Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Neurol., 16 December 2016
Sec. Pediatric Neurology

Neural Signature of DCD: A Critical Review of MRI Neuroimaging Studies

  • 1URI Octogone-Lordat (EA 4156), Université Toulouse II Jean Jaurès, Toulouse, France
  • 2Toulouse NeuroImaging Center, INSERM, University of Toulouse Paul Sabatier, Toulouse, France
  • 3Children’s Hospital, Toulouse-Purpan University Hospital, Toulouse, France

The most common neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., developmental dyslexia (DD), autism, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)) have been the subject of numerous neuroimaging studies, leading to certain brain regions being identified as neural correlates of these conditions, referring to a neural signature of disorders. Developmental coordination disorder (DCD), however, remains one of the least understood and studied neurodevelopmental disorders. Given the acknowledged link between motor difficulties and brain features, it is surprising that so few research studies have systematically explored the brains of children with DCD. The aim of the present review was to ascertain whether it is currently possible to identify a neural signature for DCD, based on the 14 magnetic resonance imaging neuroimaging studies that have been conducted in DCD to date. Our results indicate that several brain areas are unquestionably linked to DCD: cerebellum, basal ganglia, parietal lobe, and parts of the frontal lobe (medial orbitofrontal cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex). However, research has been too sparse and studies have suffered from several limitations that constitute a serious obstacle to address the question of a well-established neural signature for DCD.

Introduction

Developmental coordination disorder (DCD) is a highly prevalent neurodevelopmental disorder (1.8–6% of school-aged children), with an early age of onset and persistence into adulthood (13). Although it is a heterogeneous disorder (4) with different phenotypes (5), it can be characterized by a persistent motor impairment that negatively affects daily living activities and/or academic achievement and cannot better be explained by intellectual disability or an underlying neurological condition (1). Posture, motor learning, and sensorimotor coordination are the main areas of functional difficulties experienced by children with DCD (6). The motor performances of children with DCD are slower, less accurate, and more variable than those of their peers. Characteristic signs include poor distal control and hand coordination, impaired balance, and difficulty in motor learning and motor imagery (5, 710), which, in turn, cause numerous functional difficulties [e.g., dressing, writing, using utensils, running, catching balls, and playing sports (11)].

No single cause has been identified, and its etiology appears to be multifactorial (12, 13). However, for many years, brain features have been assumed to constitute a valid explanation for all the symptoms in DCD, and several hypotheses have been formulated accordingly, some substantiated by behavioral studies.

It is, therefore, worth wondering whether it is currently possible to identify a neural signature for DCD, i.e., define the neuroanatomic basis of DCD (one or more brain areas or brain networks) in relationship with symptoms of disorder. For this purpose, we first reviewed studies that have made assumptions about specific brain areas linked to DCD, and secondly, all magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies realized in DCD population.

Neural Correlates of DCD: From Early Hypotheses to the First Behavioral Studies

There have been three critical stages in the search for a link between brain characteristics and the symptoms of DCD: (i) assumptions based on brain lesions (studies investigating the behavior of individuals with brain damage); (ii) brain hypotheses based on behavioral data (inferences drawn from neurological examinations about which neural circuitries or brain areas are involved); and (iii) neuroimaging studies, especially MRI. In this section, we deal with the first two stages.

Early Assumptions about the Brain

The clinical symptoms (difficulties in postural control, balance, coordination, motor planning, or learning) of an underlying brain impairment were first reported in the 1960s, leading to several different assumptions about the relevant brain features, including impaired brain dominance (14) and hemispheric disconnection (15). The presence of soft neurological signs (16) also suggested an atypical recruitment of the cerebellum, basal ganglia (BG), and parietal and frontal lobes (1719). For example, Luria (20) analyzed the cerebral organization of perception and action (as well as attention, memory, speech, and intellectual processes), and discussed the role of the BG, and parietal and frontal cortices in motor dysfunctions. Observing that lesions in these areas could generate motor deficits in adults, he extended these causal links to clumsy children. He, therefore, deduced that occipitoparietal lesions could generate a visuospatial deficit, damage to the BG, and premotor cortex could impact the sequencing of elementary movements, and frontal lesions could affect action control (planning and realization). Shortly afterward, Gubbay (18) reported that clumsiness could be due to damaged neurological functions affecting the execution of movements (pyramidal and extrapyramidal circuits) and motor praxis (central nervous system, cerebellum, and peripheral nervous system). Finally, noting that underdevelopment of the cerebellum in premature or injured children causes clumsiness and difficulties in motor control, as well as in the performance of visuomotor and graphomotor tasks (despite preserved verbal skills), Lesný (19) suggested that atypical cerebellar development is implicated in DCD.

Concurrently, with these first assumptions, several more global approaches were proposed. Although they do not concern any specific disorder, they can be used to address the full range of neurodevelopmental disorders and account for the high rates of comorbidity among them. To initiate debate in neurological research on learning disorders and given that neurodevelopmental disorders are typically non-specific (symptoms and syndromes), heterogeneous, and frequently co-occurring, some authors proposed the term atypical brain development (21). This term acknowledged the overlap between these disorders and emphasized the role of the brain in their etiology. It replaced the older concept of minimal brain dysfunction (22), which served broadly the same purpose. More recently, we have witnessed the advent of a third term, early symptomatic syndromes eliciting neurodevelopmental clinical examinations [ESSENCE (23)], which refers to the various impairments displayed by children, rather than providing a specific diagnosis. It provides descriptions of impairments in multiple areas of functioning (language, motor coordination, attention, social interaction, etc.) that are associated with many neurodevelopmental disorders and accounts of co-occurrence between them. We should also mention the term developmental brain dysfunction, recently introduced by Moreno-De-Luca et al. (24), which includes neurodevelopmental disorders ranging from severe (underpinned by severe brain dysfunctions; i.e., intellectual disability and ASD) to lighter (underpinned by minimal brain dysfunctions; e.g., DCD, ADHD, and DD). Finally, we should also briefly describe the neural system typography for learning difficulties (25, 26), which contrasts general learning disabilities secondary to an impairment of the declarative learning system (i.e., intellectual developmental disorder) with neurodevelopmental disorders secondary to an impairment of the procedural learning system (i.e., ADHD, SLI, DD, and DCD), suggesting that neurodevelopmental disorders have a common impairment of the corticostriatal and/or corticocerebellar circuits.

It is important to stress that none of the abovementioned assumptions, whether they concern a single neurodevelopmental disorder or encompass all such disorders, are anything more than suppositions. The next stage was, therefore, to conduct behavioral investigations with tasks known to involve the brain areas thought to be dysfunctional.

Neural Correlates Inferred from Neurological and Behavioral Examinations

Between 1990 and 2010, there was a huge increase in the number of studies using brain imaging techniques to explore the neural mechanisms behind motor learning. These established objective criteria for determining which tasks activate which brain areas. Consequently, over this 20-year period, researchers were able to infer hypotheses about the neural correlates of DCD from behavioral studies, thus significantly contributing to our knowledge about the neural bases of DCD. Zwicker et al. (27) and Bo and Lee (28) comprehensively reviewed the behavioral evidence for the neural correlates of DCD, exploring dominant hypotheses and their relationships with motor skill production and learning in children with DCD, leading to the identification of three main brain bases of DCD: the cerebellum, BG, and parietal lobes.

The literature on the behavioral deficits associated with DCD strongly suggests that the cerebellum (and/or its network of connections) is a logical source of the dysfunction. An increasing number of studies have therefore been undertaken to demonstrate the role of the cerebellum in uncoordinated behaviors, clumsiness, poor coordination, and postural control (2931) – the hallmarks of DCD. Several behavioral paradigms have been designed specifically to highlight cerebellar dysfunction, including finger-to-nose touching, rapid alternating hand movements, and even motor adaptation tasks (32, 33). Given early assumptions about cerebellar dysfunctions in DCD, some studies were conducted with these specific tasks. These showed that children with DCD tend to perform more poorly than their peers on tasks such as finger-to-nose touching and rapid alternating hand movements, the most traditional tests of cerebellar function (16, 34). Difficulties with motor adaptation paradigms have also been demonstrated (3537). Children with DCD seem to be affected by visual distortions in drawing tasks (their performance does not change significantly across trials), but seem to be able to recognize errors (adjusting their internal map) when there is an abrupt visuomotor distortion (36, 37). More recently, Lejeune et al. (38) found slower performance and lower asymptotic performance in DCD on a visuomotor adaptation task. Similar findings were reported by Cantin et al. (35, 39) and Brookes et al. (40) on a prism adaptation test, where participants with DCD were less efficient than their peers, especially for complex tasks (39). The results of these behavioral studies, therefore, point to the involvement of either the cerebellum itself, the cerebellar loops, or both in the physiopathology of DCD.

Despite growing evidence implicating the cerebellum in DCD, it is highly unlikely to be the only neural correlate. The BG also play a key role in motor control, movement initiation, movement learning, and automatization (32, 41, 42), making them a plausible source of DCD symptomatology. Lundy-Ekman et al. (16) talked about “soft neurological signs of BG dysfunction” in DCD, although their role in this disorder was essentially unknown at that time. These authors concluded that there are two coexisting subtypes of clumsiness, resulting from either BG or cerebellar dysfunction. They attributed difficulties in the timing of muscle contractions to a faulty central timer mechanism resulting from soft neurological signs of cerebellar dysfunction, and impaired control of the amplitude of the isometric force pulse to soft neurological signs of BG dysfunction. Their hypothesis was recently refined by Gheysen et al. (43) and Biotteau et al. (44), who found that children with DCD performed more poorly on the sequencing of simple movements in a serial reaction time task [SRTT; (43)] and a finger-tapping task (44) – tasks known to involve the corticostriatal network (32). It should, however, be noted that when Wilson and McKenzie (45) and Lejeune et al. (46) applied SRTT paradigms, their results did not support the BG assumption [children with DCD performed similarly to typically developing (TD) children]. Results, therefore, appear more contrasted for the BG than for the cerebellum, although there are good grounds for considering them in the neurobiology of DCD.

The final brain structure thought to be implicated in DCD is the parietal lobe. Given its role in the processing of visuospatial information, action prediction and observation, executive functions, facial recognition, and motor imagery (29, 4750), it is legitimate to assume that it is involved in DCD. Wilson and McKenzie’s review (45) showed that children with DCD have poorer visuospatial processing than TD children. They also have difficulties with facial recognition (51), executive functions (52, 53), response inhibition (54, 55), and motor imagery (7, 10, 56, 57). All these processes involve the parietal lobes (and the prefrontal lobe for executive functions), and have, therefore, led researchers to conclude that this brain structure may be involved in DCD (7, 58, 59).

As outlined above, there is apparent evidence for the involvement of the cerebellum, BG, and parietal lobes in DCD. Some other brain areas have sporadically been identified, including the insula/claustrum, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and frontal lobe (27). Some behavioral results also point to a deficit in intra- and interhemispheric transfer in DCD (55, 60). For example, Sigmundsson (60) administered behavioral tests to children with a subtype of DCD with hand–eye coordination problems to test their right- and left-hand coordination. Results indicated specific motor difficulties in controlling their non-dominant (left) hand and coordinating the left and right parts of their body, suggesting a right-hemispheric insufficiency (lack of hemispheric specialization) with or without a dysfunctional corpus callosum.

Taken together, these findings raise the question of whether DCD has a brain signature. Many neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., autism, dyslexia, and ADHD) have been the subject of extensive neuroimaging research, leading to the identification of certain brain regions as neural signature of disorders. For example, left temporoparietal regions, the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), occipitotemporal cortex, and inferior parietal lobules have been identified as neural correlates of dyslexia (61), whereas functional abnormalities in right hemispheric frontal–BG networks (related to inhibition) and the DLPFC, parietal lobes, and cerebellar areas (related to attention) are typically found in ADHD (62). So in this context, is it currently possible to identify a cerebral signature for DCD on the basis of existing neuroimaging data?

Neuroimaging Studies

Our understanding of DCD has increased steadily over the past three decades and, as with all other neurodevelopmental disorders, it has prompted researchers to conduct neuroimaging studies. Over the past 5 years in particular, there has been a growing desire in the fields of neuroscience and psychology to understand how DCD influences neural development and functioning. Brain imaging studies have, therefore, been designed to provide evidence for the cerebral validity of DCD symptoms, based on the hypothesis that impaired perceptual motor functions are the result of atypical brain development. Nonetheless, given the acknowledged link between motor difficulties and brain features, surprisingly, few research studies have systematically explored the brains of children with DCD (Table 1). The review conducted by Peters et al. (63) highlighted this dearth of neuroimaging studies in DCD (only four MRI studies at the time). The available data were therefore extremely heterogeneous and suggested that multiple brain areas are involved in the neuropathophysiology of DCD. Two years later, Brown-Lum and Zwicker (64) listed seven MRI studies and two diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), but were unable to reach any firmer formal conclusions. Our own review, conducted 1 year later, brought five more neuroimaging studies to light. These 14 existing studies were clearly aimed at identifying the cerebral bases of the deficits observed in children with DCD, and it, therefore, seemed relevant to take a fresh look at the question of a neural correlate for DCD. Noted that we decided not to include the two MRI studies that assessed dysgraphia (65, 66), as their results, combined with those of Van Hoorn et al.’s (67) review (based on the limited data available on the neural correlate hypothesis in dysgraphia), point to the contribution of cortical areas (frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital) and the cerebellum.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Main characteristics of the included neuroimaging studies.

We identified nine fMRI studies, two structural MRI studies, and three DTI studies. All the children had motor problems in daily life and, for the most part, had been assessed with the MABC (one study did not clearly report the recruitment criteria). It should be noted that the children in the comparison groups were not always assessed on motor or cognitive criteria. Four studies clearly included comorbid DCD, but comorbidities for the DCD and comparison groups were not generally explored beyond the more usual ones (i.e., autism and ADHD, never DD or SLI). The studies included only small numbers of participants, varying from 7 to 21. All of them included children aged 7–12 years (ranges: 8–12, 8–10, and 7–10), except for three studies, where mean ages varied from 8 to 17 years. A variety of tasks were performed during the fMRI scans (visuomotor, finger sequence, tracking, motor timing, go/no-go, and trail-tracing).

The most striking result of this review is that no two studies observed the same differences. For this reason, we provide a detailed description of each individual study in the following section. As illustrated in Figure 1, few regions are consistent across studies, owing to several different factors that are summarized below, to avoid having to reiterate them in each individual study description.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Foci of atypical brain involvement reported from neuroimaging studies of children with DCD compared to TD children. Studies: circles 1–8: fMRI; triangles 9–10: sMRI; squares 11–13: DTI. Each number/color represented one study. [1] Querne et al. (68); [2] Kashiwagi et al. (69); [3] Zwicker et al. (70); [4] Zwicker et al. (71); [5] Debrabant et al. (73); [6] McLeod et al. (74); [7] Licari et al. (77); [8] Reynolds et al. (78); [9] Langevin et al. (76); [10] Caeyenberghs et al. (80); [11] Zwicker et al. (72); [12] Langevin et al. (75); [13] Debrabant et al. (79). Brain areas: ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; c., cortex; DLPCF, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; g., gyrus; lob., lobule; OFC, orbito frontal cortex; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex; RLIC, retrolenticular part of the internal capsule; SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus; TPJ, temporoparietal junction; t, tract.

First, part of the explanation may lie in the heterogeneity of the sample. DCD is heterogeneous in nature, but this heterogeneity may be increased by (1) the use of different MRI scanners (scanning site) with specific acquisition designs, (2) the administration of different tasks during the fMRI (go/no-go, finger tapping, tracing tasks, etc.), and (3) comorbidity. Regarding this last point, it is surprising to note that the vast majority of studies failed to explore DCD comorbidities other than the most common ones. While ADHD and ASD were excluded in some studies, DD and SLI were only excluded in one of the 14 studies (9), despite being frequently associated with DCD (9, 81, 82). A fourth source of heterogeneity is the use of different assessment batteries to select the children. Although the MABC is now generally used to assess motor functions (used in 13/14 studies), cutoff scores for inclusion in the DCD and TD groups can vary. Although the 15th percentile is used to determine the clinical status of children with DCD, researchers prefer to use the 5th percentile of the motor test score to ensure that the sample is best suited for answering the research question (83). Only four of the 14 studies respected this threshold (9, 73, 77, 79). All the other studies included children below the 15th or the 16th percentile. Finally, heterogeneity may be increased by a large age range. While most of the studies included children aged 7–12 years (ranges: 8–12, 8–10, and 7–10), in three studies (7476), mean ages varied between 8 and 17 years. It is, therefore, very difficult to directly compare the findings of these studies with those of previous ones.

Second, comparison with a control group was problematic in the majority of studies. It seems very important to ensure that the TD group has none of the signs listed in the clinical criteria for DCD (83), especially a motor performance score above the 15th percentile, but these do not always appear to have been verified. In some cases, the TD group did not even undergo a motor assessment (68, 69, 78). Additionally, we frequently found that there was no safety margin between the TD and DCD groups (e.g., DCD below the 15th percentile, TD above the 15th). Thus, fewer than half the studies (70, 71, 73, 77, 79) clearly used a motor inclusion criterion for TD children. In the remaining ones, those children who were not selected for the DCD group were included in the TD group. Additionally, the TD group was not systematically tested for IQ, or for comorbidities such as ADHD and ASD [see, for example, Reynold et al. (78)]. One study (9) did not even use control groups. These are all potential sources of bias, making comparisons difficult, if not impossible.

Third, some results need to be interpreted with the utmost caution, owing to methodological inconsistencies in inclusion. For example, the large age ranges mentioned above for McLeod et al. (74) and Langevin et al. (75, 76) are highly problematic for the intrinsic interpretation of findings. Even when analyses were performed using age as a covariate, the inclusion range was too great, given the rapid developmental maturation of the brain (84, 85). Other results also need to be interpreted with great care, owing to significant differences between the DCD and TD groups in age, sex distribution, IQ, handedness, or level of attention (71, 7476) – all factors known to influence the brain’s structure or functioning (8488). Furthermore, the DCD group was generally very small, making it extremely difficult to lend credence to the results (sample varying from 7 to 21 children with DCD, depending on the study; fewer than 10 in 6 studies, and fewer than 15 in 11 studies). Sometimes, even though the numbers of children differed considerably between the DCD and TD groups, tests were still conducted between the two [e.g., 7 DCD vs. 23 TD in McLeod et al. (74)]. All these factors can introduce bias into the data analysis and, by extension, undermine the authenticity of the results.

A final issue concerns the absence of methodological MRI standards. Some studies investigated the whole brain, while others were interested in a specific region. Statistical power varied from one study to another, and was sometimes uncorrected for multiple comparisons [despite 68 brain regions in Caeyenberghs et al. (80) and 28 in Langevin et al. (76)]. In some cases, the authors talked about differences even when the voxel size was very small [e.g., below 20 in Reynolds et al. and Debrabant et al. (73, 78)]. It should also be noted that studies often failed to find significant group differences for brain data and, therefore, focused on correlations between behavioral data.

Functional MRI Studies

Querne et al. (68)

Using fMRI during a go/no-go task performed by children with DCD and TD, these authors assessed the impact of DCD on effective connectivity applied to a putative model of inhibition. No difference in behavioral performances was found between the DCD and TD groups (even if DCD responses were slower and more variable than TD responses). However, structural equation modeling indicated that the coefficients of the paths from both the middle frontal cortex (MFC) and ACC to the inferior parietal cortex (IPC) increased in the children with DCD, especially for the left hemisphere. Moreover, in the children with DCD, coefficients of the paths between the striatum and parietal cortex decreased in the right hemisphere. The authors suggested that DCD can be characterized by abnormal brain hemispheric specialization during development (executive functions tended to recruit a left-lateralized network in DCD and a right-lateralized network in TD). They also hypothesized that children with DCD compensate for their poor efficiency by more actively engaging the ACC, thus maintaining a good level of inhibition.

Kashiwagi et al. (69)

Using fMRI during a visuomotor task (visually guided tracking task), this study was designed to identify the brain mechanisms underlying clumsiness in children with DCD and TD. The behavioral performances of children with DCD were significantly less accurate than those of TD. Between-group differences showed that there was less brain activity in the superior and inferior parietal lobes, left posterior parietal cortex (PPC), and left postcentral gyrus in the children with DCD than in the TD children. The authors suggested a link between the left PPC and clumsiness in DCD.

Zwicker et al. (70)

Using fMRI during a fine motor trail-tracing task, the authors measured brain activation patterns in children with DCD. Similar levels of behavioral performance were noted. Between-group differences were found in patterns of brain activity. The children with DCD had greater activation in nine areas, essentially in the right hemisphere: middle frontal gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, lingual gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus, posterior cingulate gyrus, precentral gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, cerebellum (Lobule VI), and left inferior parietal lobule. They had less activation in five brain regions: left precuneus, superior frontal gyrus, IFG, postcentral gyrus, and right superior temporal gyrus/insula.

Zwicker et al. (71)

Using fMRI, the authors mapped brain activity associated with skilled motor practice of a trail-tracing task (5 days’ practice, two scans: days 1 and 5) in children with DCD [same population sample as in Zwicker et al. (70)]. The children with DCD did not show any improvement in motor accuracy following skilled practice. Between-group differences showed that nine brain areas were less activated in the children with DCD at both early practice and retention: the bilateral inferior parietal lobules, right lingual gyrus, right middle frontal gyrus, left fusiform gyrus, right cerebellum (Crus I), left cerebellum (Lobule VI), and left cerebellum (Lobule IX).

Debrabant et al. (73)

Using fMRI during a visuomotor reaction time task (sequences of visual stimuli with predictive or unpredictive interstimulus intervals, ISIs), the authors investigated the neural correlates of motor timing in DCD. The children with DCD reacted more quickly to predictive ISIs than TD children did, and there was no difference in their brain activation between the two conditions (TD exhibited greater activation in the right DLPFC and right IFG in response to unpredictive vs. predictive ISIs). Between-group differences in the predictive condition showed that the children with DCD activated their DLPFC, temporoparietal junction (TPJ), and left posterior cerebellum (Crus I) less. The authors concluded that the children with DCD needed to perform extra processing, owing to impaired predictive encoding.

McLeod et al. (74)

Using resting-state fMRI, the authors investigated the functional connections of the motor network in children with DCD and/or ADHD and predicted that, compared with TD, the children with DCD and/or ADHD would exhibit altered functional connectivity (FC) between the primary motor cortex (M1) and brain regions involved in motor functioning and sensorimotor processing. We excluded results for the ADHD group, given that this pathology was not the subject of our review. Between-group differences are, therefore, only shown for the DCD, DCD + ADHD, and TD groups [for details of the ADHD group, see McLeod et al (74)]. Compared with TD, the DCD group demonstrated decreased FC with M1 for the bilateral IFG, right frontal operculum cortex, right supramarginal gyrus, bilateral insular cortices and superior temporal gyri, bilateral caudate and right nucleus accumbens, pallidum, and putamen. The DCD + ADHD group exhibited lower FC with M1 for the right motor cortex, left supramarginal gyrus, bilateral postcentral gyri, left putamen, left pallidum, and left amygdala, and greater FC for the left frontopolar cortex and lingual gyrus. The children with DCD + ADHD exhibited greater FC between M1 and the bilateral caudate nuclei and anterior superior temporal gyri, left premotor cortex, postcentral gyri and frontopolar cortex, and right IFG and parietal operculum cortex. The authors concluded that the decreased FC between M1 and the striatum and angular gyrus (observed in all the groups) indicated that these brain regions are common neurophysiological substrates underlying both DCD and ADHD. They also suggested that the co-occurrence of neurodevelopmental disorders may have a distinct impact on FC (unique alterations in FC between M1 and sensory networks for these children).

Licari et al. (77)

Using fMRI during two tasks (finger sequencing and hand clenching), the authors investigated cortical activation patterns contributing to increased motor overflow in children with DCD. Behavioral results showed that the children with DCD performed more poorly than TD on both tasks. Analysis failed to reveal between-group differences in the hand-clenching condition. However, compared with TD, the children with DCD activated their left superior frontal gyrus (BA 9) and left IFG (BA 44), and right postcentral gyrus (BA 3) more in the finger-sequencing task.

Reynolds et al. (78)

Cortical activations during the performance of an imitative finger-sequencing task (observation, action execution, and action imitation) were observed through the use of fMRI. The authors hypothesized a deficit in the mirror neuron system (MNS). A between-group analysis revealed differences in six areas (right IFG, precentral gyrus, precuneus, left precentral gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, and posterior cingulate), but only in the observation condition. Region of interest analysis revealed less activation in the pars opercularis in the DCD group in the imitation condition. The authors concluded that the results supported a possible MNS dysfunction.

Biotteau et al. (9)

Using fMRI during a finger-tapping task administered to children with DCD with or without comorbidity, the authors assessed the impact of comorbidity in DCD on brain functioning. No difference was found in behavioral performances between DCD and the other groups. However, compared with the DD group, results revealed greater activity in the bilateral cingulate gyrus (BAs 24 and 31), bilateral sensorimotor cortex (BAs 3 and 4), bilateral premotor cortex (BA 6), bilateral temporoparietal cortex, right insula (BA 13), left thalamus and left globus pallidus, right caudate and right claustrum, and right anterior and posterior cerebellum. The authors suggested that, compared with the DD and DD + DCD groups, the DCD group was characterized by a distinct pattern of functioning in the neural correlates recruited for procedural learning. The DD and comorbid groups were very close, whereas the DCD group presented specific characteristics, raising the issue of the nature of motor problems in DD.

Structural MRI Studies

Langevin et al. (76)

Using structural MRI, the authors examined whether comorbid motor and attention problems influence cortical thickness in children. They compared the brain patterns of four groups. Between-group differences in cortical thickness were large, but in summary the children with DCD + ADHD had a greater overall decrease in cortical thickness than the children with DCD or ADHD alone (concentrated in the frontal, parietal, and temporal lobes). The children with DCD alone only differed from TD on the right medial orbitofrontal cortex (MOC), which was thinner in DCD. The authors concluded that the DCD + ADHD co-occurrence is associated with a distinct overall pattern of decreased regional cortical thickness, highlighting the unique neurobiology of comorbid neurodevelopmental disorders.

Caeyenberghs et al. (80)

Using structural MRI, this study aimed to address the question of whether abnormal connectivity in DCD overlaps with that seen in ASD or comorbid DCD + ASD. The authors investigated differences in the global and regional topological properties of structural brain networks (small-world networks between 68 brain regions, based on cortical thickness) in 53 children divided in four groups. Between-group differences between ASD (with or without DCD) and other groups were large, but are not provided here (see Caeyenberghs et al. (80) for details). The DCD group exhibited only one difference from the TD group, in the right lateral orbitofrontal cortex (higher clustering coefficient). Regarding comorbidity, compared with the DCD group, the children with DCD + ASD had higher nodal clustering coefficients in the left lingual gyrus, pars opercularis of the left IFG, left temporal pole, right entorhinal cortex, and right MOC. By contrast, the DCD + ASD group had lower clustering coefficients in the right posterior cingulate gyrus, right postcentral gyrus, and left transverse temporal gyrus. The authors concluded that the DCD, ASD, and TD groups had prominent small-world properties in their cortical thickness networks, even if the overall organization of networks in the children with DCD was relatively intact, as shown by the absence of group effects on overall network parameters (global network values close to those of TD).

DTI Studies

Zwicker et al. (72)

The authors measured diffusivity and fractional anisotropy (FA) in the corticospinal tract, posterior thalamic radiation, and superior and middle cerebellar peduncles in DCD. Mean diffusivity of the corticospinal tract and posterior thalamic radiation was lower in DCD than in TD. By contrast, FA in these tracts and diffusion parameters in the cerebellar pathways did not differ between groups. The authors concluded that reduced axial diffusivity in motor and sensory tracts may be implicated in DCD.

Langevin et al. (75)

Using DTI in children with DCD, the authors examined the three major white-matter tracts involved in attention and motor processes [corpus callosum, cingulum, and superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF)]. They explored associations between attention/executive and motor measures, and white-matter microstructure. Results for the ADHD group are not mentioned here (see Langevin et al. (75) for details). The authors found microstructural abnormalities in the white-matter connections underlying the primary and somatosensory motor cortices that were unique to the DCD group, with FA reductions in regions of the corpus callosum underlying parietal brain regions (superior/posterior parietal cortex, corpus callosum), as well as the left SLF. Children with comorbid DCD + ADHD exhibited alterations found in children with DCD or ADHD only (two distinct callosal regions). The authors, therefore, suggested that ADHD and DCD share a common basis in the callosal structure (reflecting a neurobiological basis for motor and attention disorders in children), with regionally and functionally distinct alterations.

Debrabant et al. (79)

Using DTI, the authors investigated whole-brain structural connectomics to identify abnormal microstructural properties of specific sensorimotor white-matter tracts in children with DCD. First, the children with DCD displayed a significant decrease in mean FA, together with an increase in mean radial diffusivity of the left retrolenticular limb of the internal capsule. Second, DCD-related FA reductions in the left retrolenticular limb of the internal capsule were associated with poor visuomotor tracing outcomes. Third, nodal efficiency in the cerebellum (Lobule VI) and right parietal superior gyrus was found to be a significant predictor of DCD.

Discussion

Neuroimaging studies have been conducted with a view to supporting the most promising hypotheses formulated on the basis of neuropsychological and behavioral observations in children with DCD. These point to impaired activation in the cerebellum, parietal lobes, and BG. In the following subsections, we discuss the available neuroimaging evidence.

Cerebellum

The cerebellum was targeted in these studies because of its role in movement, balance, coordination, learning, and automatization. Four studies (70, 71, 73, 79) observed specific features within this area. Lobule VI was highlighted in three of them (70, 71, 79), and Debrabant et al. (79) concluded that it is a significant predictor for DCD. Crus I was also mentioned by Zwicker et al. (71) and Debrabant et al. (73), as well as Lobule IX (71). Our review, therefore, highlights a particular role of Lobule VI in the neuropathology of DCD. However, there is converging evidence in the literature that the cerebellum is a common source of neuropathology in children with neurodevelopmental disorders (89). Thus, although DCD is probably linked to cerebellar dysfunction, it would be difficult to use the latter as a specific signature of DCD, given the evidence of cerebellar involvement in all other neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g., ADHD, ASD, DD).

Basal Ganglia

On account of their primary role in movement initiation, planning, motor control, learning, and automatization, the BGs are viewed as being potentially implicated in DCD. Several neuroimaging studies seem to corroborate this hypothesis (68, 72, 74, 79). Querne et al. (68) found atypical (decreased) coefficients for the paths between the striatum and parietal cortex in the right hemisphere. McLeod et al. (74) showed atypical recruitment of the caudate, nucleus accumbens, pallidum, and putamen for both DCD and DCD + ADHD. These authors even concluded that the striatum could be a common neurophysiological substrate of DCD and ADHD. Finally, using DTI techniques, Debrabant et al. (79) and Zwicker et al. (72) found particularities that mainly involved the thalamus: the corticospinal tract and posterior thalamic for Zwicker et al. (72), and the retrolenticular limb of the internal capsule for Debrabant et al. (79). Once again, however, it would be presumptuous to regard the BG as a unique brain characteristic of DCD, because of their suspected or proven involvement in a large number of neurodevelopmental disorders (9094).

Parietal Lobe

The parietal lobe is a promising structure in the search for neural correlates in DCD. Eleven of the 14 neuroimaging studies mentioned its involvement in DCD. Differences were found in the IPC (6871), superior parietal cortex (69, 79), PPC (69), postcentral gyrus (69, 70, 74, 77), supramarginal gyrus (70, 74), TPJ (73), and parietal operculum cortex (74). Additionally, Querne et al. (68) noted decreased coefficients for the paths between the striatum and parietal cortex in the right hemisphere in children with DCD. These findings were consistent with those of McLeod et al. (74) who found decreased FC between M1 and the striatum and angular gyrus in the DCD group. It should, however, be noted that the authors concluded that these brain regions are common neurophysiological substrates of both DCD and ADHD, as this specific decrease was observed in all three groups. Once again, therefore, abnormalities in this brain region are probably not peculiar to DCD and could be common to other neurodevelopmental disorders.

Other Areas

Limbic Lobe

There are few hypotheses of limbic lobe involvement in the physiopathology of DCD, even though several neuroimaging studies have found evidence of such involvement (68, 70, 74, 78). In particular, fMRI studies have reported differences in the ACC (68) and posterior cingulate cortex (70, 78), parahippocampal gyrus (70), insular cortices and insula (70, 74), and left amygdala (74). However, as suggested by Querne et al. (68), children with DCD may compensate for their poor efficiency by more actively engaging the cingulate cortex, thereby maintaining a good level of inhibition. This assumption can be applied to the other limbic areas mentioned.

Frontal Lobe

Frontal lobe dysfunction has been found in almost all MRI studies, but it is very difficult to draw the right conclusions about its involvement in DCD, owing to its unspecific nature. However, several features in specific frontal areas may appear to be potential neural correlate of DCD. First, for DCD alone compared with TD, Langevin et al. (76) noted only one difference – in the right MOC (thinner in DCD). Exactly the same result was reported by Caeyenberghs et al. (80). Using an innovative design (separate structural correlation networks based on cortical thickness), they highlighted the specific involvement of the right orbitofrontal cortex in DCD. Connectivity of the orbitofrontal cortex mainly concerned limbic areas (in particular, the insular cortex, parahippocampal regions, and amygdala, which we just mentioned as atypical in DCD) and the striatum. The MOC is also connected to the DLPFC, which was found to be implicated in the physiopathology of DCD by Debrabant et al. (73), especially, the right DLPFC. Given its role in executive functions and cognitive processes, the DLPFC seems to be an excellent signature of DCD. It should be noted that the DLPFC is connected to a variety of subcortical structures, which once again include the thalamus and BG (specifically the caudate nucleus). All in all, the DLPFC, MOC, and their connections to limbic and subcortical structures, could be viewed as solid cerebral correlates of DCD.

Lingual Gyrus

Differences have been noted in the lingual gyrus (70, 71, 74). This brain region is assumed to be engaged by low-level visual processes (95, 96) and to play an important role in vision, especially related to letters. The recent review by Richlan et al. (97) identified it as a specific brain characteristic for dyslexia. Comorbidity with dyslexia, which has not so far been explored in DCD neuroimaging studies, despite its frequency (98), is one possible (but speculative) explanation. The same question applies to other areas, such as the left fusiform gyrus mentioned by Zwicker et al. (71), temporal areas where differences between DCD and TD have been found in many studies (71, 74, 75, 78), and even Lobule VI (70, 71, 79), which has been designated as linked to reading-related activity (99). This raises the underlying question of how to assess all possible comorbidities when studies are performed with DCD children in order to determine what is specifically related to DCD and what is related to another comorbid disorder.

Abnormal Brain Hemispheric Specialization

Two studies support the notion of abnormalities in cerebral hemispheres, congruent with the older hemispheric disconnection approach (15). Querne et al. (68) evoked abnormal brain hemispheric specialization during development. These authors found that executive functions tended to recruit a left-lateralized network in DCD and a right-lateralized network in TD. Alternative findings suggest that DCD may reflect a hemispheric disconnection syndrome. In particular, Langevin et al. (75) observed reduced connectivity between the parietal region of the corpus callosum and parietal areas, compared with TD.

Conclusion

The findings described above open up promising avenues in the quest for a possible cerebral signature for DCD.

First, the cerebellum and BG are unquestionably linked to DCD. But there is converging evidence that both are involved in several neurodevelopmental disorders and are probably indicative of the nature of neurodevelopmental disorders in general, rather than the unique and intrinsic nature of DCD. However, the cerebellum and BG are nowadays most often regarded as a single block (in a relatively general and imprecise manner). If they had to be investigated in greater detail, links might probably emerge between specific disorders and specific cerebellar or BG areas. Therefore, their degree of involvement and the precise role they play in the neuropathology of neurodevelopmental disorders have yet to be clearly defined.

Second, similar doubts can be expressed about the parietal lobe, even if its specific implication in DCD is more probable.

Third, two regions (MOC and DLPFC) closely connected to the cerebellum and BG, and also closely interconnected, could constitute a good signature of DCD.

Fourth and last, the limbic lobe, especially the cingulate cortex and parahippocampal gyrus, would appear to be viewed as compensatory brain mechanisms in DCD, more actively engaged to ensure that a good level of achievement is maintained.

Altogether, can the results of these 14 neuroimaging studies really be said to point to a neural signature for DCD? If we include the cerebellum, BG, parietal lobe, MOC, and DPFC, the answer is probably yes, but uncertain. Indeed, during our review of these neuroimaging studies, it became clear that the current literature struggles to find a consistent picture on neural correlates linked to DCD (Table 1; Figure 1), which prevents us to reach firm conclusions on a cerebral signature of DCD. There are many possible explanations for this.

First, there have been too few MRI studies to date, and there has been no uniformization of the tasks performed during fMRI. The use of different designs (task, paradigm, and duration), especially in a pediatric population, is a major limitation, making comparisons impossible and findings hard to interpret when it comes to determining what is related to the neurophysiological basis of DCD, to task difficulty or to compensatory mechanisms.

Second, samples have often been very small, and above all, non-homogeneous. They differed across our 14 studies for many reasons. Regarding inclusion ages, studies adhered closely to the minimum age for DCD diagnosis, only including participants aged 5 years or older, in accordance with the European Academy for Childhood Disability [EACD (100)]. However, the age range extended from 7 to 17 years which, given the development of the brain across childhood, adolescence, and puberty (85, 101), prevented researchers from extending the significance of their findings. Inclusion criteria were also a real problem. While the use of MABC (or MABC-2) received a broad consensus (13/14 studies used it), the cutoff scores used to assess children with DCD were not homogeneous. Concerning the inclusion criteria for TD, motor and/or cognitive abilities were not systematically controlled for, and even where this was done, a safety margin was not always created between the TD and DCD groups for motor skills.

Third, for the most part, the authors chose not to look for comorbidities other than the more usual ones (ASD or ADHD). Multicomorbidities in neurodevelopmental disorders are, however, extremely frequent (102), resulting in the co-occurence of two and even three or more disorders, with a high level of co-occurrence with SLI or DD for DCD (44, 81, 82). More homogeneous subtypes of DCD, excluding not only ASD and ADHD but also DD and SLI, are required, in order to distinguish the brain characteristics of DCD alone from those of other comorbid disorders. Additionally, given the absence of homogeneity in DCD, it would appear appropriate to explore the heterogeneous presentation of the children who are studied (where do they experience difficulty: in motor learning, and/or motor skill acquisition, and/or gross motor skills, and/or fine motor skills?). This has not been the case up to now, even though it could have enhanced the analysis and comprehension of the findings.

Fourth and last, can we really talk about differences when cluster sizes are below 50 or when results are uncorrected for multiple comparisons? More rigorous analysis of imaging data will be required to ensure the validity of such differences.

Thus, to confirm our conclusions about the involvement of the cerebellum, BG, parietal lobe, MOC, and DPFC in DCD, new neuroimaging studies need to be designed. Based on this review, we strongly encourage researchers:

1. to include groups more homogeneous with strict inclusion criteria for DCD and TD, especially concerning cognitive and motor tests (safety margin between the two groups is for example required), featuring larger samples;

2. to take comorbidities into account and not only autism or ADHD but also SLI or DD;

3. to pay careful attention to rigorous analysis of imaging data;

4. to use preferentially more advanced neuroimaging techniques as DTI and resting state or even to realize longitudinal structural neuroimaging studies in order to isolate cortical alterations inherent to DCD or reflecting environmental influence (e.g., poor motor experience);

5. finally, to combine several neuroimaging markers as it has already done in other pathologies (103).

Even if we know the difficulties encountered by such recommendations, it seems necessary in order to allow for the comparison of results from many studies and providing an opportunity to share imagery data.

Author Contributions

MBi, J-MA, and YC all contributed to the elaboration of the ideas developed in the manuscript. MBi and J-MA wrote the first draft of the manuscript, table, and figure. YC completed parts of the manuscript, revised, and rewrote sections of the initial article. JT and MBl made critical amendments and essential feedback to this second version of the manuscript. PP provided critical feedback to figure and table, reorganized, and rewrote parts of the table. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Funding

This study was supported by a Grant (ANR-13-APPR-0010-01) from The French National Research Agency.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5). 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing (2013).

Google Scholar

2. Lingam R, Hunt L, Golding J, Jongmans M, Emond A. Prevalence of developmental coordination disorder using the DSM-IV at 7 years of age: a UK population-based study. Pediatrics (2009) 123(4):e693–700. doi:10.1542/peds.2008-1770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Kirby A, Williams N, Thomas M, Hill EL. Self-reported mood, general health, wellbeing and employment status in adults with suspected DCD. Res Dev Disabil (2013) 34(4):1357–64. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Green D, Chambers ME, Sugden DA. Does subtype of developmental coordination disorder count: is there a differential effect on outcome following intervention? Hum Mov Sci (2008) 27(2):363–82. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2008.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Gomez A, Piazza M, Jobert A, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Dehaene S, Huron C. Mathematical difficulties in developmental coordination disorder: symbolic and nonsymbolic number processing. Res Dev Disabil (2015) 4(3–44):167–78. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2015.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Geuze RH. Postural control in children with developmental coordination disorder. Neural Plast (2005) 12(2–3):183–96. doi:10.1155/NP.2005.183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Adams ILJ, Lust JM, Wilson PH, Steenbergen B. Compromised motor control in children with DCD: a deficit in the internal model? A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2014) 47:225–44. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.08.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Biotteau M, Chaix Y, Albaret J-M. What do we really know about motor learning in children with developmental coordination disorder? Curr Dev Disord Rep (2016) 3(2):152–60. doi:10.1007/s40474-016-0084-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Biotteau M, Péran P, Vayssière N, Tallet J, Albaret J-M, Chaix Y. Neural changes associated to procedural learning and automatization process in developmental coordination disorder and/or developmental dyslexia. Eur J Paediatr Neurol (2016). doi:10.1016/j.ejpn.2016.07.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Wilson PH, Ruddock S, Smits-Engelsman B, Polatajko H, Blank R. Understanding performance deficits in developmental coordination disorder: a meta-analysis of recent research. Dev Med Child Neurol (2013) 55(3):217–28. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04436.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Summers J, Larkin D, Dewey D. Activities of daily living in children with developmental coordination disorder: dressing, personal hygiene, and eating skills. Hum Mov Sci (2008) 27(2):215–29. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2008.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Cairney J. Developmental coordination disorder and its consequences: an introduction to the problem. In: Cairney J, editor. Developmental Coordination Disorder and Its Consequences. London: University of Toronto Press (2015). p. 5–30.

Google Scholar

13. Mosca SJ, Langevin LM, Dewey D, Innes AM, Lionel AC, Marshall CC, et al. Copy-number variations are enriched for neurodevelopmental genes in children with developmental coordination disorder. J Med Genet (2016) 53:812–19. doi:10.1136/jmedgenet-2016-103818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Reuben RN, Bakwin H. Developmental clumsiness. Pediatr Clin North Am (1993) 15(3):601–10. doi:10.1016/S0031-3955(16)32163-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Geschwind N. The apraxias. Neural mechanisms of disorders of learned movement. Am Sci (1975) 63:188–95.

Google Scholar

16. Lundy-Ekman L, Ivry R, Keele S, Woollacott M. Timing and force control deficits in clumsy children. J Cogn Neurosci (1991) 3:367–76. doi:10.1162/jocn.1991.3.4.367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Cermak SA. Developmental dyspraxia. In: Roy E, editor. Neuropsychological Studies of Apraxia and Related Disorders. Amsterdam: North-Holland (1985). p. 225–48.

Google Scholar

18. Gubbay SS. The Clumsy Child: A Study of Developmental Apraxia and Agnosic Ataxia. London: Saunders (1975).

Google Scholar

19. Lesný IA. Developmental dyspraxia-dysgnosia as a cause of congenital children’s clumsiness. Brain Dev (1980) 2:69–71. doi:10.1016/S0387-7604(80)80009-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Luria AR. The Working Brain. New York, NY: Basic Books (1973).

Google Scholar

21. Gilger JW, Kaplan BJ. Atypical brain development: a conceptual framework for understanding developmental learning disabilities. Dev Neuropsychol (2001) 20(2):465–81. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn2002_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Clements SD. Minimal Brain Dysfunction in Children – Terminology and Identification. Washington, DC: US Public Health Service Publication (1966).

Google Scholar

23. Gillberg C. The ESSENCE in child psychiatry: early symptomatic syndromes eliciting neurodevelopmental clinical examinations. Res Dev Disabil (2010) 31(6):1543–51. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Moreno-De-Luca A, Myers SM, Challman TD, Moreno-De-Luca D, Evans DW, Ledbetter DH. Developmental brain dysfunction: revival and expansion of old concepts based on new genetic evidence. Lancet Neurol (2013) 12(4):406–14. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70011-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ. Procedural learning difficulties: reuniting the developmental disorders? Trends Neurosci (2007) 30(4):135–41. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2007.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ. Dyslexia, dysgraphia, procedural learning and the cerebellum. Cortex (2011) 47:117–27. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.08.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Zwicker JG, Missiuna C, Boyd LA. Neural correlates of developmental coordination disorder: a review of hypotheses. J Child Neurol (2009) 24(10):1273–81. doi:10.1177/0883073809333537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Bo J, Lee C-M. Motor skill learning in children with developmental coordination disorder. Res Dev Disabil (2013) 34(6):2047–55. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Blakemore S-J, Sirigu A. Action prediction in the cerebellum and in the parietal lobe. Exp Brain Res (2003) 153(2):239–45. doi:10.1007/s00221-003-1597-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Ivry RB. Cerebellar involvement in clumsiness and other developmental disorders. Neural Plast (2003) 10:141–53. doi:10.1155/NP.2003.141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Koziol LF, Budding D, Andreasen N, D’Arrigo S, Bulgheroni S, Imamizu H, et al. Consensus paper: the cerebellum’s role in movement and cognition. Cerebellum (2014) 13(1):151–77. doi:10.1007/s12311-013-0511-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Doyon J, Bellec P, Amsel R, Penhune V, Monchi O, Carrier J, et al. Contributions of the basal ganglia and functionally related brain structures to motor learning. Behav Brain Res (2009) 199(1):61–75. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2008.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Ghez C, Thach WT. The cerebellum. 4th ed. In: Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM, editors. Principles of Neural Science. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill (2000). p. 832–52.

Google Scholar

34. O’Hare A, Khalid S. The association of abnormal cerebellar function in children with developmental coordination disorder and reading difficulties. Dyslexia (2002) 8:234–48. doi:10.1002/dys.230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Cantin N, Polatajko HJ, Thach WT, Jaglal S. Developmental coordination disorder: exploration of a cerebellar hypothesis. Hum Mov Sci (2007) 26(3):491–509. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Kagerer FA, Bo J, Contreras-Vidal JL, Clark JE. Visuomotor adaptation in children with developmental coordination disorder. Motor Control (2004) 8(4):450–60. doi:10.1123/mcj.8.4.450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kagerer FA, Contreras-Vidal JL, Bo J, Clark JE. Abrupt, but not gradual visuomotor distortion facilitates adaptation in children with developmental coordination disorder. Hum Mov Sci (2006) 25:622–33. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2006.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Lejeune C, Wansard M, Geurten M, Meulemans T. Procedural learning, consolidation, and transfer of a new skill in developmental coordination disorder. Child Neuropsychol (2016) 22(2):143–54. doi:10.1080/09297049.2014.988608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Cantin N, Ryan J, Polatajko HJ. Impact of task difficulty and motor ability on visual-motor task performance of children with and without developmental coordination disorder. Hum Mov Sci (2014) 34:217–32. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2014.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Brookes RL, Nicolson RI, Fawcett AJ. Prisms throw light on developmental disorders. Neuropsychologia (2007) 45:1921–30. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Calabresi P, Picconi B, Tozzi A, Ghiglieri V, Di Filippo M. Direct and indirect pathways of basal ganglia: a critical reappraisal. Nat Neurosci (2014) 17(8):1022–30. doi:10.1038/nn.3743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Groenewegen HJ. The basal ganglia and motor control. Neural Plast (2003) 10(1–2):107–20. doi:10.1155/np.2003.107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Gheysen F, Van Waelvelde H, Fias W. Impaired visuo-motor sequence learning in developmental coordination disorder. Res Dev Disabil (2011) 32:749–56. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Biotteau M, Chaix Y, Albaret J-M. Procedural learning and automatization process in children with developmental coordination disorder and/or developmental dyslexia. Hum Mov Sci (2015) 43:78–89. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2015.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Wilson PH, McKenzie BE. Information processing deficits associated with developmental coordination disorder: a meta-analysis of research findings. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1998) 39(6):829–40. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Lejeune C, Catale C, Willems S, Meulemans T. Intact procedural motor sequence learning in developmental coordination disorder. Res Dev Disabil (2013) 34(6):1974–81. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Bueti D, Walsh V. The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (2009) 364(1525):1831–40. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Desmurget M, Sirigu A. Conscious motor intention emerges in the inferior parietal lobule. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2012) 22(6):1004–11. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2012.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Fontana AP, Kilner JM, Rodrigues EC, Joffily M, Nighoghossian N, Vargas CD, et al. Role of the parietal cortex in predicting incoming actions. Neuroimage (2012) 59(1):556–64. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Sack AT. Parietal cortex and spatial cognition. Behav Brain Res (2009) 202(2):153–61. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Cummins A, Piek JP, Dyck MJ. Motor coordination, empathy, and social behaviour in school-aged children. Dev Med Child Neurol (2005) 47(7):437–42. doi:10.1017/S001216220500085X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Leonard HC, Bernardi M, Hill EL, Henry LA. Executive functioning, motor difficulties, and developmental coordination disorder. Dev Neuropsychol (2015) 40(4):201–15. doi:10.1080/87565641.2014.997933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Rahimi-Golkhandan S, Steenbergen B, Piek JP, Caeyenberghs K, Wilson PH. Revealing hot executive function in children with motor coordination problems: what’s the go? Brain Cogn (2016) 106:55–64. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2016.04.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Bernardi M, Leonard HC, Hill EL, Henry LA. Response inhibition and processing speed in children with motor difficulties and developmental coordination disorder. Child Neuropsychol (2016) 22(5):627–34. doi:10.1080/09297049.2015.1014898

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Tallet J, Albaret J-M, Barral J. Developmental changes in lateralized inhibition of symmetric movements in children with and without developmental coordination disorder. Res Dev Disabil (2013) 342:2523–32. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Caçola P, Ibana M, Ricard M, Gabbard C. Children with developmental coordination disorder demonstrate a spatial mismatch when estimating coincident-timing ability with tools. Res Dev Disabil (2016) 48:124–31. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Williams J, Omizzolo C, Galea MP, Vance A. Motor imagery skills of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and developmental coordination disorder. Hum Mov Sci (2013) 32:121–35. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2012.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Katschmarsky S, Cairney S, Maruff P, Wilson PH, Currie J. The ability to execute saccades on the basis of efference copy: impairments in double-step saccade performance in children with developmental co-ordination disorder. Exp Brain Res (2001) 136(1):73–8. doi:10.1007/s002210000535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Wilson PH, Maruff P, Lum J. Procedural learning in children with developmental coordination disorder. Hum Mov Sci (2003) 22(4–5):515–26. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2003.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Sigmundsson H. Perceptual deficits in clumsy children: inter- and intra-modal matching approach – a window into clumsy behavior. Neural Plast (2003) 10(1–2):27–38. doi:10.1155/NP.2003.27

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Richlan F. Developmental dyslexia: dysfunction of a left hemisphere reading network. Front Hum Neurosci (2012) 6:120. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Hart H, Radua J, Nakao T, Mataix-Cols D, Rubia K. Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: exploring task-specific, stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA Psychiatry (2013) 70(2):185–98. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Peters LHJ, Maathuis CGB, Hadders-Algra M. Neural correlates of developmental coordination disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol (2013) 55:59–64. doi:10.1111/dmcn.12309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Brown-Lum M, Zwicker JG. Brain imaging increases our understanding of developmental coordination disorder: a review of literature and future directions. Curr Dev Disord Rep (2015) 2(2):131–40. doi:10.1007/s40474-015-0046-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Mariën P, de Smet E, de Smet HJ, Wackenier P, Dobbeleir A, Verhoeven J. “Apraxic dysgraphia” in a 15-year-old left-handed patient: disruption of the cerebello-cerebral network involved in the planning and execution of graphomotor movements. Cerebellum (2013) 12(1):131–9. doi:10.1007/s12311-012-0395-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Richards TL, Berninger VW, Stock P, Altemeier L, Trivedi P, Maravilla K. fMRI sequential-finger movement activation differentiating good and poor writers. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (2009) 31(8):967–83. doi:10.1080/13803390902780201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Van Hoorn JF, Maathuis CGB, Hadders-Algra M. Neural correlates of paediatric dysgraphia. Dev Med Child Neurol (2013) 55:65–8. doi:10.1111/dmcn.12310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Querne L, Berquin P, Vernier-Hauvette M-P, Fall S, Deltour L, Meyer M-E, et al. Dysfunction of the attentional brain network in children with developmental coordination disorder: a fMRI study. Brain Res (2008) 1244:89–102. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.07.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Kashiwagi M, Iwaki S, Narumi Y, Tamai H, Suzuki S. Parietal dysfunction in developmental coordination disorder: a functional MRI study. Neuroreport (2009) 20(15):1319–24. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e32832f4d87

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Zwicker JG, Missiuna C, Harris SR, Boyd LA. Brain activation of children with developmental coordination disorder is different than peers. Pediatrics (2010) 126(3):e678–86. doi:10.1542/peds.2010-0059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Zwicker JG, Missiuna C, Harris SR, Boyd LA. Brain activation associated with motor skill practice in children with developmental coordination disorder: an fMRI study. Int J Dev Neurosci (2011) 29(2):145–52. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2010.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Zwicker JG, Missiuna C, Harris SR, Boyd LA. Developmental coordination disorder: a pilot diffusion tensor imaging study. Pediatr Neurol (2012) 46(3):162–7. doi:10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2011.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Debrabant J, Gheysen F, Caeyenberghs K, Van Waelvelde H, Vingerhoets G. Neural underpinnings of impaired predictive motor timing in children with developmental coordination disorder. Res Dev Disabil (2013) 34(5):1478–87. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. McLeod KR, Langevin LM, Goodyear BG, Dewey D. Functional connectivity of neural motor networks is disrupted in children with developmental coordination disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuroimage Clin (2014) 4:566–75. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2014.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Langevin LM, MacMaster FP, Crawford S, Lebel C, Dewey D. Common white matter microstructure alterations in pediatric motor and attention disorders. J Pediatr (2014) 164(5):1157–64. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2014.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Langevin LM, MacMaster FP, Dewey D. Distinct patterns of cortical thinning in concurrent motor and attention disorders. Dev Med Child Neurol (2015) 57(3):257–64. doi:10.1111/dmcn.12561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Licari M, Billington J, Reid S, Wann J, Elliott C, Winsor A, et al. Cortical functioning in children with developmental coordination disorder: a motor overflow study. Exp Brain Res (2015) 233(6):1703–10. doi:10.1007/s00221-015-4243-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Reynolds JE, Licari MK, Billington J, Chen Y, Aziz-Zadeh L, Werner J, et al. Mirror neuron activation in children with developmental coordination disorder: a functional MRI study. Int J Dev Neurosci (2015) 47(Pt B):309–19. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2015.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Debrabant J, Vingerhoets G, Van Waelvelde H, Leemans A, Taymans T, Caeyenberghs K. Brain connectomics of visual-motor deficits in children with developmental coordination disorder. J Pediatr (2016) 169:21–27.e22. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.09.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Caeyenberghs K, Taymans T, Wilson PH, Vanderstraeten G, Hosseini H, van Waelvelde H. Neural signature of developmental coordination disorder in the structural connectome independent of comorbid autism. Dev Sci (2016) 19(4):599–612. doi:10.1111/desc.12424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Flapper BCT, Schoemaker MM. Developmental coordination disorder in children with specific language impairment: co-morbidity and impact on quality of life. Res Dev Disabil (2013) 34(2):756–63. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Hill EL. Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: a review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. Int J Lang Commun Disord (2001) 36(2):149–71. doi:10.1080/13682820010019874

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Geuze RH, Schoemaker MM, Smits-Engelsman BCM. Clinical and research criteria for Developmental Coordination Disorder - Should they be one and the same? Curr Dev Disord Rep (2015) 2(2):127–30. doi:10.1007/s40474-015-0043-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Amlien IK, Fjell AM, Tamnes CK, Grydeland H, Krogsrud SK, Chaplin TA, et al. Organizing principles of human cortical development – thickness and area from 4 to 30 years: insights from comparative primate neuroanatomy. Cereb Cortex (2016) 26(1):257–67. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Tamnes CK, Walhovd KB, Dale AM, Østby Y, Grydeland H, Richardson G, et al. Brain development and aging: overlapping and unique patterns of change. Neuroimage (2013) 68:63–74. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Giedd JN, Clasen LS, Lenroot R, Greenstein D, Wallace GL, Ordaz S, et al. Puberty-related influences on brain development. Mol Cell Endocrinol (2006) 25(4–255):154–62. doi:10.1016/j.mce.2006.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Lenroot RK, Giedd JN. Sex differences in the adolescent brain. Brain Cogn (2010) 72(1):46–55. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Plessen KJ, Hugdahl K, Bansal R, Hao X, Peterson BS. Sex, age, and cognitive correlates of asymmetries in thickness of the cortical mantle across the life span. J Neurosci (2014) 34(18):6294. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3692-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Stoodley CJ. The cerebellum and neurodevelopmental disorders. Cerebellum (2016) 15(1):34–7. doi:10.1007/s12311-015-0715-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Desmottes L, Meulemans T, Maillart C. Later learning stages in procedural memory are impaired in children with specific language impairment. Res Dev Disabil (2016) 48:53–68. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2015.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Lum JAG, Conti-Ramsden G, Morgan AT, Ullman MT. Procedural learning deficits in specific language impairment (SLI): a meta-analysis of serial reaction time task performance. Cortex (2014) 51:1–10. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2013.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. von Rhein D, Oldehinkel M, Beckmann CF, Oosterlaan J, Heslenfeld D, Hartman CA, et al. Aberrant local striatal functional connectivity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2016) 57(6):697–705. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Schuetze M, Park MTM, Cho IYK, MacMaster FP, Chakravarty MM, Bray SL. Morphological alterations in the thalamus, striatum, and pallidum in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology (2016) 41(11):2627–37. doi:10.1038/npp.2016.64

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Worbe Y, Marrakchi-Kacem L, Lecomte S, Valabregue R, Poupon F, Guevara P, et al. Altered structural connectivity of cortico-striato-pallido-thalamic networks in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Brain (2015) 138(2):472–82. doi:10.1093/brain/awu311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Dien J. A tale of two recognition systems: implications of the fusiform face area and the visual word form area for lateralized object recognition models. Neuropsychologia (2009) 47:1–16. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Jobard G, Crivello F, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading: a metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage (2003) 20(2):693–712. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00343-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Richlan F, Kronbichler M, Wimmer H. Functional abnormalities in the dyslexic brain: a quantitative meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp (2009) 30(10):3299–308. doi:10.1002/hbm.20752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Chaix Y, Albaret J-M, Brassard C, Cheuret E, de Castelnau P, Bénesteau J, et al. Motor impairment in dyslexia: the influence of attention disorders. Eur J Paediatr Neurol (2007) 11:368–74. doi:10.1016/j.ejpn.2007.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Stoodley CJ, Stein JF. The cerebellum and dyslexia. Cortex (2011) 47(1):101–16. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Blank R, Smits-Engelsman B, Polatajko H, Wilson P. European Academy for Childhood Disability (EACD): recommendations on the definition, diagnosis and intervention of developmental coordination disorder (long version). Dev Med Child Neurol (2012) 54:54–93. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2011.04171.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Giedd JN, Lalonde FM, Celano MJ, White SL, Wallace GL, Lee NR, et al. Anatomical brain magnetic resonance imaging of typically developing children and adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (2009) 48(5):465–70. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819f2715

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Dewey D, Bernier FP. The concept of atypical brain development in developmental coordination disorder (DCD) – a new look. Curr Dev Disord Rep (2016) 3(2):161–9. doi:10.1007/s40474-016-0086-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Péran P, Cherubini A, Assogna F, Piras F, Quattrocchi C, Peppe A, et al. Magnetic resonance imaging markers of Parkinson’s disease nigrostriatal signature. Brain (2010) 133(11):3423–33. doi:10.1093/brain/awq212

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: neurodevelopmental disorder, neuroimaging, brain, developmental coordination disorder, neural correlates

Citation: Biotteau M, Chaix Y, Blais M, Tallet J, Péran P and Albaret J-M (2016) Neural Signature of DCD: A Critical Review of MRI Neuroimaging Studies. Front. Neurol. 7:227. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2016.00227

Received: 28 July 2016; Accepted: 28 November 2016;
Published: 16 December 2016

Edited by:

Andrea Guzzetta, University of Pisa, Italy

Reviewed by:

Simona Fiori, IRCCS Fondazione Stella Maris, Italy
Katrijn Klingels, KU Leuven, Belgium

Copyright: © 2016 Biotteau, Chaix, Blais, Tallet, Péran and Albaret. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maëlle Biotteau, maelle.biotteau@inserm.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.