Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 14 December 2015
Sec. Movement Disorders

Age-Related Sexual Dimorphism in Temporal Discrimination and in Adult-Onset Dystonia Suggests GABAergic Mechanisms

\r\n      \r\nJohn S. Butler,&#x;&#x;John S. Butler1,†‡ \r\nInes M. Beiser,&#x;Ines M. Beiser2,3† \r\nLaura Williams,&#x;Laura Williams2,3† \r\nEavan McGovern,&#x;Eavan McGovern2,3† \r\nFiona MolloyFiona Molloy4 \r\nTim LynchTim Lynch5 \r\nDan G. HealyDan G. Healy4 \r\nHelena MooreHelena Moore6 \r\nRichard WalshRichard Walsh7 \r\nRichard B. Reilly,Richard B. Reilly1,8 \r\nSen O&#x;Riordan,Seán O’Riordan2,3 \r\nCathal Walsh,Cathal Walsh9,10Michael Hutchinson,*\r\n   Michael Hutchinson2,3*
  • 1Trinity Centre for Bioengineering, School of Engineering, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
  • 2Department of Neurology, St. Vincent’s University Hospital, Dublin, Ireland
  • 3School of Medicine and Medical Sciences, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
  • 4Beaumont Hospital, Dublin, Ireland
  • 5Dublin Neurological Institute, Mater Misericordiae University Hospital, Dublin, Ireland
  • 6Cork University Hospital, Cork, Ireland
  • 7Adelaide and Meath Hospital, Dublin, Ireland
  • 8School of Medicine, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
  • 9Department of Statistics, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
  • 10Department of Mathematics and Statistics, University of Limerick, Limerick, Ireland

Background: Adult-onset isolated focal dystonia (AOIFD) presenting in early adult life is more frequent in men, whereas in middle age it is female predominant. Temporal discrimination, an endophenotype of adult-onset idiopathic isolated focal dystonia, shows evidence of sexual dimorphism in healthy participants.

Objectives: We assessed the distinctive features of age-related sexual dimorphism of (i) sex ratios in dystonia phenotypes and (ii) sexual dimorphism in temporal discrimination in unaffected relatives of cervical dystonia patients.

Methods: We performed (i) a meta-regression analysis of the proportion of men in published cohorts of phenotypes of adult-onset dystonia in relation to their mean age of onset and (ii) an analysis of temporal discrimination thresholds in 220 unaffected first-degree relatives (125 women) of cervical dystonia patients.

Results: In 53 studies of dystonia phenotypes, the proportion of men showed a highly significant negative association with mean age of onset (p < 0.0001, pseudo-R2 = 59.6%), with increasing female predominance from 40 years of age. Age of onset and phenotype together explained 92.8% of the variance in proportion of men. Temporal discrimination in relatives under the age of 35 years is faster in women than men but the age-related rate of deterioration in women is twice that of men; after 45 years of age, men have faster temporal discrimination than women.

Conclusion: Temporal discrimination in unaffected relatives of cervical dystonia patients and sex ratios in adult-onset dystonia phenotypes show similar patterns of age-related sexual dimorphism. Such age-related sexual dimorphism in temporal discrimination and adult-onset focal dystonia may reflect common underlying mechanisms. Cerebral GABA levels have been reported to show similar age-related sexual dimorphism in healthy participants and may be the mechanism underlying the observed age-related sexual dimorphism in temporal discrimination and the sex ratios in AOIFD.

Introduction

Twenty years ago, a paper from the late David Marsden’s research group, reporting on the sex ratios of the phenotypes of adult-onset dystonia, concluded by stating: “Our study confirms a clear but mild preponderance of females with various types of craniocervical dystonia and of males with writer’s cramp. Why this is so remains to be discovered” (1).

Dystonia is a movement disorder, characterized by “sustained or intermittent muscle contractions causing abnormal, often repetitive, movements, postures, or both” (2). The most common form of dystonia, adult-onset isolated focal dystonia (AOIFD) is inherited in an autosomal dominant manner with a reduced penetrance of 12–15% (3, 4); phenotypes include cervical dystonia, blepharospasm, focal hand dystonia, spasmodic dysphonia, oromandibular dystonia, and task-specific dystonia. Evidence from studies of affected sib-pairs (5) and multiplex families (6, 7) indicates that the same presumed genetic mutation(s) may cause different phenotypes. There is an unexplained male predominance in focal hand dystonia and musician’s dystonia with a female excess in the craniocervical phenotypes.

The temporal discrimination threshold (TDT) is the shortest interval at which two sequential stimuli appear to the observer to be asynchronous; normally, the TDT is 30–50 ms depending on the gender and age (8). Abnormal TDTs are found in 97% of cervical dystonia patients (specificity: 98–100%) and somewhat less frequently in other AOIFD phenotypes (810). Abnormal TDTs show autosomal dominant transmission in families of sporadic and familial cervical dystonia patients (8, 11). In unaffected first-degree female relatives, an abnormal TDT is fully penetrant by 48 years of age; in male relatives, there is 40% penetrance after 25 years of age (12). Abnormal TDTs in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with AOIFD are associated with putaminal hypertrophy (8) and reduced putaminal fMRI activity during a temporal discrimination task (12). We have proposed that an abnormal TDT is a mediational endophenotype of AOIFD and that unaffected first-degree relatives (of AOIFD patients) with abnormal TDTs are unaffected gene carriers (13, 14).

In a group of healthy control participants, we observed that young women had faster temporal discrimination than similarly aged men (12). Further analysis indicated both sex-related and age-related effects on the TDT. TDTs worsened with age at a faster rate in women than men, so that in middle age the initial female advantage was lost; women after 40 years were significantly slower in a temporal discrimination task than men (15).

Sexual dimorphism, a phenotypic difference between males and females of the same species, in temporal discrimination is age dependent; the initial advantage of the young adult woman is lost and reversed in middle age. In this paper, we further examine this sexual dimorphism in temporal discrimination in a large group of unaffected first-degree relatives of cervical dystonia patients. We also examine by meta-analysis the relationship between mean age of onset and sex ratios in published cohorts of AOIFD phenotypes. We postulate that the age-related sexual dimorphism in temporal discrimination explains the relationship between and the mean age of onset and sex ratios of AOIFD phenotypes in both published clinical cohorts and in our cervical dystonia patients. We hypothesize that, in individuals with a genetic susceptibility to AOIFD, age- and sex-related mechanisms relating to the efficiency of temporal discrimination explain the observed sex ratios in the various AOIFD phenotypes. One further corollary hypothesis, discussed but not examined in this paper, is that sexual dimorphism in the speed of temporal discrimination reflects sexual dimorphism in physiological age-related decline in GABAergic inhibition.

Participants and Methods

Study A: Age of Onset, Phenotypes, and Sex Ratios in AOIFD

Study A Population

We searched, in January 2015, PubMed and MEDLINE for publications using the search terms “adult-onset dystonia,” “focal dystonia,” “blepharospasm,” “cervical dystonia,” focal hand dystonia,” “writer’s cramp,” “spasmodic dysphonia,” “laryngeal dystonia,” oromandibular dystonia,” “Meige syndrome,” “muscician’s dystonia,” “sex ratio,” “dystonia,” “epidemiology,” “incidence,” and “prevalence.” Our inclusion criteria were publications that reported the numbers of patients, sex ratios (proportion of men), and mean age of onset in clearly defined AOIFD phenotypes from a clinic or study sample population. Three authors (Ines M. Beiser, Michael Hutchinson, and Seán O’Riordan) screened full texts to identify study eligibility. We included only studies that were published in English; the references of all eligible studies were searched to ensure that no study was missed. Exclusion criteria were studies which did not separate secondary dystonias or dystonia-plus syndromes and repeated studies from the same geographical region (in which case the largest or most recent study was used). A table of included studies, listed by phenotype and references, is given in Table S1 in Supplementary Material.

Statistical Analysis

A mixed effects meta-regression model was fitted to the logit of the sex ratio (proportion of cases that were male to total number of cases) with (i) mean age of onset and (ii) mean age of onset plus phenotype being included as moderators in the model. The model was fitted in R version 3.1.1 using the package metafor version 1.9.3 (16). In reporting, the model’s estimates of the effect sizes and SEs, together with pseudo-R2, defined as (τRE2τME2)/τRE2 are presented (17).

Study B: Age- and Sex-Related Effects on Temporal Discrimination in Relatives of Cervical Dystonia Patients

Participants

Two hundred twenty unaffected first-degree relatives of cervical dystonia patients between the ages of 18 and 65 years (125 women, mean age 40.2 years; 95 men, mean age 39.7 years) were recruited by initial contact with cervical dystonia patients and subsequently gave full informed consent. A full medical history was taken, and the relatives were assessed for any evidence of a neurological disorder. Exclusion criteria were a history of neurological disease, including dystonia, tremor, neuropathy, visual or cognitive impairment, a history of cerebral, and cervical or brachial plexus injury. The TDT results from 175 healthy control participants (88 women, mean age 40.5 years; 87 men, mean age 21.5 years) from a previous study were included as a control group (15).

Methods: Sensory Testing: Visual and Tactile TDT Testing

Methods: sensory testing: visual and tactile TDT testing was performed in a single session; in a soundproof, darkened room as described previously. Visual stimuli (two flashing LED lights) were positioned on a table, 7° in the participant’s periphery. Tactile stimuli (non-painful electrical impulses to the index and middle fingers) were presented using square-wave stimulators (Lafayette Instruments Europe, United Kingdom) and rectangular cloth electrodes (Item # TD-141C1, Discount Disposables, St. Albans, VT, USA). The stimulus current was manually increased (in 0.1 mA steps) until the participant could reliably detect the stimuli. Visual or tactile stimuli, 5 ms in duration, were presented at 5-s intervals. The stimuli were initially synchronous and separation between pairs of stimuli was introduced in 5 ms steps. When the participant reported stimuli to be asynchronous on three consecutive occasions, the first of these was taken as the TDT. Visual and tactile testing was repeated four times on each side of the body (a total of 16 runs) in a random order and the median (milliseconds) of the four trials was used to account for a practice effect. Means of the median visual, tactile, and combined values were calculated (TDT). Testing was carried out by the research registrars according to a standardized protocol.

Statistical Analysis

To investigate the effect of age and sex on temporal discrimination, regression analyses were performed. The combined TDTs for men and for women were submitted to regression analyses with age as the continuous variable. The F values, R2 values, and corresponding p values are reported along with 95% confidence intervals, t-values, and p values for the intercept; and β value for the linear fit. To compare the intercept and β values between men and women, a regression analysis was performed on the TDT data with variables age, sex (men = 0, women = 1) and age × sex (resulting in 0 s for men and the continuous variable of age for women). The sex variable tests for differences in the intercept values between relatives and women. The age × sex variable tests for differences in the β values between men and women. To investigate intercept and β values differences between relatives and control participants, a regression analysis was performed on the TDT data with variables age, group (relatives = 0, controls = 1) and age × sex (resulting in 0 s for relatives and the continuous variable of age for controls). The group variable tests for differences in the intercept values between relatives and controls. The age × group variable tests for differences in the β values between relatives and controls. Approval for this project was obtained from the Ethics and Medical Research Committee, St. Vincent’s University Hospital.

Study C: Cervical Dystonia: Difference in Mean Age of Onset by Sex

From our clinic population of 331 cervical dystonia patients, the age of onset could be reliably determined by report in 278 patients (188 women). Three other studies, which reported mean age of onset of cervical dystonia for both sexes, were also used in a meta-analysis of mean age of onset by sex in cervical dystonia (1618).

Statistical Analysis

Meta-analysis of the mean age of onset by sex in our cohort and three other studies (1820), which reported mean age of onset of cervical dystonia for both sexes, was performed and the mean difference in age of onset between men and women presenting with cervical dystonia was determined. A random effects meta-analysis was performed using Review Manager version 5.3, Cochrane database tool (http://tech.cochrane.org/revman).

Results

Study A: Age of Onset, Phenotypes, and Sex Ratios in AOIFD

Analysis Set

A total of 78 papers fulfilling the search criteria were found. Of these, 24 studies listed both the mean age of onset and the sex ratio for the AOIFD phenotypes of interest; there were 54 papers which did not provide adequate information for analysis or were repeated studies from the same site/region. The 24 included studies generated a total of 53 reports of mean age of onset and sex ratio for the each of the five phenotypes: cervical dystonia (15 reports), focal hand dystonia (12 reports), musician’s dystonia (3 reports), laryngeal dystonia (9 reports), and blepharospasm (14 reports). A list of all analyzed studies and phenotypes is available in Table S1 in Supplementary Material.

Meta-Regression Analysis of Relationship Between Sex Ratio and Mean Age of Onset

For all phenotypes (53 reports of phenotypes from 24 studies), using a mixed effects meta-regression analysis, a statistically significant association between the sex ratio (proportion men) and mean age of onset was identified; the coefficient of age of onset in a model for logit (proportion men) was −0.0709, (95% CI −0.0900 to −0.0518) p < 0.0001, (Table 1). The pseudo-R2 value for this association was 59.63%. This indicates evidence of a linear decreasing male sex ratio with increasing age at symptom onset and that almost 60% of the variance in sex ratio is accounted for by age at onset. This effect is illustrated in the plot of the proportion of men versus mean age of onset in Figure 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Meta-regression analysis of relationship between mean age of onset and sex ratio in phenotypes of adult-onset dystonia.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Meta-regression analysis of the relationship between sex ratio (proportion of men) and mean age of onset in phenotypes of adult-onset dystonia. Fifty-three reports of mean age of onset and sex ratio of five adult-onset dystonia phenotypes (listed in Table S1 in Supplementary Material). In each filled circle, the diameter of the circle is proportional to the square root of the number of individuals in that study. Circle color indicates musician’s dystonia (pink), focal hand dystonia (blue), cervical dystonia (green), laryngeal dystonia (turquoise), and blepharospasm (red). A linear decreasing proportion of men (reducing male:female ratio) with increasing mean age of onset is statistically significant (p < 0.0001). The pseudo-R2 value for this association is 59.63%. The solid line is the fitted meta-regression back transformed from the logit scale. The dashed lines are the 95% confidence intervals for the mean association back transformed to the original scale.

Meta-Regression Analysis of Relationship Between Sex Ratio and Phenotype at Presentation

Using the same dataset of 53 reports in a mixed effects meta-regression analysis, a statistically significant association between the sex ratio (proportion of men) and phenotype was identified; the coefficient of phenotypes in the model for logit (proportion men) are presented in Table 1. The pseudo-R2 value for this association was 81%.

Meta-Regression Analysis of Relationship Between Sex Ratio and Both Mean Age of Onset and Phenotype at Presentation

Using the same dataset of 53 reports in a mixed effect meta-regression analysis, a statistically significant association between the sex ratio (proportion of men) and both age at onset and phenotype was identified (Table 1). The pseudo-R2 value for this association was 92.83%. This indicates that the almost 93% of the variance in sex ratio (proportion men) in adult-onset dystonia is accounted for by the combination of mean age at onset and phenotype. A model, which included interactions between phenotype and age, was also fitted but these were not found to be statistically significant.

Study B: Age- and Sex-Related Effects on Temporal Discrimination in Unaffected Relatives

The linear regression fits of TDT as a function of age for women (red) and men (blue) in 220 unaffected first-degree relatives (solid lines) and 175 control participants (broken lines) are illustrated in Figure 2. The regression for female relatives showed that age explained a significant amount of the variance in the TDT values [F(1,124) = 45.758, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.271], with a significant intercept 12.7 ms, {t(124) = 2.08, p < 0.05; 95% CI [8.254, 24.814]}, and a significant β = 0.985 ms, {t(124) = 6.76, p < 0.001; 95% CI [0.697, 1.273]} (Table S2 in Supplementary Material). In male relatives, there was a significant relationship between age and TDT values [F(1,94) = 8.9, p < 0.005, R2 = 0.087] demonstrating a significant intercept 31.09 ms, {t(94) = 5.065, p < 0.001; 95% CI [18.9, 43.283]}, and a significant β = 0.437 ms, {t(94) = 2.984, p < 0.005; 95% CI [0.22, 0.738]}. Comparison of regression analysis in relatives between women and men revealed a significantly lower intercept in women than men −18.382 ms {t(218) = −2.11, p < 0.05; 95% CI [−35.49, −1.273]} and significant β value for the interaction of sex and age 0.548 ms {t(218) = 2.646, p < 0.01; 95% CI [0.14, 0.956]}, with a 1 ms increase in the TDT every year in women; in men an increase by 0.5 ms each year. The regression line for men and the regression line for women intersected at approximately 31 years of age. As with the unaffected relatives, the regression analysis between men and women in the 175 control participants showed an effect of sex and an interaction of sex and age [see Table S2 in Supplementary Material and Ref. (15)]. Comparison of regression fits of control participants and relatives resulted in a significant difference in the β value for the interaction of age and group −0.343 {t(392) = −2.293, p < 0.05; 95% CI [−0.594, −0.049]}, with no significant effect of group (Table S2 in Supplementary Material). While both unaffected relatives and control participants at age 20 years have similar TDTs, with increasing age the TDT worsens by 0.34 ms/year in relatives compared to healthy control participants.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Sexual dimorphism in temporal discrimination in unaffected first-degree relatives of cervical dystonia patients. The effect of age and gender on the temporal discrimination threshold (in milliseconds) in 220 unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with cervical dystonia. Each open circle represents an individual relative’s temporal discrimination threshold (men: blue; women: red). The solid lines represent the regression fit of the data for men (blue) and women (red) of temporal discrimination thresholds with age. The broken red and blue lines indicate the regression fit of the temporal discrimination threshold for 175 healthy control participants [from Ref. (15)]. The sexually dimorphic effects of age on the temporal discrimination thresholds in men and women are statistically significant for both the unaffected first-degree relatives and the healthy control participants.

Study C: Cervical Dystonia: Differential Mean Age of Onset by Sex

Meta-analysis of the mean age of onset by sex in our cervical dystonia cohort in 278 patients (188 women) and 3 other studies (1820), which reported mean age of onset of cervical dystonia for both sexes is shown in Figure 3. In each of the four cohorts (total: 303 men and 506 women), men had an earlier mean age of onset than women; in the meta-analysis, the mean age of onset of cervical dystonia was significantly earlier by 4.3 years (95% CI; 1.8–6.8) in men than women (p = 0.0008). Thus, despite the overall preponderance of women in cervical dystonia, men have a significantly earlier mean age of onset than women.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Meta-analysis of mean age of onset of cervical dystonia in men and women: meta-analysis of the mean age of onset by sex in our cohort of 278 cervical dystonia patients and 3 other studies which reported mean age of onset of cervical dystonia for both sexes (1618). This analysis demonstrated a significantly earlier mean age of onset for men than women. The mean age of onset of cervical dystonia was significantly earlier by 4.30 years (95% CI; 1.80–6.80) in men than women (p = 0.0008).

Discussion

Age-Related Sexual Dimorphism in Disease Penetrance in AOIFD

The male:female sex ratio (proportion of men) in AOIFD decreases with increasing mean age at onset; this association is highly significant and mean age of onset accounts for almost 60% of the variance in the proportion of men. AOIFD with onset below the age of 40 years affects predominantly men in focal hand dystonia and musician’s dystonia. After 40–45 years of age, the AOIFD phenotypes predominantly affect women and, importantly, with increasing age there is a steady linear decrease in the proportion of men affected (Figure 1). The cervical dystonia phenotype is of particular interest because, although overall it affects women 1.5–2.0 times more frequently than men, men have a mean age of onset approximately 4 years earlier than women (Figure 3).

Age-Related Sexual Dimorphism in Temporal Discrimination in Unaffected Relatives

In both unaffected first-degree relatives and healthy control participants (15), temporal discrimination shows a significant effect of sex, age, and an interaction of sex and age. Female relatives and female control participants under the age of 40 years have faster temporal discrimination than age-matched male relatives and controls. Overall temporal discrimination worsens as a function of age but this is more pronounced in women than men; the TDT in women deteriorates two times faster than in men. Sexual dimorphism of temporal discrimination is thus seen both in unaffected relatives (Figure 2) and in healthy participants (15). However, the effect of age on the TDT of unaffected relatives was more marked than in healthy participants, worsening by 0.34 ms/year more in relatives than controls. We postulate that this increased worsening with age in unaffected relatives indicates an effect of non-manifesting gene carriage on the TDT endophenotype; approximately half of these unaffected first-degree relatives are non-manifesting carriers of susceptibility genes for adult-onset dystonia (13, 14).

Age-Related Sexual Dimorphism in Ambient GABA

GABA levels, measured by magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS), in the frontal lobes in healthy participants show age-related sexual dimorphism (21). Under 40 years of age women have higher GABA levels than men. However, with increasing age, women show more marked progressive reduction in frontal lobe GABA than men, so that after 40 years of age men have increasingly higher frontal lobe GABA than women.

Deficient inhibitory GABAergic mechanisms are considered to be important in the pathogenesis of dystonia. Cortical and subcortical GABA levels are reduced contralateral to the affected hand in patients with focal hand dystonia (22). Alterations in inhibitory circuits in dystonia have been noted at spinal cord, brainstem, and cortical levels (23). Transcranial magnetic stimulation studies have shown impaired intra-cortical inhibition in both hemispheres in focal hand dystonia, even though symptoms were present on only one side of the body (24). Deficient inhibition may be responsible for excessive and overflow movements seen in AOIFD (24, 25).

Age, Cognition, and GABA

Normal aging is associated with a decline in aspects of cognitive function, including slowing in performance of visual orientation discrimination (26), visual face matching (27), three-dimensional shape determination (28), and motion direction detection (29). Even in young healthy individuals, variation in cortical GABA levels has been found to relate to a number of psychophysical measures, including tactile discrimination thresholds (30) and luminance grating orientation (31), with poorer performance associated with regionally reduced GABA levels. The ability to respond to a visual target speedily in the presence of a visual distraction has been linked to frontal lobe GABA measured by MRS (32). Reduced GABAergic inhibition, either age-related or due to physiological variation, results in blunting of stimulus–response tuning specificity and task performance. In animals, degradation in these visual discriminatory abilities, associated with a lower signal to noise ratio in the striate cortex, has been linked to a reduction (by 45–60%) of GABA immuno-reactive neurones in the older animal (33, 34). Age-related reduction in GABA has been noted in the inferior colliculus (35), and the hippocampus in rats (36).

Linking Clinical and Laboratory Observations

We postulate that the age-related sexual dimorphism in (a) disease penetrance in AOIFD, (b) temporal discrimination, and (c) ambient cerebral GABA are linked and illuminate pathogenic mechanisms in AOIFD. The underlying hypothesis is that the efficiency of temporal discrimination is a measure of effective temporal tuning in the basal ganglia/superior colliculus, and relates to GABAergic inhibitory mechanisms. Experimental work in animals indicates that effective GABAA and GABAB inhibitory mechanisms are necessary for the sharp onset and offset of responses to visual stimuli in the superficial laminae of the superior colliculus (3739). While GABA may be measured in large cortical structures by MRS, there is no method, as yet, to measure ambient or synaptic GABA in a structure as small as the superior colliculus in man in vivo. Thus, the hypothesis that faster temporal discrimination reflects more effective GABAergic inhibition in man remains likely, but unproven. The clinical relevance to adult-onset focal dystonia is that age-related sexual dimorphism in ambient GABA (causing the observed sexual dimorphism in temporal discrimination), results in the age-related sexual dimorphism in AOIFD disease penetrance.

Arising from these observations, we hypothesize that, in a person with a genetic susceptibility to AOIFD, penetrance varies according to gender and age (Figure 4): (i) under 40 years of age, women have faster temporal discrimination than men, have more ambient GABA and are resistant to disease penetrance; thus, the observed male predominance in focal hand dystonia and musician’s dystonia. (ii) The increased age-related rate of deterioration in temporal discrimination in women reflects faster reducing ambient GABA levels in women than men. After 40–45 years, women have increasingly worse temporal discrimination and increasingly lower ambient GABA than men. Older women are thus more susceptible to disease penetrance and thus the observed progressively increasing age-related female predominance in adult-onset dystonia phenotypes presenting after the age of 40 years (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Graphic illustrating the relationship between age-related sexual dimorphism in temporal discrimination thresholds, mean age of onset and sex ratios in phenotypes of adult-onset isolated focal dystonia. Temporal discrimination (in milliseconds) is inverted on the left Y-axis so that worse discrimination is lower on the axis. The X-axis represents age in years (open arrow at the top of the figure). Each of the five phenotypes (musician’s dystonia, focal hand dystonia, cervical dystonia, laryngeal dystonia, and blepharospasm) are represented by filled bars, positioned at their mean age of onset, with the sex ratio for each phenotype illustrated by the colored inserts (pink for women: blue for men) (right Y-axis); the increasing female:male sex ratio with age is illustrated by the relative size of the filled bars. The regression lines (red for women; blue for men) from the analysis of the temporal discrimination thresholds (TDTs) in the unaffected first-degree relatives are drawn to illustrate the age-related sexual dimorphism of the TDT in relationship to the increasing female predominance with age of the phenotypes.

Author Contributions

Conception (MH); design (MH, RR, and SO); organization (JB, LW, IB, and EM); statistical analysis (CW, JB, and LW); execution (IB, LW, EM, JB, DH, TL, HM, FM, and RW); review and critique (RR, MH, and JB); manuscript preparation: writing the first draft (MH); review and critique (all authors).

Conflict of Interest Statement

John S. Butler, Ines M. Beiser, Laura Williams, Eavan McGovern, Dan G. Healy, Helena Moore, Richard Walsh, and Cathal Walsh have no disclosures. Michael Hutchinson: serves as associate editor of the Multiple Sclerosis Journal, has received speaker’s honoraria from Biogen Idec, Bayer-Schering, and Novartis and receives research grants from Dystonia Ireland, the Health Research Board of Ireland (CSA-2012-5), Foundation for Dystonia Research (Belgium), and the Irish Institute of Clinical Neuroscience. Tim Lynch reports receiving educational grants from Bayer Schering, Biogen Idec, Lundbeck, Medtronic; research grants from Irish Institute of Clinical Neuroscience, Mater College, PRTL1 funding. Speaker’s honoraria from Novartis. UCB Pharma, Teva, Merck Serono, and Biogen Idec. Sean O’Riordan reports receiving a speaker’s honararium from Abbvie. Richard B. Reilly is in receipt of research grants from Health Research Board Ireland: FP7-288914-VERVE, “VERVE: Vanquishing fear and apathy through E-inclusion: personalized and populated Realistic Virtual Environments for clinical, home, and mobile platforms” co-PI with Prof. C. O’Sullivan and Prof. F. Newell. Total project €4.6M Oct 2012–Oct 2014; HRB: “INCA: Inhaler device for objective analysis of medication adherence” with Prof. R. Costello, Beaumont Hospital. €120,987, Oct 2013–Oct 2016. €132,894 from Cochlear Ltd. for objective analysis of temporal and spectral processing in Cochlear Implant users. Fiona Molloy has received speaker’s honorarium from Allergan and Ipse.

Acknowledgments

We thank the patients, their relatives, and the control participants for giving their time in this study.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from: Dystonia Ireland, Health Research Board, Ireland (CSA-2012-5) and Foundation for Dystonia Research (Belgium).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fneur.2015.00258

Abbreviations

AOIFD, adult-onset isolated focal dystonia; TDT, temporal discrimination threshold.

References

1. Soland VL, Bhatia KP, Marsden CD. Sex prevalence of focal dystonias. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (1996) 60:204–5. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.60.2.204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Albanese A, Bhatia K, Bressman SB, Delong MR, Fahn S, Fung VS, et al. Phenomenology and classification of dystonia: a consensus update. Mov Disord (2013) 28:863–73. doi:10.1002/mds.25475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Waddy HM, Fletcher NA, Harding AE, Marsden CD. A genetic study of idiopathic focal dystonias. Ann Neurol (1991) 29:320–4. doi:10.1002/ana.410290315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Leube B, Kessler KR, Goecke T, Auburger G, Benecke R. Frequency of familial inheritance among 488 index patients with idiopathic focal dystonia and clinical variability in a large family. Mov Disord (1997) 12:1000–6. doi:10.1002/mds.870120625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Defazio G, Berardelli A, Hallett M. Do primary adult-onset focal dystonias share aetiological factors? Brain (2007) 130:1183–93. doi:10.1093/brain/awl355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Brancati F, Defazio G, Caputo V, Valente EM, Pizzuti A, Livrea P, et al. Novel Italian family supports clinical and genetic heterogeneity of primary adult-onset torsion dystonia. Mov Disord (2002) 17:392–7. doi:10.1002/mds.10077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. O’Riordan S, Raymond D, Lynch T, Saunders-Pullman R, Bressman SB, Daly L, et al. Age at onset as a factor in determining the phenotype of primary torsion dystonia. Neurology (2004) 63:1423–6. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000142035.26034.C2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Bradley D, Whelan R, Walsh R, Reilly RB, Hutchinson S, Molloy F, et al. Temporal discrimination threshold: VBM evidence for an endophenotype in adult-onset primary torsion dystonia. Brain (2009) 132:2327–35. doi:10.1093/brain/awp156

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Bradley D, Whelan R, Walsh R, O’Dwyer J, Reilly R, Hutchinson S, et al. Comparing endophenotypes in adult-onset primary torsion dystonia. Mov Disord (2010) 25:84–90. doi:10.1002/mds.22889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Bradley D, Whelan R, Kimmich O, O’Riordan S, Mulrooney N, Brady P, et al. Temporal discrimination thresholds in adult-onset primary torsion dystonia: an analysis by task type and by dystonia phenotype. J Neurol (2012) 259:77–82. doi:10.1007/s00415-011-6125-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Kimmich O, Bradley D, Whelan R, Mulrooney N, Reilly RB, Hutchinson S, et al. Sporadic adult onset primary torsion dystonia is a genetic disorder by the temporal discrimination test. Brain (2011) 134:2656–63. doi:10.1093/brain/awr194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Kimmich O, Molloy A, Whelan R, Williams L, Bradley D, Balsters J, et al. Temporal discrimination, a cervical dystonia endophenotype: penetrance and functional correlates. Mov Disord (2014) 29:804–11. doi:10.1002/mds.25822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Hutchinson M, Kimmich O, Molloy A, Whelan R, Molloy F, Lynch T, et al. The endophenotype and the phenotype: temporal discrimination and adult onset dystonia. Mov Disord (2013) 28:1766–74. doi:10.1002/mds.25676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Hutchinson M, Isa T, Molloy A, Kimmich O, Williams L, Molloy F, et al. Cervical dystonia: a disorder of the midbrain network for covert attentional orienting. Front Neurol (2014) 5:54. doi:10.3389/fneur.2014.00054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Williams LJ, Butler JS, Molloy A, McGovern E, Beiser I, Kimmich O, et al. Young women do it better: sexual dimorphism in temporal discrimination. Front Neurol (2015) 6:160. doi:10.3389/fneur.2015.00160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw (2010) 36:1–48. doi:10.18637/jss.v036.i03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Raudenbush SW. Analyzing effect sizes: random effects models. 2nd ed. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV, Valentine JC, editors. The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation (2009). 295 p.

Google Scholar

18. Warner T, Ben-Shlomo Y; Group ESoDiEEC. Sex-related influences on the frequency and age of onset of primary dystonia. Epidemiologic Study of Dystonia in Europe (ESDE) Collaborative Group. Neurology (1999) 53:1871–3. doi:10.1212/WNL.53.8.1871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Pekmezović T, Ivanović N, Svetel M, Nalić D, Smiljković T, Raicević R, et al. Prevalence of primary late-onset focal dystonia in the Belgrade population. Mov Disord (2003) 18:1389–92. doi:10.1002/mds.10615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Elia AE, Filippini G, Bentivoglio AR, Fasano A, Ialongo T, Albanese A. Onset and progression of primary torsion dystonia in sporadic and familial cases. Eur J Neurol (2006) 13:1083–8. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2006.01387.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Gao F, Edden RA, Li M, Puts NA, Wang G, Liu C, et al. Edited magnetic resonance spectroscopy detects an age-related decline in brain GABA levels. Neuroimage (2013) 78:75–82. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Levy LM, Hallett M. Impaired brain GABA in focal dystonia. Ann Neurol (2002) 51:93–101. doi:10.1002/ana.10073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Berardelli A, Rothwell JC, Hallett M, Thompson PD, Manfredi M, Marsden CD. The pathophysiology of primary dystonia. Brain (1998) 121:1195–212. doi:10.1093/brain/121.7.1195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Bütefisch CM, Boroojerdi B, Chen R, Battaglia F, Hallett M. Task-dependent intracortical inhibition is impaired in focal hand dystonia. Mov Disord (2005) 20:545–51. doi:10.1002/mds.20367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Hallett M. Neurophysiology of dystonia: the role of inhibition. Neurobiol Dis (2011) 42:177–84. doi:10.1016/j.nbd.2010.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Betts LR, Sekuler AB, Bennett PJ. The effects of aging on orientation discrimination. Vision Res (2007) 47:1769–80. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2007.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Wilson HR, Mei M, Habak C, Wilkinson F. Visual bandwidths for face orientation increase during healthy aging. Vision Res (2011) 51:160–4. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.10.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Norman JF, Norman HF, Pattison K, Taylor MJ, Goforth KE. Aging and the depth of binocular rivalry suppression. Psychol Aging (2007) 22:625–31. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.22.3.625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Betts LR, Taylor CP, Sekuler AB, Bennett PJ. Aging reduces center surround antagonism in visual motion processing. Neuron (2005) 45:361–6. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Puts NA, Edden RA, Evans CJ, McGlone F, McGonigle DJ. Regionally specific human GABA concentration correlates with tactile discrimination thresholds. J Neurosci (2011) 31:16556–60. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4489-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Edden RAE, Muthukumaraswamy SD, Freeman TCA, Singh KD. Orientation discrimination performance is predicted by GABA concentration and gamma oscillation frequency in human primary visual cortex. J Neurosci (2009) 29:15721–6. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4426-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Sumner P, Edden RA, Bompas A, Evans CJ, Singh KD. More GABA, less distraction: a neurochemical predictor of motor decision speed. Nat Neurosci (2010) 13:825–7. doi:10.1038/nn.2559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hua T, Li X, He L, Zhou Y, Wang Y, Leventhal AG. Functional degradation of visual cortical cells in old cats. Neurobiol Aging (2006) 27:155–62. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Hua T, Kao C, Sun Q, Li X, Zhou Y. Decreased proportion of GABA neurons accompanies age-related degradation of neuronal function in cat striate cortex. Brain Res Bull (2008) 75:119–25. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Ouda L, Syka J. Immunocytochemical profiles of inferior colliculus neurons in the rat and their changes with aging. Front Neural Circuits (2012) 6:68. doi:10.3389/fncir.2012.00068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Stanley EM, Fadel JR, Mott DD. Interneuron loss reduces dendritic inhibition and GABA release in hippocampus of aged rats. Neurobiol Aging (2012) 33:e1–13. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kaneda K, Phongphanphanee P, Katoh T, Isa K, Yanagawa Y, Obata K, et al. Regulation of burst activity through pre- and postsynaptic GABAB receptors in mouse superior colliculus. J Neurosci (2008) 28:816–27. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4666-07.2008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Kaneda K, Isa K, Yanagawa Y, Isa T. Nigral inhibition of GABAergic neurons in mouse superior colliculus. J Neurosci (2008) 28:11071–8. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3263-08.2008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Kaneda K, Isa T. GABAergic mechanisms for shaping transient visual responses in the mouse superior colliculus. Neuroscience (2013) 235:129–40. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.12.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: adult-onset isolated focal dystonia, sex ratio, temporal discrimination, sexual dimorphism, penetrance

Citation: Butler JS, Beiser IM, Williams L, McGovern E, Molloy F, Lynch T, Healy DG, Moore H, Walsh R, Reilly RB, O’Riordan S, Walsh C and Hutchinson M (2015) Age-Related Sexual Dimorphism in Temporal Discrimination and in Adult-Onset Dystonia Suggests GABAergic Mechanisms. Front. Neurol. 6:258. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00258

Received: 07 October 2015; Accepted: 23 November 2015;
Published: 14 December 2015

Edited by:

Antonio Pisani, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Italy

Reviewed by:

Graziella Madeo, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Italy
Pedro Ribeiro, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil

Copyright: © 2015 Butler, Beiser, Williams, McGovern, Molloy, Lynch, Healy, Moore, Walsh, Reilly, O’Riordan, Walsh and Hutchinson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Michael Hutchinson, mhutchin2@mac.com

John S. Butler, Ines M. Beiser, Laura Williams and Eavan McGovern have contributed equally to this work.

Present address: John S. Butler, School of Mathematical Sciences, Dublin Institute of Technology, Dublin, Ireland

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.