivity also revealed spatially dissociable patterns of correlation with symptom severity according to the Buckley subtypes (Cognitive and Affective) of depressive symptoms (67), further indicating the utility of network-based rsfMRI as a sensitive tool for assessing the relationships between brain networks and depressive symptoms in TBI individuals.

Advantages of Large-Scale Network Approaches to Comorbid Depressive Symptoms in TBI

This is the first study that identified a connectivity-based biomarker for depressive symptoms among TBI individuals. We characterized the spatial patterns of altered amygdala connectivity among TBI individuals with depressive symptoms in the context of large-scale networks such as the DAN, DMN, SN, SMN, and VN. Large-scale network approaches to TBI studies have been increasingly favored because one of the primary injury mechanisms for TBI is DAI and DAI disrupts structural and functional connectivity [see Sharp et al. (17) for a review]. Recently, large-scale network approaches have also been appearing with greater frequency in the depression literature (28, 29, 31, 34, 37, 122, 123). The utility of large-scale network approaches for evaluating depression is promising because depression is well known for its heterogeneity of symptoms, systems, regions, and biochemical influences (30). Prior attempts to identify localized brain regions responsible for depressive symptoms have met with limited success. Based on previous demonstrations of the utility of applying large-scale network approaches in TBI and depression studies, our findings demonstrated potential advantages of large-scale network approaches to the evaluation of comorbid depressive symptoms among TBI individuals. As demonstrated in Figure 3, the patterns of widely spread alterations in amygdala connectivity of the TBI individuals with comorbid depressive symptoms were more precisely characterized when we compared the group comparison maps (Figures 1 and 2) with functional connectivity-based parcelation maps of the healthy brain (106, 107). Thus, we suggest that taking a large-scale network perspective, as opposed to region-by-region assessments, can augment the ability to understand altered patterns of connectivity in future studies of both civilians and veterans with TBI and comorbid depression. Furthermore, utilizing graph theory to assess the brain network topology of these TBI individuals with depressive symptoms may also be of great interest and may further enhance our understanding of network alterations among these participants, since our findings suggest that depressive symptoms may disrupt information processing between the amygdala and other brain networks.

Findings in Relation to Previous Studies

Patterns of Altered Amygdala Connectivity

The amygdala is densely interconnected with the rest of the brain (49). Previous resting-state functional connectivity in healthy individuals has demonstrated rich connections from several brain areas with the amygdala (124). Specifically, the amygdala is functionally connected with the cingulate gyrus, precuneus, superior, middle and inferior frontal gyri, superior, middle and inferior temporal gyri, precentral gyrus, superior and inferior parietal lobules, angular gyrus, lateral occipital lobe, lingual gyrus, fusiform gyrus, insula, hippocampus, caudate, thalamus, brain stem, pons, and cerebellum during the rest-state in healthy individuals. Amygdala connectivity in depressed individuals is altered in a variety of brain regions (29, 31). Thus, it was not surprising that alterations in the amygdala connectivity among TBI individuals with depressive symptoms occurred over multiple brain regions affiliated with the SN, DAN, SMN, and VN as well as the limbic–cortical mood-regulating network. It is, however, a novel finding and the specific areas involved were not previously identified in this population.

Alterations with the Salience Network. The SN (35), comprising the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, anterior insula, temporal pole, presupplementary motor area, amygdala, putaman, periaqueductal gray, substantia nigra, ventral tegmental area, dorsomedial thalamus, and hypothalamus, is involved in filtering information to support behavior choice. In the depression literature, the SN has also been described as the AN because of the large overlapping regions between these networks. The AN consists of the anterior cingulate cortex, amygdala, hypothalamus, entorhinal cortex, nucleus accumbens, and other limbic structures, and the AN involved in emotion regulation and processing emotional stimuli. (125). An extensive literature describes alterations in AN activity in depression [see Johansen-Berg et al. (126) and Price and Drevets (127) for a review], and some previous studies have directly examined the AN in depression at rest (37, 128). Among the altered regions in the SN, the insula and the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex are of particular interest. Task activation studies demonstrated that the insula is associated with emotional responses to interoceptive sensory stimuli (129) and the dorsal anterior cingulate is involved in cognition, emotion regulation, and attention (130).

Alterations with the Dorsal Attention Network. The DAN is comprised of the superior parietal lobule, the middle temporal complex, and the frontal eye field (120). The DAN is involved in top-down control of attention in that it is associated with the control of spatial attention through selecting sensory stimuli according to internal goals or expectations and applying these toward making appropriate motor responses (131). No prior studies had directly investigated the DAN in depression at rest. However, previous studies in healthy individuals reported increased connectivity within the DAN and between the DAN and VN when the participants engaged in the reappraisal task relative to maintaining emotional responses, suggesting that the DAN may be critical in volitional emotion regulation (132). Furthermore, recent functional neuroimaging research revealed that enhanced sensory responses to emotional stimuli can gain prioritized access to awareness after competing for attentional resources (133). In this “emotional attention” process, the amygdala plays a crucial role by providing both direct and indirect top-down signals on sensory pathways (133, 134). Taken together, the altered amygdala connectivity with the DAN suggests that the interaction of emotion and attention was altered among the TBI individuals with depressive symptoms (134).

Alterations with the Somatomotor Network. Altered amygdala connectivity also occurred in the primary motor cortex, primary somatosensory cortex, and supplementary motor area (SMA) within the SMN in Yeo et al. (107). Though there are no prior reports of depression studies that investigated the SMN connectivity with the amygdala, a relevant animal study demonstrated that direct electrical stimulation of the amygdala can interrupt ongoing motor behaviors (135). In humans, the amygdala is functionally connected with the SMN (124, 136). Utilizing an emotional version of the stop-signal fMRI task, Sagaspe et al. (137) revealed that the amygdala involves motor inhibition by emotional signals through interaction with the SMA. Thus, altered amygdala connectivity with the SMN among TBI individuals with depressive symptoms may be associated with disrupted motor inhibition in response to emotional stimuli. Future task connectivity studies are required to support this possibility. Among the altered regions in the SMN, the SMA is of particular interest. The SMA was strongly altered along the midline in our study (Figures 13). Within the complex network perspective, the SMA together with the PCC serve as functional core hubs that balance segregation and integration of local brain systems to maintain the small-world architecture (136). In this vein, it would be interesting to assess if altered amygdala connectivity with the SMA leads to imbalance between segregation and integration associated with among TBI individuals with depressive symptoms in future studies.

Alterations with the Visual Network. Within the VN, the parieto-occipital sulcus and lingual gyrus were altered (Figures 13). The amygdala is extensively connected with VN both structurally (138) and functionally (124). Previous depression studies also demonstrated abnormal connectivity in brain regions within the VN (46, 122, 139, 140). Based on an extensive supporting literature, Pessoa and Adolphs (141) proposed that affective visual signals flow not only through fast and automatic pathway from the visual cortex to the amygdala, but also through slow, diverse pathways from the pulvinar to the amygdala and back to the pulvinar via distributed cortical regions. Furthermore, previous connectivity studies in depression that reported abnormalities in the visual cortex have also revealed abnormalities in both the amygdala and other brain regions (46, 122, 140). Thus, altered amygdala connectivity within the VN in concert with other resting-state networks suggests that depressive symptoms in TBI may disrupt a modulatory role of the amygdala in evaluating affective visual stimuli such as salience, significance, ambiguity, and unpredictability (142145) via a wide array of networks. Future studies utilizing task-state functional connectivity and behavioral assessment will be required to confirm this hypothesis.

Alterations with the Other Networks. Altered amygdala connectivity also occurred in the regions within the DMN (the left DMPFC), the FPCN (the right DLPFC), and the thalamus (Figures 1, 3, 78). The DMPFC, which is structurally connected with the amygdala (49), may mediate the alterations of amygdala connectivity with the DMN and FPCN in our study since Sheline et al. (37) demonstrated that connectivity increases in the DMN, FPCN, and AN overlap within the DMPFC. In the broader context of the depression literature, the amygdala, DLPFC, and thalamus serve as parts of the limbic–cortical mood-regulating network where the coordinated interactions within the limbic–cortical network are critical to the integration of mood regulation and mood-related motor, cognitive and somatic behaviors (45).

Alterations with the Cerebellum. Depressive symptoms in TBI also altered amygdala connectivity with the cerebellum (Figures 13). Traditionally, the cerebellum was thought to involve in motor coordination. However, converging, recent evidence has also suggested that the cerebellum may also be important in both emotion and cognition (146, 147). Furthermore, recent resting-state functional connectivity evaluated in healthy individuals has demonstrated interactions of the cerebellum with entire resting-state networks within the cerebral cortex (106). Indeed, previous resting-state fMRI studies in depression have revealed abnormalities in the cerebellum (46, 139, 140, 148152). Alterations in amygdala connectivity with the cerebellum among individuals with depression also have been reported (46, 140). A novel finding on altered amygdala connectivity with the cerebellum in our study is that cerebellar regions that showed altered amygdala connectivity fell onto the DAN, SMN, and SN which showed alterations in the cerebral cortex. This consistency between the cerebellum and cerebral cortex in altered networks with amygdala connectivity in TBI with depressive symptoms reinforces the idea that depressive symptoms in TBI disrupt coordinating roles of the amygdala with multiple resting-state networks over the whole brain.

Elevated Amygdala Connectivity in TBI Individuals with Depressive Symptoms. Relative increases in amygdala connectivity among TBI individuals with depressive symptoms indicate that depressive symptoms in TBI may increase neural resource recruitment relevant to information processing between the amygdala and other brain regions in emotion responses. Future task-state functional connectivity and metabolism studies may confirm this hypothesis. Increases in amygdala connectivity in TBI with depressive symptoms appear to be inconsistent with findings from previous resting-state amygdala connectivity studies in major depressive disorder (40, 43, 46, 48, 122). Such discrepancies might be attributed to potential effects of interactions between other pathology (TBI in this study) and depressive symptoms on resting-state functional connectivity. For example, depressed individuals with Parkinson’s disease showed increased amygdala connectivity over non-depressed individuals with Parkinson’s disease whereas depressed individuals with Parkinson’s disease showed both increased and decreased amygdala connectivity over healthy individuals (153). Furthermore, Maller and colleagues (54, 56) demonstrated that cortical volumes and DTI measures were different among individuals with TBI-depression, TBI-no-depression, and no-TBI-depression, suggesting the effects of interactions between depression and TBI on the brain.

Correlations of Amygdala Connectivity with Subtypes of Depressive Symptoms

Correlation analysis of amygdala connectivity and the Buckley BDI-II factors exhibited spatial patterns of amygdala connectivity specific to cognitive and affective factors of depressive symptoms (Figures 4 and 5). Our correlation analysis results support potential utility of amygdala connectivity for TBI individuals with depressive symptoms in a clinical standpoint. Depression is a complex psychiatric disorder, and individuals with depression show heterogeneous symptoms ranging from somatic symptoms, panic attacks, obsessive behavior, ruminations, poor concentration to suicidal thoughts (154). Thus, stratifying individuals with heterogeneous depressive symptoms in an objective fashion based on amygdala connectivity may be useful for clinicians to plan more effective and individualized treatments for these heterogeneous patients. In this vein, our findings in Figures 4 and 5 further extended previous studies on neuroimaging correlates of total depressive symptom severity in TBI (40, 41, 52, 54, 56, 59).

We were also able to identify sensitive neuroimaging biomarker for subtypes of depressive symptoms with greater regional specificity than previous DTI study (58). In contrast to Strain et al. (58) that showed spatially overlapped correlation patterns across subtypes of depressive symptoms in concussion, our study demonstrated spatially dissociable patterns of amygdala connectivity according to subtypes of depressive symptoms. Specifically, the cognitive factor was correlated with right amygdala connectivity in the aPFC, amPFC, and left superior central sulcus of the DMN and SMN (Figure 4). The affective factor was correlated with left amygdala connectivity in the cerebral areas of the SN, DAN, VN, SMN, and FPCN and the cerebellum area of the SMN, FPCN, and SN (Figure 4).

One could wonder why statistically significant correlations between the Buckley cognitive factor and amygdala connectivity occurred in the areas of the SMN. Note that the Buckley BDI-II cognitive factor includes test items for sadness, pessimism, past failure, guilty feelings, punishment feelings, self-dislike, self-criticalness, suicidal thoughts, and worthless (Table 2). Thus, we should not confuse the Buckley cognitive factor with executive function, working memory, or reasoning in the context of cognitive neuroscience.

Amygdala connectivity was associated with only the Buckley cognitive and affective factors. This finding may be explained by differences in sensitivity to depressive severity over the Buckley factors. A previous study that investigated optimal BDI items for discriminating depressive severity in 335 medical patients, 16% of whom were neurologically impaired, identified that the items related to past failure, self-dissatisfaction, punishment feelings, suicidal thoughts, crying, loss of social interest, and indecisiveness well discriminated depressive severity in the medical patients (155). Three of these seven items are related to the Buckley BDI-II cognitive factor and the other four items are related to the Buckley BDI-II affective factor. Thus, cognitive and affective factors might have sufficient sensitivity to depressive symptom severity that enables to yield statistically significant correlations with amygdala connectivity across our TBI participants with depressive symptoms.

Although our findings of relationships between amygdala connectivity and the Buckley BDI sub-scores were interesting, the data-driven connectivity analysis over 268 nodes revealed that these relationships could be better represented via other seeds (Figure 6; Figure S8 in Supplementary Material). Furthermore, we observed positive associations between the Buckley BDI cognitive factor and connectivity with most of the other seeds than the amygdala (Figure S8 in Supplementary Material). These results demonstrates limitations of seed-based approaches that warrant complex network approaches in future directions to more comprehensively characterize the relationships between network topology and the BDI sub-scores in chronic TBI.

Limitations and Future Research

The present study has several limitations. First, we assessed depressive symptom severity based on self-reports from the TBI participants. Although the BDI-II is one of the most widely used measures for depressive symptoms and shows good reliability (62), frequent impairments in self-awareness among TBI individuals could bias the reported depressive symptoms obtained in the BDI-II questionnaire (156). Thus, our findings should be interpreted with caution due to this limitation and clinical diagnosis for depression should not be made based exclusively on participants’ BDI-II total scores. Second, similar to the first limitation, we retrospectively identified the existence of brain injury, and estimated initial injury severity and the duration of LOC from the OSU TBI screening form (71), as opposed to reporting the gold standard measure for initial injury severity (i.e., GCS) and other clinical information from the acute-care, inpatient facilities where they were hospitalized sometimes multiple years ago. Thus, our study participants may be best characterized as individuals with a self-identified, probable history of TBI. Although we made our best efforts to identify TBI and estimate the clinical information at the acute stage such as initial injury severity and the duration of LOC utilizing the OSU TBI-ID method, whose validity and reliability have been demonstrated (71, 72), our findings should be interpreted within this limitation. Third, our TBI groups are a mixture of individuals with probable mild, probable moderate, and probable severe TBI, which may not be ideal, particularly for studies in sub-acute (3–6 months post-injury) and short-term chronic (6 months–2 years post-injury) stages of TBI. However, at the long-term chronic stage of TBI (>2 years post-injury), initial injury severity often plays less critical role in characterizing TBI individuals at the time of study (157, 158) and chronic TBI studies occasionally have reported with a mixture of different injury severity (10, 12, 13, 15, 19, 55, 158). In our case, there were no systematic effects of estimated injury severity on the BDI-II total scores or amygdala connectivity (Figures S6 and S7 in Supplementary Material). Nonetheless, care should be taken in the interpretations of our findings as injury severity was retrospectively estimated. Fourth, we discussed our findings in the context of previous resting-state functional connectivity studies in depressive individuals without other comorbid neurological conditions because no previous resting-state functional connectivity studies in TBI with comorbid depressive symptoms were reported. We do not know whether the patterns of altered amygdala in TBI individuals with comorbid depressive symptoms are similar to those of depressed individuals without TBI. This is a complex issue, due to the differences that likely exist between depressive symptoms in uninjured individuals and those who suffer from TBI-related symptoms. It may be possible that future studies that include additional groups of healthy individuals and individuals with depressive symptoms but without TBI would be required to address this concern. Fifth, one could argue that our findings may be driven by the effects of PTSD symptom severity on amygdala connectivity because of the frequently linked comorbidity of depression and PTSD in TBI (104) and statistically significant group differences in PTSD symptom severity in our groups. However, we included PTSD symptom severity and its interactions with other factors as covariates. Furthermore, (1) spatial extent of the regions that showed statistically significant effects of these covariates and (2) overlaps between these regions and the regions that showed group differences in amygdala connectivity were minimal (Figure S5 in Supplementary Material). Thus, our findings on altered amygdala connectivity in the TBI-plus-depressive symptoms group were primarily associated with depressive symptom severity rather than PTSD symptom severity. Sixth, we were not able to measure other potential confounding factors such as genetic predisposition, environment, other anxiety disorders, and chronic pain levels present in the participants. Especially in future amygdala connectivity studies in TBI with depressive symptoms, gene, and environment should be measured because of reported influences of genes and environment on amygdala connectivity in depression (159). Seventh, we investigated amygdala connectivity with the amygdala as a single entity. In fact, the amygdala consists of structurally and functionally distinct nuclei. For example, laterobasal, centromedial, and superficial amygdala subdivisions have different functional connectivity patterns in healthy individuals (124). Future amygdala connectivity studies with amygdala subdivisions may be further elucidate alteration patterns in TBI with depressive symptoms. Lastly, our sample consisted of individuals who were often multiple years post-TBI, and individuals who had experienced multiple TBI incidents. This leads to the possibility that connectivity alterations may be different in individuals who are evaluated closer in time to their TBI incident.

Our future studies include an assessment of if and how altered amygdala connectivity in chronic TBI with comorbid depressive symptoms may be reorganized following rehabilitation. We will also further identify alteration patterns utilizing graph theory. Finally, we will address concerns discussed above in our future communications.

Conclusion

In conclusion, we demonstrated pronounced, widespread alterations in amygdala connectivity among chronic TBI individuals with comorbid depressive symptoms. Such widespread alterations in amygdala connectivity indicate alterations in modularity roles of the amygdala in emotion processes among TBI individuals with depressive symptoms. Amygdala connectivity also showed spatially dissociable patterns of correlation with symptoms severity according to different subtypes of depressive symptoms. Taken together, our findings suggest that amygdala connectivity may be a potentially effective neuroimaging biomarker for comorbid depressive symptoms among individuals with chronic TBI.

Author Contributions

All authors were involved in the conceptualization and design of this study, the drafting and revising of the manuscript, and the interpretation of the data. KH substantially contributed to the data analysis.

Disclaimer

The views and opinions expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not reflect the official policy or position of the Department of the Army, Department of the Air Force, Department of Defense, or United States Government.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all study participants and their families that supported this study. Our thanks also go to Mr. Weikei Yu and Mr. Michael Holloway for the acquisition of MRI scans, Ms. Tiffani Jantz, Mr. David Martinez and Ms. Jellena Rakic for the acquisition of neuropsychological test scores and Mr. Barry Rodgers for his assistance with the analysis of neuropsychological test scores. Lastly, we would like to thank Dr. Ian Robertson and Dr. Vas Asha for their helpful suggestions. This work has been supported by Department of Defense CDMRP grants W81XWH-11-2-0194 to DCK and W81XWH-11-2-0195 to SBC and a grant from the Meadows Foundation to DCK and SBC.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fneur.2015.00231

Footnotes

References

1. Ashman TA, Spielman LA, Hibbard MR, Silver JM, Chandna T, Gordon WA. Psychiatric challenges in the first 6 years after traumatic brain injury: cross-sequential analyses of axis I disorders. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2004) 85:36–42. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2003.08.117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Deb S, Lyons I, Koutzoukis C, Ali I, McCarthy G. Rate of psychiatric illness 1 year after traumatic brain injury. Am J Psychiatry (1999) 156:374–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

3. Fann JR, Katon WJ, Uomoto JM, Esselman PC. Psychiatric disorders and functional disability in outpatients with traumatic brain injuries. Am J Psychiatry (1995) 152:1493–9. doi:10.1176/ajp.152.10.1493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hibbard MR, Uysal S, Kepler K, Bogdany J, Silver J. Axis I psychopathology in individuals with traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil (1998) 13:24–39. doi:10.1097/00001199-199808000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. van Reekum R, Bolago I, Finlayson MAJ, Garner S, Links PS. Psychiatric disorders after traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj (1996) 10:319–28. doi:10.1080/026990596124340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Bombardier CH, Fann JR, Temkin NR, Esselman PC, Barber J, Dikmen SS. Rates of major depressive disorder and clinical outcomes following traumatic brain injury. JAMA (2010) 303:1938–45. doi:10.1001/jama.2010.599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Chamelian L, Feinstein A. The effect of major depression on subjective and objective cognitive deficits in mild to moderate traumatic brain injury. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci (2006) 18:33–8. doi:10.1176/jnp.18.1.33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Jorge RE, Robinson RG, Moser D, Tateno A, Crespo-Facorro B, Arndt S. Major depression following traumatic brain injury. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2004) 61:42–50. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.61.1.42

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Rapoport MJ, McCullagh S, Shammi P, Feinstein A. Cognitive impairment associated with major depression following mild and moderate traumatic brain injury. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci (2005) 17:61–5. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.17.1.61

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Findler M, Cantor J, Haddad L, Gordon W, Ashman T. The reliability and validity of the SF-36 health survey questionnaire for use with individuals with traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj (2001) 15:715–23. doi:10.1080/02699050010013941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Haagsma JA, Scholten AC, Andriessen TMJC, Vos PE, van Beeck EF, Polinder S. Impact of depression and post-traumatic stress disorder on functional outcome and health-related quality of life of patients with mild traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma (2015) 32:853–62. doi:10.1089/neu.2013.3283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Hibbard MR, Ashman TA, Spielman LA, Chun D, Charatz HJ, Melvin S. Relationship between depression and psychosocial functioning after traumatic brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (2004) 85:43–53. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2003.08.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Silver JM, Kramer R, Greenwald S, Weissman M. The association between head injuries and psychiatric disorders: findings from the New Haven NIMH epidemiologic catchment area study. Brain Inj (2001) 15:935–45. doi:10.1080/02699050110065295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Hibbard MR, Gordon WA, Flanagan S, Haddad L, Labinsky E. Sexual dysfunction after traumatic brain injury. NeuroRehabilitation (2000) 15:107–20.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

15. Brown M, Gordon WA, Spielman L. Participation in social and recreational activity in the community by individuals with traumatic brain injury. Rehabil Psychol (2003) 48:266–74. doi:10.1037/0090-5550.48.4.266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Mooney G, Speed J, Sheppard S. Factors related to recovery after mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj (2005) 19:975–87. doi:10.1080/02699050500110264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Sharp DJ, Scott G, Leech R. Network dysfunction after traumatic brain injury. Nat Rev Neurol (2014) 10:156–66. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2014.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Mayer AR, Mannell MV, Ling J, Gasparovic C, Yeo RA. Functional connectivity in mild traumatic brain injury. Hum Brain Mapp (2011) 32:1825–35. doi:10.1002/hbm.21151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Sharp DJ, Beckmann CF, Greenwood R, Kinnunen KM, Bonnelle V, de Boissezon X, et al. Default mode network functional and structural connectivity after traumatic brain injury. Brain (2011) 134:2233–47. doi:10.1093/brain/awr175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Slobounov SM, Gay M, Zhang K, Johnson B, Pennell D, Sebastianelli W, et al. Alteration of brain functional network at rest and in response to YMCA physical stress test in concussed athletes: rsFMRI study. Neuroimage (2011) 55:1716–27. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Tang L, Ge Y, Sodickson DK, Miles L, Zhou Y, Reaume J, et al. Thalamic resting-state functional networks: disruption in patients with mild traumatic brain injury. Radiology (2011) 260:831–40. doi:10.1148/radiol.11110014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Nakamura T, Hillary FG, Biswal BB. Resting network plasticity following brain injury. PLoS One (2009) 4:e8220. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0008220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Pandit AS, Expert P, Lambiotte R, Bonnelle V, Leech R, Turkheimer FE, et al. Traumatic brain injury impairs small-world topology. Neurology (2013) 80:1826–33. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182929f38

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Han K, Mac Donald CL, Johnson AM, Barnes Y, Wierzechowski L, Zonies D, et al. Disrupted modular organization of resting-state cortical functional connectivity in U.S. military personnel following concussive ‘mild’ blast-related traumatic brain injury. Neuroimage (2014) 84:76–96. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Smith DH, Meaney DF, Shull WH. Diffuse axonal injury in head trauma. J Head Trauma Rehabil (2003) 18:307–16. doi:10.1097/00001199-200307000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Mayberg HS. Targeted electrode-based modulation of neural circuits for depression. J Clin Invest (2009) 119:717–25. doi:10.1172/JCI38454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Harro J, Oreland L. Depression as a spreading adjustment disorder of monoaminergic neurons: a case for primary implication of the locus coeruleus. Brain Res Rev (2001) 38:79–128. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00082-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hamilton JP, Chen MC, Gotlib IH. Neural systems approaches to understanding major depressive disorder: an intrinsic functional organization perspective. Neurobiol Dis (2013) 52:4–11. doi:10.1016/j.nbd.2012.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Dutta A, McKie S, Deakin JFW. Resting state networks in major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res (2014) 224:139–51. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Northoff G, Wiebking C, Feinberg T, Panksepp J. The ‘resting-state hypothesis’ of major depressive disorder – a translational subcortical–cortical framework for a system disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2011) 35:1929–45. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Wang L, Hermens DF, Hickie IB, Lagopoulos J. A systematic review of resting-state functional-MRI studies in major depression. J Affect Disord (2012) 142:6–12. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2012.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Greicius MD, Flores BH, Menon V, Glover GH, Solvason HB, Kenna H, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity in major depression: abnormally increased contributions from subgenual cingulate cortex and thalamus. Biol Psychiatry (2007) 62:429–37. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Vincent JL, Kahn I, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME, Buckner RL. Evidence for a frontoparietal control system revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol (2008) 100:3328–42. doi:10.1152/jn.90355.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Kaiser RH, Andrews-Hanna JR, Wager TD, Pizzagalli DA. Large-scale network dysfunction in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. JAMA Psychiatry (2015) 72:603–11. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci (2007) 27:2349–56. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Manoliu A, Meng C, Brandl F, Doll A, Tahmasian M, Scherr M, et al. Insular dysfunction within the salience network is associated with severity of symptoms and aberrant inter-network connectivity in major depressive disorder. Front Hum Neurosci (2014) 7:930. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Sheline YI, Price JL, Yan Z, Mintun MA. Resting-state functional MRI in depression unmasks increased connectivity between networks via the dorsal nexus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2010) 107:11020–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.1000446107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Ochsner KN, Ray RR, Hughes B, McRae K, Cooper JC, Weber J, et al. Bottom-up and top-down processes in emotion generation: common and distinct neural mechanisms. Psychol Sci (2009) 20:1322–31. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02459.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Anand A, Li Y, Wang Y, Wu J, Gao S, Bukhari L, et al. Activity and connectivity of brain mood regulating circuit in depression: a functional magnetic resonance study. Biol Psychiatry (2005) 57:1079–88. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Chen C-H, Suckling J, Ooi C, Fu CHY, Williams SCR, Walsh ND, et al. Functional coupling of the amygdala in depressed patients treated with antidepressant medication. Neuropsychopharmacol (2008) 33:1909–18. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Chen J-K, Johnston KM, Petrides M, Ptito A. Neural substrates of symptoms of depression following concussion in male athletes with persisting postconcussion symptoms. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2008) 65:81–9. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Drevets WC, Price JL, Simpson JR, Todd RD, Reich T, Vannier M, et al. Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders. Nature (1997) 386:824–7. doi:10.1038/386824a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Lui S, Wu Q, Qiu L, Yang X, Kuang W, Chan RCK, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity in treatment-resistant depression. Am J Psychiatry (2011) 168:642–8. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10101419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Matthews SC, Strigo IA, Simmons AN, Yang TT, Paulus MP. Decreased functional coupling of the amygdala and supragenual cingulate is related to increased depression in unmedicated individuals with current major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord (2008) 111:13–20. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2008.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Mayberg HS. Limbic-cortical dysregulation: a proposed model of depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci (1997) 9:471–81. doi:10.1176/jnp.9.3.471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Ramasubbu R, Konduru N, Cortese F, Bray S, Gaxiola-Valdez I, Goodyear B. Reduced intrinsic connectivity of amygdala in adults with major depressive disorder. Front Psychiatry (2014) 5:17. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Siegle GJ, Steinhauer SR, Thase ME, Stenger VA, Carter CS. Can’t shake that feeling: event-related fMRI assessment of sustained amygdala activity in response to emotional information in depressed individuals. Biol Psychiatry (2002) 51:693–707. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01314-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Tahmasian M, Knight DC, Manoliu A, Schwerthöffer D, Scherr M, Meng C, et al. Aberrant intrinsic connectivity of hippocampus and amygdala overlap in the fronto-insular and dorsomedial-prefrontal cortex in major depressive disorder. Front Hum Neurosci (2013) 7:639. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Price JL, Drevets WC. Neural circuits underlying the pathophysiology of mood disorders. Trends Cogn Sci (2012) 16:61–71. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2001) 98:676–82. doi:10.1073/pnas.98.2.676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Bonnelle V, Ham TE, Leech R, Kinnunen KM, Mehta MA, Greenwood RJ, et al. Salience network integrity predicts default mode network function after traumatic brain injury. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2012) 109:4690–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113455109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Hudak A, Warner M, Marquez de la Plata C, Moore C, Harper C, Diaz-Arrastia R. Brain morphometry changes and depressive symptoms after traumatic brain injury. Psychiatry Res (2011) 191:160–5. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Jorge RE, Acion L, Starkstein SE, Magnotta V. Hippocampal volume and mood disorders after traumatic brain injury. Biol Psychiatry (2007) 62:332–8. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Maller JJ, Thomson RHS, Pannek K, Bailey N, Lewis PM, Fitzgerald PB. Volumetrics relate to the development of depression after traumatic brain injury. Behav Brain Res (2014) 271:147–53. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2014.05.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Schönberger M, Ponsford J, Reutens D, Beare R, Clarke D, O’Sullivan R. The relationship between mood disorders and MRI findings following traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj (2011) 25:543–50. doi:10.3109/02699052.2011.565013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Maller JJ, Thomson RHS, Pannek K, Rose SE, Bailey N, Lewis PM, et al. The (eigen)value of diffusion tensor imaging to investigate depression after traumatic brain injury. Hum Brain Mapp (2014) 35:227–37. doi:10.1002/hbm.22171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Rao V, Mielke M, Xu X, Smith GS, McCann UD, Bergey A, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging atlas-based analyses in major depression after mild traumatic brain injury. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci (2012) 24:309–15. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.11080188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Strain J, Didehbani N, Cullum CM, Mansinghani S, Conover H, Kraut MA, et al. Depressive symptoms and white matter dysfunction in retired NFL players with concussion history. Neurology (2013) 81:25–32. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e318299ccf8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Matthews SC, Strigo IA, Simmons AN, O’Connell RM, Reinhardt LE, Moseley SA. A multimodal imaging study in U.S. veterans of Operations Iraqi and Enduring Freedom with and without major depression after blast-related concussion. Neuroimage (2011) 54:S69–75. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Rao V, Munro CA, Rosenberg P, Ward J, Bertrand M, Degoankar M, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of depression in post traumatic brain injury: preliminary results of a pilot study. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci (2010) 22:231–5. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.22.2.231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Romero K, Black SE, Feinstein A. Differences in cerebral perfusion deficits in mild traumatic brain injury and depression using single-photon emission computed tomography. Front Neurol (2014) 5:158. doi:10.3389/fneur.2014.00158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Beck AT, Steer RA, Carbin MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev (1988) 8:77–100. doi:10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Palmer GA, Happe MC, Paxson JM, Jurek BK, Graca JJ, Olson SA. Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory-II for OEF/OIF veterans in a polytrauma sample. Mil Med (2014) 179:879–84. doi:10.7205/MILMED-D-14-00048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Rowland SM, Lam CS, Leahy B. Use of the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) with persons with traumatic brain injury: analysis of factorial structure. Brain Inj (2005) 19:77–83. doi:10.1080/02699050410001719988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Siegert RJ, Walkey FH, Turner-Stokes L. An examination of the factor structure of the Beck Depression Inventory-II in a neurorehabilitation inpatient sample. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2009) 15:142–7. doi:10.1017/S1355617708090048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Rosenthal M, Christensen BK, Ross TP. Depression following traumatic brain injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil (1998) 79:90–103. doi:10.1016/S0003-9993(98)90215-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Buckley TC, Parker JD, Heggie J. A psychometric evaluation of the BDI-II in treatment-seeking substance abusers. J Subst Abuse Treat (2001) 20:197–204. doi:10.1016/S0740-5472(00)00169-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Krawczyk DC, Marquez de la Plata C, Schauer GF, Vas AK, Keebler M, Tuthill S, et al. Evaluating the effectiveness of reasoning training in military and civilian chronic traumatic brain injury patients: study protocol. Trials (2013) 14:29. doi:10.1186/1745-6215-14-29

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Wilson JTL, Pettigrew LEL, Teasdale GM. Structured interviews for the Glasgow outcome scale and the extended Glasgow outcome scale: guidelines for their use. J Neurotrauma (1998) 15:573–85. doi:10.1089/neu.1998.15.573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Teasdale G, Jennett B. Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. Lancet (1974) 304:81–4. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(74)91639-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Corrigan JD, Bogner J. Initial reliability and validity of the Ohio State University TBI identification method. J Head Trauma Rehabil (2007) 22:318–29. doi:10.1097/01.HTR.0000300227.67748.77

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Bogner J, Corrigan JD. Reliability and predictive validity of the Ohio State University TBI identification method with prisoners. J Head Trauma Rehabil (2009) 24:279–91. doi:10.1097/HTR.0b013e3181a66356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. Report to Congress on Mild Traumatic Brain Injury in the United States: Steps to Prevent a Serious Health Problem. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2003).

Google Scholar

74. Thurman D, Sniezek J, Johnson D, Greenspan A, Smith S. Guidelines for Surveillance of Central Nervous System Injury. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (1995).

Google Scholar

75. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. BDI-II, Beck Depression Inventory: Manual. 2nd ed. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; Harcourt Brace (1996).

Google Scholar

76. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (1960) 23:56–62. doi:10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Hamilton M. The assessment of anxiety states by rating. Br J Med Psychol (1959) 32:50–5. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8341.1959.tb00467.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Rabinak CA, Angstadt M, Welsh RC, Kenndy AE, Lyubkin M, Martis B, et al. Altered amygdala resting-state functional connectivity in post-traumatic stress disorder. Front Psychiatry (2011) 2:62. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2011.00062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Sripada R, King A, Garfinkel S, Wang X, Sripada C, Welsh R, et al. Altered resting-state amygdala functional connectivity in men with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci (2012) 37:241–9. doi:10.1503/jpn.110069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Weathers F, Litz B, Herman D, Huska J, Keane T. The PTSD Checklist (PCL): Reliability, Validity, and Diagnostic Utility. In Annual Meeting of the International Society of Traumatic Stress Studies. San Antonio, TX: International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies (1993).

Google Scholar

81. Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Pearson Education, Inc (1999).

Google Scholar

82. Wechsler D. Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (2001).

Google Scholar

83. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third Edition (WAIS–III). San Antonio, TX: Pearson Education, Inc (1997).

Google Scholar

84. Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH. D-KEFS Executive Function System: Examiners Manual. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Education, Inc (2001).

Google Scholar

85. Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale – Fourth Edition (WMS-IV). San Antonio, TX: Pearson Education, Inc (2008).

Google Scholar

86. Hanten G, Chapman SB, Gamino JF, Zhang L, Benton SB, Stallings-Roberson G, et al. Verbal selective learning after traumatic brain injury in children. Ann Neurol (2004) 56:847–53. doi:10.1002/ana.20298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Diener E, Emmons RA, Larsen RJ, Griffin S. The satisfaction with life scale. J Pers Assess (1985) 49:71–5. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Cox RW. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res (1996) 29:162–73. doi:10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Evans AC, Collins DL, Mills SR, Brown ED, Kelly RL, Peters TM. 3D statistical neuroanatomical models from 305 MRI volumes. Proc IEEE Nucl Sci Symp Med Imaging Conf (1993) 3:1813–7. doi:10.1109/NSSMIC.1993.373602

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Johnstone T, Walsh KSO, Greischar LL, Alexander AL, Fox AS, Davidson RJ, et al. Motion correction and the use of motion covariates in multiple-subject fmri analysis. Hum Brain Mapp (2006) 27:779–88. doi:10.1002/hbm.20219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, van Essen DC, Raichle ME. From the cover: the human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2005) 102:9673–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.0504136102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Gotts SJ, Saad ZS, Jo HJ, Wallace GL, Cox RW, Martin A. The perils of global signal regression for group comparisons: a case study of autism spectrum disorders. Front Hum Neurosci (2013) 7:356. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Jo HJ, Gotts SJ, Reynolds RC, Bandettini PA, Martin A, Cox RW, et al. Effective preprocessing procedures virtually eliminate distance-dependent motion artifacts in resting state FMRI. J Appl Math (2013) 2013:1–9. doi:10.1155/2013/935154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Murphy K, Birn RM, Handwerker DA, Jones TB, Bandettini PA. The impact of global signal regression on resting state correlations: are anti-correlated networks introduced? Neuroimage (2009) 44:893–905. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Saad ZS, Gotts SJ, Murphy K, Chen G, Jo HJ, Martin A, et al. Trouble at rest: how correlation patterns and group differences become distorted after global signal regression. Brain Connect (2012) 2:25–32. doi:10.1089/brain.2012.0080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Saad ZS, Reynolds RC, Jo HJ, Gotts SJ, Chen G, Martin A, et al. Correcting brain-wide correlation differences in resting-state FMRI. Brain Connect (2013) 3:339–52. doi:10.1089/brain.2013.0156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage (2012) 59:2142–54. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Loughead J, Ruparel K, Elliott MA, Hakonarson H, et al. Impact of in-scanner head motion on multiple measures of functional connectivity: relevance for studies of neurodevelopment in youth. Neuroimage (2012) 60:623–32. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. van Dijk KRA, Sabuncu MR, Buckner RL. The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage (2012) 59:431–8. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Power JD, Mitra A, Laumann TO, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage (2014) 84:320–41. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. van Dijk KRA, Hedden T, Venkataraman A, Evans KC, Lazar SW, Buckner RL. Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties, and optimization. J Neurophysiol (2010) 103:297–321. doi:10.1152/jn.00783.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Landeau B, Papathanassiou D, Crivello F, Etard O, Delcroix N, et al. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage (2002) 15:273–89. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Sakaki M, Nga L, Mather M. Amygdala functional connectivity with medial prefrontal cortex at rest predicts the positivity effect in older adults’ memory. J Cogn Neurosci (2013) 25:1206–24. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Levin HS, Brown SA, Song JX, McCauley SSR, Boake C, Contant CF, et al. Depression and posttraumatic stress disorder at three months after mild to moderate traumatic brain injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (2001) 23:754–69. doi:10.1076/jcen.23.6.754.1021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Nichols T, Brett M, Andersson J, Wager T, Poline J-B. Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage (2005) 25:653–60. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Buckner RL, Krienen FM, Castellanos A, Diaz JC, Yeo BTT. The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol (2011) 106:2322–45. doi:10.1152/jn.00339.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol (2011) 106:1125–65. doi:10.1152/jn.00338.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Vanheule S, Desmet M, Groenvynck H, Rosseel Y, Fontaine J. The factor structure of the Beck Depression Inventory-II: an evaluation. Assessment (2008) 15:177–87. doi:10.1177/1073191107311261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Cao H, Plichta MM, Schäfer A, Haddad L, Grimm O, Schneider M, et al. Test–retest reliability of fMRI-based graph theoretical properties during working memory, emotion processing, and resting state. Neuroimage (2014) 84:888–900. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, et al. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron (2011) 72:665–78. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Bilek E, Schafer A, Ochs E, Esslinger C, Zangl M, Plichta MM, et al. Application of high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation to the DLPFC alters human prefrontal-hippocampal functional interaction. J Neurosci (2013) 33:7050–6. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3081-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Zacks JM, Raichle ME. Coherent spontaneous activity accounts for trial-to-trial variability in human evoked brain responses. Nat Neurosci (2006) 9:23–5. doi:10.1038/nn1616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Lancaster JL, Tordesillas-Gutiérrez D, Martinez M, Salinas F, Evans A, Zilles K, et al. Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template. Hum Brain Mapp (2007) 28:1194–205. doi:10.1002/hbm.20345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Ham TE, Bonnelle V, Hellyer P, Jilka S, Robertson IH, Leech R, et al. The neural basis of impaired self-awareness after traumatic brain injury. Brain (2014) 137:586–97. doi:10.1093/brain/awt350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Han K, Talavage TM. Effects of combining field strengths on auditory functional MRI group analysis: 1.5T and 3T. J Magn Reson Imaging (2011) 34:1480–8. doi:10.1002/jmri.22823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. van Essen DC. A population-average, landmark- and surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage (2005) 28:635–62. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. van Essen DC, Drury HA, Dickson J, Harwell J, Hanlon D, Anderson CH. An integrated software suite for surface-based analyses of cerebral cortex. J Am Med Inform Assoc (2001) 8:443–59. doi:10.1136/jamia.2001.0080443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn Reson Med (1995) 34:537–41. doi:10.1002/mrm.1910340409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, et al. Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA (2009) 106:13040–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.0905267106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Fox MD, Corbetta M, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Raichle ME. Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2006) 103:10046–51. doi:10.1073/pnas.0604187103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Lowe MJ, Mock BJ, Sorenson JA. Functional connectivity in single and multislice echoplanar imaging using resting-state fluctuations. Neuroimage (1998) 7:119–32. doi:10.1006/nimg.1997.0315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Veer IM, Beckmann CF, van Tol MJ, Ferrarini L, Milles J, Veltman DJ, et al. Whole brain resting-state analysis reveals decreased functional connectivity in major depression. Front Syst Neurosci (2010) 4:41. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2010.00041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Wei M, Qin J, Yan R, Bi K, Liu C, Yao Z, et al. Association of resting-state network dysfunction with their dynamics of inter-network interactions in depression. J Affect Disord (2015) 174:527–34. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Roy AK, Shehzad Z, Margulies DS, Kelly AMC, Uddin LQ, Gotimer K, et al. Functional connectivity of the human amygdala using resting state fMRI. Neuroimage (2009) 45:614–26. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Öngür D, Ferry AT, Price JL. Architectonic subdivision of the human orbital and medial prefrontal cortex. J Comp Neurol (2003) 460:425–49. doi:10.1002/cne.10609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Johansen-Berg H, Gutman DA, Behrens TEJ, Matthews PM, Rushworth MFS, Katz E, et al. Anatomical connectivity of the subgenual cingulate region targeted with deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression. Cereb Cortex (2008) 18:1374–83. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Price JL, Drevets WC. Neurocircuitry of mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology (2010) 35:192–216. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Zhang X, Zhu X, Wang X, Zhu X, Zhong M, Yi J, et al. First-episode medication-naive major depressive disorder is associated with altered resting brain function in the affective network. PLoS One (2014) 9:e85241. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0085241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Reiman EM, Lane RD, Ahern GL, Schwartz GE, Davidson RJ, Friston KJ, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of externally and internally generated human emotion. Am J Psychiatry (1997) 154:918–25. doi:10.1176/ajp.154.7.918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Elliott R, Baker SC, Rogers RD, O’Leary DA, Paykel ES, Frith CD, et al. Prefrontal dysfunction in depressed patients performing a complex planning task: a study using positron emission tomography. Psychol Med (1997) 27:931–42. doi:10.1017/S0033291797005187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci (2002) 3:215–29. doi:10.1038/nrn755

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Sripada C, Angstadt M, Kessler D, Phan KL, Liberzon I, Evans GW, et al. Volitional regulation of emotions produces distributed alterations in connectivity between visual, attention control, and default networks. Neuroimage (2014) 89:110–21. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Vuilleumier P. How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cogn Sci (2005) 9:585–94. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Taylor JG, Fragopanagos NF. The interaction of attention and emotion. Neural Netw (2005) 18:353–69. doi:10.1016/j.neunet.2005.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Applegate C, Kapp B, Underwood M, Mcnall C. Autonomic and somatomotor effects of amygdala central N. stimulation in awake rabbits. Physiol Behav (1983) 31:353–60. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(83)90201-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. de Pasquale F, Sabatini U, Penna SD, Sestieri C, Caravasso CF, Formisano R, et al. The connectivity of functional cores reveals different degrees of segregation and integration in the brain at rest. Neuroimage (2013) 69:51–61. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Sagaspe P, Schwartz S, Vuilleumier P. Fear and stop: a role for the amygdala in motor inhibition by emotional signals. Neuroimage (2011) 55:1825–35. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Amaral DG, Insausti R. Retrograde transport of D-[3H]-aspartate injected into the monkey amygdaloid complex. Exp Brain Res (1992) 88:375–88. doi:10.1007/BF02259113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Guo W, Liu F, Xue Z, Gao K, Liu Z, Xiao C, et al. Abnormal resting-state cerebellar–cerebral functional connectivity in treatment-resistant depression and treatment sensitive depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2013) 44:51–7. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Zeng L-L, Shen H, Liu L, Wang L, Li B, Fang P, et al. Identifying major depression using whole-brain functional connectivity: a multivariate pattern analysis. Brain (2012) 135:1498–507. doi:10.1093/brain/aws059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Pessoa L, Adolphs R. Emotion processing and the amygdala: from a ‘low road’ to ‘many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nat Rev Neurosci (2010) 11:773–83. doi:10.1038/nrn2920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Adolphs R. Fear, faces, and the human amygdala. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2008) 18:166–72. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2008.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Hsu M, Bhatt M, Adolphs R, Tranel D, Camerer CF. Neural systems responding to degrees of uncertainty in human decision-making. Science (2005) 310:1680–3. doi:10.1126/science.1115327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. Sander D, Grafman J, Zalla T. The human amygdala: an evolved system for relevance detection. Rev Neurosci (2003) 14:303–16. doi:10.1515/REVNEURO.2003.14.4.303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Whalen PJ. Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity: initial neuroimaging studies of the human amygdala. Curr Direct Psychol Sci (1998) 7:177–88. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10836912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Schmahmann JD. The role of the cerebellum in cognition and emotion: personal reflections since 1982 on the dysmetria of thought hypothesis, and its historical evolution from theory to therapy. Neuropsychol Rev (2010) 20:236–60. doi:10.1007/s11065-010-9142-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Stoodley CJ. The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional imaging studies. Cerebellum (2012) 11:352–65. doi:10.1007/s12311-011-0260-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. Alalade E, Denny K, Potter G, Steffens D, Wang L. Altered cerebellar-cerebral functional connectivity in geriatric depression. PLoS One (2011) 6:e20035. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Liu Z, Xu C, Xu Y, Wang Y, Zhao B, Lv Y, et al. Decreased regional homogeneity in insula and cerebellum: a resting-state fMRI study in patients with major depression and subjects at high risk for major depression. Psychiatry Res (2010) 182:211–5. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Guo W-B, Sun X-L, Liu L, Xu Q, Wu R-R, Liu Z-N, et al. Disrupted regional homogeneity in treatment-resistant depression: a resting-state fMRI study. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2011) 35:1297–302. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Guo W, Liu F, Liu J, Yu L, Zhang Z, Zhang J, et al. Is there a cerebellar compensatory effort in first-episode, treatment-naive major depressive disorder at rest? Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2013) 46:13–8. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2013.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Peng D-H, Jiang K-D, Fang Y-R, Xu Y-F, Shen T, Long X-Y, et al. Decreased regional homogeneity in major depression as revealed by resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging. Chin Med J (Engl) (2011) 124:369–73. doi:10.3760/cma.j.issn.0366-6999.2011.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Hu X, Song X, Yuan Y, Li E, Liu J, Liu W, et al. Abnormal functional connectivity of the amygdala is associated with depression in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord (2015) 30:238–44. doi:10.1002/mds.26087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Goldberg D. The heterogeneity of “major depression”. World Psychiatry (2011) 10:226–8. doi:10.1002/j.2051-5545.2011.tb00061.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Clark DC, Cavanaugh SV, Gibbons RD. The core sysmptoms of depression in medical and psychiatric patients. J Nerv Ment Dis (1983) 171:705–13. doi:10.1097/00005053-198312000-00001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. Malec JF, Testa JA, Rush BK, Brown AW, Moessner AM. Self-assessment of impairment, impaired self-awareness, and depression after traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil (2007) 22:156–66. doi:10.1097/01.HTR.0000271116.12028.af

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

157. Arciniegas D, Olincy A, Topkoff J, McRae K, Cawthra E, Filley CM, et al. Impaired auditory gating and P50 nonsuppression following traumatic brain injury. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci (2000) 12:77–85. doi:10.1176/jnp.12.1.77

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Kinnunen KM, Greenwood R, Powell JH, Leech R, Hawkins PC, Bonnelle V, et al. White matter damage and cognitive impairment after traumatic brain injury. Brain (2010) 134:449–63. doi:10.1093/brain/awq347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. Córdova-Palomera A, Tornador C, Falcón C, Bargalló N, Nenadic I, Deco G, et al. Altered amygdalar resting-state connectivity in depression is explained by both genes and environment: amygdalar resting-state in depression: genes and environment. Hum Brain Mapp (2015) 36:3761–76. doi:10.1002/hbm.22876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: TBI, depression, functional connectivity, fMRI, amygdala, resting-state, BDI, beck depression inventory-II

Citation: Han K, Chapman SB and Krawczyk DC (2015) Altered amygdala connectivity in individuals with chronic traumatic brain injury and comorbid depressive symptoms. Front. Neurol. 6:231. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00231

Received: 24 July 2015; Accepted: 19 October 2015;
Published: 04 November 2015

Edited by:

John Lawrence Graner, Duke University, USA

Reviewed by:

Andrei Irimia, University of Southern California, USA
Frank Hillary, The Pennsylvania State University, USA

Copyright: © 2015 Han, Chapman and Krawczyk. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kihwan Han, kihwan.han@utdallas.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.