Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 25 October 2017
Sec. Movement Disorders

Divergent Thinking in Parkinsonism: A Case–Control Study

\r\nMargherita Canesi*Margherita Canesi1*Maria Luisa RusconiMaria Luisa Rusconi2Emanuele CeredaEmanuele Cereda3Alessandra RanghettiAlessandra Ranghetti1Viviana CeredaViviana Cereda1Federica MoroniFederica Moroni2Gianni Pezzoli\r\nGianni Pezzoli1
  • 1Parkinson Institute, Azienda Socio Sanitaria Territoriale Pini-CTO, Milano, Italy
  • 2Department of Human and Social Sciences, Università degli Studi di Bergamo, Bergamo, Italy
  • 3Nutrition and Dietetics Service, Fondazione IRCCS Policlinico San Matteo, Pavia, Italy

Background: Creativity is a multidimensional phenomenon and an important component of human capacities. This ability is characterized by the involvement of several cognitive functions particularly linked to the prefrontal cortex. We compared divergent thinking, a measure of creativity, in patients affected by progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP), other parkinsonian syndromes, and healthy controls (HCs).

Methods: Creativity features were evaluated using the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA). Consecutive PSP outpatients were screened for inclusion. Then, patients with multiple system atrophy (MSA) and Parkinson’s disease (PD) and a group of HC were studied. All groups have preserved cognitive functions and were matched for gender, education, disease duration, and age at onset with exception of PD patients who were matched by disease severity rather than disease duration.

Results: PSP patients were characterized by lower values in total ATTA and all subscales than HC and both MSA and PD patients. No differences were found comparing HC versus both MSA and PD patients. PSP patients were characterized by more impaired frontal functioning [assessed by means of Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB)] than HC and both PD and MSA patients.

Conclusion: In the present study, ATTA was significantly lower in PSP patients than in the other study groups. The worst performance in ATTA-total score and the lower score in FAB in PSP patients support the role of frontal function in creative processes.

Introduction

Creative skills and creative process are still a fascinating, but unresolved topic up for debate as regards a variety of brain diseases (in particular, stroke and neurodegenerative diseases). It is known as the ability “to understand, develop and express in a systematic, novel orderly relationship” and also “to generate ideas that are both novel and useful in a particular social setting” (1, 2). Guilford assimilated creativity with divergent thinking (DT) which is the ability to generate a number of ideas that are useful to solve particular problems (3).

Measurement of creativity is crucial in order to be able to study and understand the phenomenon. One approach is to measure DT, which includes a number of aspects, such as fluency, flexibility, originality, and elaboration (4). Although DT is not the equivalent of creativity, it has frequently been used as a surrogate measure of creative cognition both in healthy controls (HCs) and different patients, such as those with cerebrovascular disease (5), or neurodegenerative diseases (6, 7). Also neuropsychological examinations are an effective approach for investigating creative skills. They are valid tools to establish which cognitive functions and brain regions are involved. Although creativity is an important component of human capacities, the interest in this field has recently grown and the underlying mechanisms and neural correlates have not been completely elucidated. This ability, expressed in different domains (i.e., arts, technology, and science) is characterized by the involvement of several higher cognitive functions (particularly, executive functions) linked to the prefrontal cortex (PFC): mental-set shifting, inhibitory control, and updating working memory (8).

Nevertheless, inconclusive results link executive functions and creativity. Miller et al. (9) have studied patients with semantic variants of frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD), who created works of art in spite of severe degeneration of striatal, temporal, and left inferior frontal–insular regions. On the other hand, significant deterioration of creative ability was observed in frontal variants of FTLD (10). In addition, heterogeneity in creative thinking has been observed in other neurodegenerative diseases, such as Alzheimer’s disease, Lewy bodies disease, and stroke affecting a number of different brain areas, which may be characterized by variable involvement of the PFC (11). Taken as a whole, these data suggest that the integrity of PFC is associated with creative skills.

In addition, DT—which is affected by mesolimbic dopamine—may be influenced or triggered by the use of psychotropic substances. This has recently been studied in Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients (712), in whom dopaminergic drugs seem to be able to enhance verbal and visual creative thinking. However, the observations support the idea that dopaminergic treatment may enhance the drive to create with no significant influence on DT itself (13, 14).

There are also other neurodegenerative diseases that are characterized by different types of cortical and subcortical dysfunction, dysregulation of dopamine circuitries, and response to dopaminergic therapy. Progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP), a predominant 4-repeat tauopathy, is characterized by midbrain atrophy, as well as atrophy of the internal pallidum, subthalamic nucleus, thalamus, and frontal cortex resulting in rigid-akinetic syndrome, ocular motor dysfunction, postural instability and falls, frontal signs, and bulbar dysfunction (15). No therapeutic strategies are available and dopaminergic treatment is almost ineffective. Specific cognitive domains are affected in PSP patients and include behavioral change (apathy, impulsivity, irritability) (16, 17), executive deficits (18, 19), verbal fluency (20, 21), memory (22), and social cognitive deficits (18). Cognitive impairment is evident also in PSP patients not identified as demented by means of Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (19). Multiple system atrophy (MSA), an α-synucleinopathy, is a disorder with combined manifestations of parkinsonism, autonomic failure, and cerebellar ataxia. It is characterized by neuropathological changes involving subcortical (basal ganglia) and cortical structures (23). Symptomatic therapeutic strategies are lacking although mild and temporary responses to dopaminergic stimulation are observed in some cases. Although the presence of cognitive decline has been considered as an exclusion criterion for MSA (2426), in more recent works, it was found to be present also in the early phase of the disease (1827). As suggested by neuropsychological studies (1828), executive dysfunction is the prominent cognitive disturbance in MSA (both parkinsonian and cerebellar variant). Neuroimaging and postmortem findings support the involvement of frontal, temporal and parietal regions in MSA cases (29, 30). Of note, cognitive impairment, particularly executive dysfunction, is prominent in PSP and MSA. Cognitive decline largely overlaps in PSP and MSA and only quantitative differences may clinically discriminate between the two diseases (31). Executive abilities are relevant for creative thought as suggested in the work of Benedek et al. (32). Moreover, executive functions seem to be linked to frontal areas both in functional and structural terms (33). To further explore the involvement of executive functions in creative abilities measured by means of DT, we decided to test patients with PSP in comparison with patients suffering from other parkinsonian diseases (MSA and PD) and with HC.

Materials and Methods

Patient/Subject Selection

We conducted a prospective case–control study at the outpatient clinic of the Parkinson Institute (ASST Gaetano Pini-CTO, Milan, Italy). First, consecutive PSP patients were included according to the criteria of the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke/Society for the diagnosis of Progressive Supranuclear Palsy (NINDS/SPSP) (24). Then, we recruited two additional groups of patients: patients with MSA [matched (1:1) by gender, disease duration (±1 year), age at onset (±1 year), and education (±1 year)] and PD [matched (1:1) by gender, education (±1 year)], age at assessment (±1 year), and disease severity (Hoehn–Yahr stage)] (34). PD patients could not be matched by disease duration due to the more benign course of the disease. Also a group of HC [matched (1:1) by gender, education (±1 year), and age at assessment (±1 year)] was studied.

Probable MSA was diagnosed according to the criteria provided in the second consensus statement on the diagnosis of MSA idiopathic PD was diagnosed in agreement with the UK PD Society Brain Bank criteria (35). All patients underwent magnetic resonance imaging to exclude alternative diagnoses, such as vascular parkinsonism or normal pressure hydrocephalus (36). We excluded patients with severe deficits which precluded the execution of the neuropsycological evaluation, i.e., severe dysarthria, severe bradykinesia, and rigidity with the impossibility of writing and drawing. We also excluded patients on antipsychotic and antidepressant therapy (6). Also patients with a positive history of impulsive–compulsive behavior were excluded (37).

Dementia (as assessed by MMSE score > 24/30 points) (38, 39) was an exclusion criterion for all patients and HC and nobody had been a professional artist or had been creating works of art as a hobby in the past. Patients treated with neurosurgical procedures (mainly deep brain stimulation) were excluded and none of the HC included was a caregiver or family member of study patients.

Measurements

The Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA) (4) was used to assess creativity. Specifically, the test is used to investigate four factors of creative thinking: Flexibility (a kind of thinking that allows an exchange of ideas and strategies, and the ability to pass from one category to another), Fluency (the ability to conceive many ideas and hypotheses without focusing on their quality, which is potentially useful in problem solving), Originality (the capacity to find unusual and rare answers), and Elaboration (the ability to further develop a concept by adding new elements).

A visuospatial task [Clock Drawing Test (CDT)] (40) and a frontal functions battery [Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB)] (41) were also administered. All tests were administered by trained psychologists, expert in movement disorders, in the morning hours, 90 min after the last dose of dopaminergic therapy. Patients who were not undergoing dopaminergic therapy were tested during their best condition.

On the same day, clinical work-up included the evaluation of disease severity and motor functions (in worst “OFF” conditions) by means of the Hoehn and Yahr (H&Y) staging system (34) and the Unified Parkinson Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) part III (motor score) (42), respectively. Although specific scales for PSP (PSP rating scale) and MSA (UMSARS) are available (43, 44), we used UPDRS part III to measure and compare disability conditions between the different neurodegenerative diseases. Information on dopaminergic treatment was also recorded in all study groups and doses of dopaminergic medication were converted to equivalent levodopa-equivalent daily doses (LEDDs) (45).

Statistical Analysis

Sample size was calculated on the difference in primary outcome variable (ATTA total score) between PSP patients and HCs. Considering a meaningful difference in total ATTA score of at least 20 points and a common SD of 18 points (13), the sample size sufficient to have a power of 80% with a type I error of 5% is 13 patients in each group.

Continuous variables were reported as mean and SD and compared between groups using the Student’s t-test for paired data with exception of the comparison with CBS patients (test for unpaired data). Specifically, we compared PSP patients to HC and other parkinsonian syndromes. The comparison between HC and other types of parkinsonian syndromes was also investigated. Categorical variables were presented as count and percentage. Finally, correlations between test scores were analyzed using Kendall’s tau test.

All data were analyzed using MedCalc Statistical Software version 16.8.4 (MedCalc Software bvba, Ostend, Belgium), setting the level of significance at a two-tailed P-value of <0.05.

Ethics

The study was performed in agreement with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and the protocol was approved by the local Ethics Committee (Protocol 272-2010). We obtain written informed consent from every patient and control subject recruited.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the study groups are shown in Table 1. As expected, taking into account the less severe progression of PD and the decision to match by disease severity, PD patients were characterized by younger age at onset and longer disease duration. No differences were found in motor disability between PSP and other parkinsonian syndromes. Most PSP, MSA and PD patients were on dopaminergic therapy, mainly levodopa (9, 10, and 13 patients, respectively), while a substantial number of PD patients were receiving a dopamine agonist (7 patients). Results of neuropsychological tests are presented in Table 2. In the absence of cognitive deterioration, as assessed by MMSE, PSP patients were characterized by more impaired frontal functioning (FAB) than HC and both PD and MSA patients. Significantly lower values (P < 0.05) in FAB were observed in MSA patients compared to controls. MSA patients showed also lower CDT score than HC (P < 0.05). Data regarding creativity, as assessed by ATTA, are reported in Table 3. With the exception of the comparison in fluency score between PSP and MSA, PSP patients were characterized by lower values in total ATTA and all subscales than HC and both MSA and PD patients. No differences were found comparing HC versus both MSA and PD patients. Although the patients included should have had normal cognitive functions, the presence of cognitive alterations could not be fully excluded. We therefore explored the possible relationship between DT and specific cognitive subscores related to functions potentially contributing to creativity: verbal fluency by FAB-lexical fluency subscore and visual-constructional abilities by MMSE-drowing copy subscore. PSP patients performed significantly worse than all the other study groups (Figure 1), (Table 2). In addition, at population level (all groups pooled), ATTA total score was significantly correlated with both FAB-lexical fluency (τ = 0.20; P = 0.034) and MMSE-design copy (τ = 0.40; P < 0.001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the study groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Neuropsychological evaluation.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Creativity features as assessed by the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Exemplification of drawing test in the different study groups [(A) basic drawing; (B) progressive supranuclear palsy; (C) Parkinson’s disease; (D) multiple system atrophy].

Discussion

In the present study, DT, measured by means of ATTA, was significantly lower in PSP patients than in the other study groups. The worst performance in terms of ATTA-total, along with the lower score in FAB, found in PSP patients suggests that frontal function has an important role in creative processes.

The role of the frontal cortex in DT has been pointed out in previous studies in HC. Regional cerebral blood flow measurements were used in control subjects with high and low scores in creative tests to study the functional organization of the cortex. Highly creative subjects showed bilateral frontal activation during DT tasks (46). Fink et al. (47) found frontal alpha synchronization as a selective top-down inhibition process in HC during DT tasks by means of the analysis of the alpha band of the electroencephalogram. Moreover, creativity requires cognitive abilities linked to prefrontal regions of brain. Dietrich proposed four basic types of creative insight occurring during two modes of thought (deliberate and spontaneous) and two types of information (emotional and cognitive) (48). Transcranial magnetic stimulation over frontal lobes can increase creativity in normal subjects (49). Flaherty suggested a useful and simplified model that helps to characterize the different contributions of the frontal, temporal and limbic systems (6). Trained musicians as creative group were investigated as regards creativity and frontal cortical activity in two experimental sets. In the first experiment, musicians showed convergent and DT and scored higher in the Schizotypal Personality Scale when compared with HC. In the second set, the authors observed greater frontal activation in musicians than HC during DT tasks at near-infrared spectroscopy (50). Although all patients included in our study were non-demented according to MMSE scores (inclusion criteria), significant differences were observed in frontal functions evaluated by means of FAB, a bedside test of executive functions, which includes abstract conceptualization, fluency, sequencing, inhibition and interference with motor command (51). In agreement with a previous study (18), PSP patients performed worse than other study groups. No differences were evident among PD, MSA and HC groups. The most common cognitive deficit in the majority of PSP patients concerns executive function, which has been linked to the involvement of the medial PFC (and the connections with the striatum) (52, 53). Verbal fluency, incorporated in the FAB test, is impaired in PSP patients and enables to discriminate between PSP and PD. Indeed, seven or less P-words per minute suggests PSP (5456). The severity of executive dysfunction and the negative ATTA outcome observed in PSP group provide further evidence suggesting a relationship between frontal integrity and creative thought.

Clock Drawing Test was significantly worse than MSA when compared to HC, likely due to more impaired visual-spatial abilities (4057). PD and PSP groups performed worse when compared to HC, but data produced just a trend, no significant differences. Besides, our results support the hypothesis of a lack rather than a weak relationship between ATTA scores and dopaminergic treatment, and point out the importance of the involvement of different brain areas, including the frontal area, in the creative process. It has been previously pointed out that dopaminergic treatment is involved in the emerging of creative drive (714). Indeed, no differences emerged in ATTA scores between HC (with no dopaminergic treatment) and both the PD and the MSA (most patients were on dopaminergic therapy) group. On the contrary, significant differences in creative tasks emerged between HC and PSP group, although some PSP patients were on low-dose dopaminergic therapy (69.2%). Nevertheless, owing to the small number of patients included in the study, we cannot entirely exclude any influence of dopaminergic treatment on ATTA between-group comparisons. Therefore, the present study is consistent with previous observations that enhancement in creative activity may occur, mainly in predisposed patients (13). Moreover, the results of this study suggest that ATTA could be used as an additional diagnostic tool to help differentiate PD from PSP.

Although the results are interesting, we are aware that our study has some limitations. First, our sample size was relatively small due to the rare nature of parkinsonisms and the inclusion criteria. Therefore, the study groups included are not fully representative of the patient populations. Particularly, it is known that cognitive deficits are frequent in these patients (57) and we included only patients without global cognitive deficits, with the aim of identifying patients able to understand the tasks. Besides, the study design and the matching procedure enabled us to reduce the bias associated with potential confounders. Second, we performed a brief, non extensive neuropsychological assessment in order to avoid fatigue and attentional problems during tests in patients with parkinsonism. However, based on previous observations, a more detailed evaluation of neuropsychiatric disorders (e.g., by means of neuropsychiatric inventory) would have provided more information on cortical functions (58, 59). Third, we are aware that creativity is not completely attributable to the performance of the frontal cortex, as previous findings have revealed the selective involvement of different brain regions in diverse aspects of creativity (60). Finally, although the use of clinical criteria (e.g., disease duration, response to levodopa) and neuroimaging investigations improved the probability of a correct diagnoses, diagnostic certainty can be achieved only by pathological confirmation on postmortem tissue examination. In conclusion, neurodegenerative diseases, such as PSP, MSA, and PD, are characterized by the involvement of different brain areas and the analysis of creative competence enables us to speculate about the role played by these brain areas. Particularly, PSP patients performed worse than the other patients, despite having disease of similar severity and similar global cognitive performances. These results suggest that creative accomplishment requires most of all the integrity of the frontal areas, which appear to play a prominent, albeit not exclusive role.

Ethics Statement

Ethics Committee: ASST-G Pini, Centro Specialistico Ortopedico Traumatologico, Milano, Italy. The study was performed in agreement with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and the protocol was approved by the local Ethics Committee (Protocol 272-2010). We obtain written informed consent from every patient and control subject recruited.

Author Contributions

All the authors significantly contributed to the work, read, and approved the final version of the manuscript. MC, MR, AR, and VC contributed to study design. MC, AR, VC, and FM; contributed to data collection. MC, MR, EC, AR, VC, and FM contributed to data analyses and interpretation; contributed to manuscript drafting. MC, MR, EC, AR, VC, and GP contributed to critical revision of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer SM and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Dr. Jennifer S. Hartwig for assistance in editing the manuscript. We thank all patients and families for their contribution.

Funding

This work was supported by the “Fondazione Grigioni per il Morbo di Parkinson” (Milan, Italy).

References

1. Chakravarty A. The creative brain–revisiting concepts. Med Hypotheses (2010) 3:606–12. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2009.10.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Runco MA, Jaeger GJ. The standard definition of creativity. Creat Res J (2012) 1:92–6. doi:10.1080/10400419.2012.650092

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Guilford JP. Creativity American Psychologist. New York: McGraw Hill (1950).

Google Scholar

4. Torrance EP. The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (Norma-Technical Manual). Bensenville, IL: Scholastic Testing Service (1974).

Google Scholar

5. Lythgoe MF, Pollak TA, Kalmus M, De Haan M, Chong WK. Obsessive, prolific artistic output following subarachnoid hemorrhage. Neurology (2005) 2:397–8. doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000150526.09499.3E

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Flaherty AW. Frontotemporal and dopaminergic control of idea generation and creative drive. JComp Neurol (2005) 1:147–53. doi:10.1002/cne.20768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Faust-Socher A, Kenett YN, Cohen OS, Hassin-Baer S, Inzelberg R. Enhanced creative thinking under dopaminergic therapy in Parkinson disease. Ann Neurol (2014) 6:935–42. doi:10.1002/ana.24181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol (2000) 1:49–100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Miller BL, Ponton M, Benson DF, Cummings JL, Mena I. Enhanced artistic productivity with temporal lobe degeneration. Lancet (1996) 348(9043):1744–5. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(05)65881-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. de Souza LC, Volle E, Bertoux M, Czernecki V, Funkiewiez A, Allali G, et al. Poor creativity in frontotemporal dementia: a window into the neural bases of the creative mind. Neuropsychologia (2010) 48(13):3733–42. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. de Souza LC, Guimarães HC, Teixeira AL, Caramelli P, Levy R, Dubois B, et al. Frontal lobe neurology and the creative mind. Front Psychol (2014) 5:761. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Canesi M, Rusconi ML, Isaias IU, Pezzoli G. Artistic productivity and creative thinking in Parkinson’s disease. Eur J Neurol (2011) 3:468–72. doi:10.1111/J1468-1331.2011.03546.X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Canesi M, Rusconi ML, Moroni F, Ranghetti A, Cereda E, Pezzoli G, et al. Creative thinking, professional artists, and Parkinson’s disease. J Parkinsons Dis (2015) 1:239–46. doi:10.3233/JPD-150681

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Inzelberg R. The awakening of artistic creativity and Parkinson’s disease. Behav Neurosci (2013) 2:256. doi:10.1037/a0031052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Respondek G, Roeber S, Kretzschmar H, Troakes C, Al-Sarraj S, Gelpi E, et al. Accuracy of the National Institute for Neurological Disorders and Stroke/Society for Progressive Supranuclear Palsy and neuroprotection and natural history in Parkinson plus syndromes criteria for the diagnosis of progressive supranuclear palsy. Mov Disord (2013) 4:504–9. doi:10.1002/mds.25327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Bak TH, Crawford LM, Berrios G, Hodges JR. Behavioural symptoms in progressive supranuclear palsy and frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2010) 81(9):1057–9. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2008.157974

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Aarsland D, Cummings JL, Larsen JP. Neuropsychiatric differences between Parkinson’s disease with dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry (2001) 16(2):184–91. doi:10.1002/1099-1166(200102)16:2<184::AID-GPS304>3.0.CO;2-K

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Brown RG, Lacomblez L, Landwehrmeyer BG, Bak T, Uttner I, Dubois B, et al. Cognitive impairment in patients with multiple system atrophy and progressive supranuclear palsy. Brain (2010) 133(8):2382–93. doi:10.1093/brain/awq158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Gerstenecker A, Mast B, Duff K, Ferman TJ, Litvan I; ENGENE-PSP Study Group. Executive dysfunction is the primary cognitive impairment in progressive supranuclear palsy. Arch Clin Neuropsychol (2012) 28(2):104–13. doi:10.1093/arclin/acs098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Bak TH, Crawford LM, Hearn VC, Mathuranath PS, Hodges JR. Subcortical dementia revisited: similarities and differences in cognitive function between progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP), corticobasal degeneration (CBD) and multiple system atrophy (MSA). Neurocase (2005) 11(4):268–73. doi:10.1080/13554790590962997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Cotelli M, Borroni B, Manenti R, Alberici A, Calabria M, Agosti C, et al. Action and object naming in frontotemporal dementia, progressive supranuclear palsy, and corticobasal degeneration. Neuropsychology (2006) 20(5):558. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.20.5.558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Litvan I, Grafman J, Gomez C, Chase TN. Memory impairment in patients with progressive supranuclear palsy. Arch Neurol (1989) 46(7):765–7. doi:10.1001/archneur.1989.00520430059018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Papp MI, Lantos PL. The distribution of oligodendroglial inclusions in multiple system atrophy and its relevance to clinical symptomatology. Brain (1994) 2:235–43. doi:10.1093/brain/117.2.235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Litvan I, Agid Y, Calne D, Campbell G, Dubois B, Duvoisin RC, et al. Clinical research criteria for the diagnosis of progressive supranuclear palsy (Steele-Richardson-Olszewski syndrome) report of the NINDS-SPSP international workshop. Neurology (1996) 47(1):1–9. doi:10.1212/WNL.47.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Gilman S, Low PA, Quinn N, Albanese A, Ben-Shlomo Y, Fowler CJ, et al. Consensus statement on the diagnosis of multiple system atrophy. J Neurol Sci (1999) 163(1):94–8. doi:10.1016/S0022-510X(98)00304-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Gilman S, Wenning GK, Low PA, Brooks DJ, Mathias CJ, Trojanowski JQ, et al. Second consensus statement on the diagnosis of multiple system atrophy. Neurology (2008) 9:670–6. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000324625.00404.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Kitayama M, Wada-Isoe K, Irizawa Y, Nakashima K. Assessment of dementia in patients with multiple system atrophy. Eur J Neurol (2009) 16(5):589–94. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.2009.02544.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Siri C, Duerr S, Canesi M, Delazer M, Esselink R, Bloem BR, et al. A cross-sectional multicenter study of cognitive and behavioural features in multiple system atrophy patients of the parkinsonian and cerebellar type. J Neural Transm (2013) 120(4):613–8. doi:10.1007/s00702-013-0997-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Paviour DC, Price SL, Jahanshahi M, Lees AJ, Fox NC. Longitudinal MRI in progressive supranuclear palsy and multiple system atrophy: rates and regions of atrophy. Brain (2006) 129(4):1040–9. doi:10.1093/brain/awl021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Wenning GK, Tison F, Ben Shlomo Y, Daniel SE, Quinn NP. Multiple system atrophy: a review of 203 pathologically proven cases. Mov Disord (1997) 12(2):133–47. doi:10.1002/mds.870120203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Stankovic I, Krismer F, Jesic A, Antonini A, Benke T, Brown RG, et al. Cognitive impairment in multiple system atrophy: a position statement by the Neuropsychology Task Force of the MDS Multiple System Atrophy (MODIMSA) study group. Mov Disord (2014) 29(7):857–67. doi:10.1002/mds.25880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Benedek M, Jauk E, Sommer M, Arendasy M, Neubauer AC. Intelligence, creativity, and cognitive control: the common and differential involvement of executive functions in intelligence and creativity. Intelligence (2014) 46:73–83. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Yuan P, Raz N. Prefrontal cortex and executive functions in healthy adults: a meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2014) 42:180–92. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Hoehn MH, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology (1967) 5:427–42. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Hughes AJ, Ben-Shlomo Y, Daniel SE, Lees AJ. What features improves the accuracy of clinical diagnosis in Parkinson’s disease: a clinicopathologic study. Neurology (2001) 57:S34–8. doi:10.1212/WNL.57.8.1497

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Rizzo G, Zanigni S, De Blasi R, Grasso D, Martino D, Savica R, et al. Contribution to the differential diagnosis of parkinsonian syndromes: an update. Parkinsons Dis (2016) 2016:27. doi:10.1155/2016/2983638

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Weintraub D, Mamikonyan E, Papay K, Shea JA, Xie SX, Siderowf A. Questionnaire for impulsive-compulsive disorders in Parkinson’s Disease–Rating Scale. Mov Disord (2012) 27(2):242–7. doi:10.1002/mds.24023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Magni E, Binetti G, Bianchetti A, Rozzini R, Trabucchi M. Mini-mental state examination: a normative study in Italian elderly population. Eur J Neurol (1996) 3:198–202. doi:10.1111/j.1468-1331.1996.tb00423.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Measso G, Cavarzeran F, Zappalà G, Lebowitz BD, Crook TH, Pirozzolo FJ, et al. The mini-mental state examination: normative study of an Italian random sample. Dev Neuropsychol (1993) 2:77–85. doi:10.1080/87565649109540545

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Shulman KI, Pushkar Gold D, Cohen CA, Zucchero CA. Clock-drawing and dementia in the community: a longitudinal study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry (1993) 6:487–96. doi:10.1002/gps.930080606

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Appollonio I, Leone M, Isella V, Piamarta F, Consoli T, Villa ML, et al. The frontal assessment battery (FAB): normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurol Sci (2005) 2:108–16. doi:10.1007/s10072-005-0443-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Fahn S, Elton RL. Members of the UPDRS development committee: unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale. In: Fahn S, Marsden CD, Calne DB, Goldstein M, editors. Recent Developments in Parkinson’s Disease. Florham Park, NJ: Macmillan Health Care Information. (Vol. 153), (1987). 304 p.

Google Scholar

43. Golbe LI, Ohman-Strickland PA. A clinical rating scale for progressive supranuclear palsy. Brain (2007) 6:1552–65. doi:10.1093/brain/awm032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Wenning GK, Colosimo C, Geser F, Poewe W. Multiple system atrophy. Lancet Neurol (2004) 2:93–103. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(03)00662-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Tomlinson CL, Stowe R, Patel S, Rick C, Gray R, Clarke CE. Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord (2010) 15:2649–53. doi:10.1002/mds.23429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Carlsson I, Wendt PE, Risberg J. On the neurobiology of creativity. Differences in frontal activity between high and low creative subjects. Neuropsychologia (2000) 6:873–85. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00128-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Fink A, Grabner RH, Benedek M, Neubauer AC. Divergent thinking training is related to frontal electroencephalogram alpha synchronization. Eur J Neurosci (2006) 8:2241–6. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04751.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Dietrich A. The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychon Bull Rev (2004) 11(6):1011–26. doi:10.3758/BF03196731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Snyder A, Mitchell J, Bossomaier T, Pallier G. The creativity quotient: an objective scoring of ideational fluency. Creat Res J (2004) 4:415–9. doi:10.1080/10400410409534552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Gibson C, Folley BS, Park S. Enhanced divergent thinking and creativity in musicians: a behavioral and near-infrared spectroscopy study. Brain Cogn (2009) 1:162–9. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Dubois B, Slachevsky A, Litvan I, Pillon BF. The FAB: a frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology (2000) 11:1621–6. doi:10.1212/WNL.55.11.1621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Gläscher J, Adolphs R, Damasio H, Bechara A, Rudrauf D, Calamia M, et al. Lesion mapping of cognitive control and value-based decision making in the prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2012) 109(36):14681–6. doi:10.1073/pnas.1206608109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Robinson H, Calamia M, Gläscher J, Bruss J, Tranel D. Neuroanatomical correlates of executive functions: a neuropsychological approach using the EXAMINER battery. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2014) 20(1):52–63. doi:10.1017/S135561771300060X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Rittman T, Ghosh BC, McColgan P, Breen DP, Evans J, Williams-Gray CH, et al. The Addenbrooke’s cognitive examination for the differential diagnosis and longitudinal assessment of patients with parkinsonian disorders. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2013) 84(5):544–51. doi:10.1136/jnnp-2012-303618

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Burrell JR, Hodges JR, Rowe JB. Cognition in corticobasal syndrome and progressive supranuclear palsy: a review. Mov Disord (2014) 29(5):684–93. doi:10.1002/mds.25872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Fiorenzato E, Weis L, Falup-Pecurariu C, Diaconu S, Siri C, Reali E, et al. Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) and mini-mental state examination (MMSE) performance in progressive supranuclear palsy and multiple system atrophy. J Neural Transm (2016) 123(12):1435–42. doi:10.1007/s00702-016-1589-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Soliveri P, Monza D, Paridi D, Carella F, Genitrini S, Testa D, et al. Neuropsychological follow up in patients with Parkinson’s disease, striatonigral de generation-type multisystem atrophy, and progressive supranuclear palsy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2000) 69:313–8. doi:10.1136/jnnp.69.3.313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, Rosenberg-Thompson S, Carusi DA, Gornbein J. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory. Comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology (1994) 44:12–2308. doi:10.1212/WNL.44.12.2308

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Binetti G, Alberici AC, Fedi V, Cappa SF, Benussi L, Moratto D, et al. Clinical features of a familial frontotemporal dementia with parkinsonism associated to chromosome 17: peculiar onset, progression, and cerebral atrophy. Neurology (1998) 4:A234.

Google Scholar

60. Abraham A, Beudt S, Ott DV, von Cramon DY. Creative cognition and the brain: dissociations between frontal, parietal–temporal and basal ganglia groups. Brain Res (2012) 1482:55–70. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2012.09.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: creativity, divergent thinking, parkinsonism, progressive supranuclear palsy, multiple system atrophy, Parkinson’s disease, Torrance test

Citation: Canesi M, Rusconi ML, Cereda E, Ranghetti A, Cereda V, Moroni F and Pezzoli G (2017) Divergent Thinking in Parkinsonism: A Case–Control Study. Front. Neurol. 8:534. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00534

Received: 30 June 2017; Accepted: 25 September 2017;
Published: 25 October 2017

Edited by:

Jaime Kulisevsky, Sant Pau Hospital – Sant Pau Institute of Biomedical Research, Spain

Reviewed by:

Saul Martinez-Horta, Sant Pau Hospital, Spain
Graziella Madeo, National Institutes of Health (NIH), United States

Copyright: © 2017 Canesi, Rusconi, Cereda, Ranghetti, Cereda, Moroni and Pezzoli. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Margherita Canesi, margheritag2007@yahoo.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.