Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurol., 22 January 2018
Sec. Sleep Disorders

Altered Regional Brain Cortical Thickness in Pediatric Obstructive Sleep Apnea

Paul M. Macey,Paul M. Macey1,2 \r\nLeila Kheirandish-Gozal*Leila Kheirandish-Gozal3* \r\nJanani P. PrasadJanani P. Prasad1 \r\nRichard A. MaRichard A. Ma1 \r\nRajesh Kumar,,Rajesh Kumar2,4,5 \r\nMona F. PhilbyMona F. Philby3David Gozal\r\nDavid Gozal3
  • 1School of Nursing, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 2Brain Research Institute, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 3Department of Pediatrics, Section of Pediatric Sleep Medicine, Pritzker School of Medicine, Biological Sciences Division, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 4Department of Anesthesiology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 5Department of Radiological Sciences, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, United States

Rationale: Obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) affects 2–5% of all children and is associated with cognitive and behavioral deficits, resulting in poor school performance. These psychological deficits may arise from brain injury, as seen in preliminary findings of lower gray matter volume among pediatric OSA patients. However, the psychological deficits in OSA are closely related to functions in the cortex, and such brain areas have not been specifically assessed. The objective was to determine whether cortical thickness, a marker of possible brain injury, is altered in children with OSA.

Methods: We examined regional brain cortical thicknesses using high-resolution T1-weighted magnetic resonance images in 16 pediatric OSA patients (8 males; mean age ± SD = 8.4 ± 1.2 years; mean apnea/hypopnea index ± SD = 11 ± 6 events/h) and 138 controls (8.3 ± 1.1 years; 62 male; 138 subjects from the NIH Pediatric MRI database) to identify cortical thickness differences in pediatric OSA subjects.

Results: Cortical thinning occurred in multiple regions including the superior frontal, ventral medial prefrontal, and superior parietal cortices. The left side showed greater thinning in the superior frontal cortex. Cortical thickening was observed in bilateral precentral gyrus, mid-to-posterior insular cortices, and left central gyrus, as well as right anterior insula cortex.

Conclusion: Changes in cortical thickness are present in children with OSA and likely indicate disruption to neural developmental processes, including maturational patterns of cortical volume increases and synaptic pruning. Regions with thicker cortices may reflect inflammation or astrocyte activation. Both the thinning and thickening associated with OSA in children may contribute to the cognitive and behavioral dysfunction frequently found in the condition.

Introduction

Pediatric obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) has a prevalence of up to 5% and is the leading sleep disorder in children (1, 2). The condition is characterized by repeated intermittent upper airway obstruction during sleep, which results in episodic alveolar hypoventilation and hypoxemia, as well as sleep fragmentation (2). The condition leads to multiple daytime symptoms and is associated with an increased risk for severity-dependent cognitive, behavioral, and cardiovascular and metabolic morbidities, all of which likely impact optimal behavioral and health development (311). In addition, the cognitive and behavioral deficits frequently encountered in pediatric OSA likely lead to poor school performance (1215). Brain injury is a possible mechanism underlying these symptoms, as shown in the recently reported gray matter changes in pediatric OSA (16). These earlier findings showed general regions with lower gray matter volume, but many structures of specific interest to cognitive function and school performance were not assessed. In particular, the methodology was suited to detecting changes deep in the brain, whereas much executive function and cognitive processing is centered in cortical structures. This lack of understanding of whether and how cortical regions are affected limits selection of possible interventional targets, particularly for the cognitive difficulties, which are especially important for children in relation to their scholastic development.

Previous studies examining OSA in adults have shown brain injury and physiological and psychological symptoms similar to those detected in pediatric OSA. Neuroimaging studies in OSA report alterations of white and gray matter, as well as changes in neurochemicals within various brain regions, raising the possibility that pediatric patients may also exhibit such brain changes (1722). Our earlier study in children with OSA focused on regional gray matter volume assessment showed selected limbic, subcortical, and brainstem regions with reduced gray matter (16). These brain regions, along with other sites, control cognition and mood, such that OSA-induced injury may underlie the cognitive deficits frequently seen in pediatric OSA. Given that more frequent behavioral symptoms are seen in even milder forms of OSA (23, 24), more widespread cortical areas may be impacted. However, the “voxel-based morphometry” technique used for the earlier gray matter quantitation is less sensitive to changes in outer cortical regions that in deeper brains structures, thus raising the need to examine regional cortical sites with a different approach.

Cortical thickness is one measure that is highly sensitive to changes only in neuronal cortical areas (25), and both the cognitive and behavioral findings suggest that cortex-specific regions may be affected, perhaps due to injury from the intermittent hypoxia or sleep fragmentation in pediatric OSA (7, 11, 26). Cortical thickness examination can be performed with standard image processing techniques, such as FreeSurfer software (27). This approach involves detecting the inner and outer surfaces of the cortical gray matter, and calculating the thickness as the distance between the two surfaces. Specialized statistics are used to assess thinning or thickening of the cortex, and this methodology has been applied to many conditions.

The purpose of this study was to assess potential injury to the brain cortical regions in children diagnosed with OSA using polysomnography. Building on earlier findings of gray matter injury in mixed cortical and subcortical areas (16), we hypothesized that regional cortical thickness alterations would occur in many areas in pediatric OSA subjects, and more specifically in those regions that are involved in cognition.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

The study was approved by the human ethics committee at the University of Chicago (IRB Protocol # 11-0280-CR004) and written informed consent was obtained from the legal caregiver of each participant in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Assent was obtained from children >7 years old. Children being evaluated for habitual snoring, who were diagnosed as OSA with an overnight polysomnography, were invited to participate. Invitations were given consecutively to children who visited the clinic until the sample was attained. Controls were recruited through our well-child clinics, after ascertaining that they did not snore based on a validated questionnaire, and further subjected to a sleep study to confirm the absence of any evidence indicating either snoring or more severe forms of sleep-disordered breathing. Participants underwent baseline anthropometric assessments, as well as overnight polysomnography, which were interpreted using standard criteria (28, 29). In addition, a neurocognitive battery was also administered to all participants in the morning (starting at 9:00 a.m.) after breakfast and following the sleep study. The details and specific tests included in the cognitive battery have been previously reported in great detail (11). In brief, the cognitive tests administered the morning following polysomnographic assessment consisted of the Differential Ability Scales (DAS) (30). This battery of tests has been developed and standardized for assessment of aspects of broad cognitive functioning, as well as more specific domains of neuropsychological status, and is frequently utilized diagnostically in educational and clinical settings. Results are normalized to a standardized score of 100 with a SD of 10 for general conceptual ability, which is viewed as a surrogate of intellectual quotient. All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.

Exclusion Criteria

Children were excluded from the study if they were diagnosed with ADHD, were using psychostimulant medications (n = 5), or exhibited known neurodevelopmental delays (n = 1). In addition, children with hypertension or using antihypertensive drug therapies were excluded (n = 2). Furthermore, children with either known or suspected diabetes, as delineated by the Global IDF/ISPAD Guideline for Diabetes in Childhood and Adolescence1 (n = 1), with a craniofacial, neuromuscular, or defined genetic syndrome, and children on chronic anti-inflammatory therapy (n = 1), or with any known acute or chronic illness were also excluded. MRI scanning exclusion criteria included metallic implants and claustrophobia. MRI data exclusion criteria include motion and other artifacts.

Anthropometry

The Centre for Disease Control 2000 and The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia online software2 were used to calculate height and weight centiles and body mass index (BMI) Z-scores respectively. Obesity was defined as BMI Z-scores >1.65.

Sphygmomanometry

All children’s arterial blood pressure was measured. Using National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute guidelines,3 systolic and diastolic BP indices (SBPi and DBPi, respectively) were calculated by dividing the average systolic and diastolic pressure by the respective 95th percentile for BP, computed for age, sex, and height. SBPi or DBPi pressures >1 was categorized as hypertension.

Overnight Polysomnography

As previously described, standard approaches were used to conduct overnight polysomnography (28, 29). The criteria for OSA diagnosis consisted of an obstructive apnea/hypopnea index (AHI) >2/h total sleep time (TST) and a nadir SpO2 <92%, and/or a respiratory arousal index >2/h TST (31, 32).

Neurocognitive Assessments

The cognitive tests were conducted in the morning after the sleep study and consisted of the DAS (30). The school age form of the DAS was administered, which yields a Spatial Cluster score in addition to the verbal, nonverbal, and global composite score, the latter called the general conceptual ability (GCA) score. The sum of the core subtest t-scores is converted to a total battery standard score, the GCA, with a mean of 100 and a SD of 15 (33).

MRI Scanning

Some control and all OSA subjects were scanned at The University of Chicago, and a large number of healthy subjects from the NIH Pediatric MRI database were used to supplement the control group.

We collected brain MRI data from children within 3–5 days after the sleep study on a Philips Achieva 1.5-T scanner. High-resolution three-dimensional T1-weighted anatomical scans were collected from 16 OSA and 9 control subjects using a custom ultrafast gradient echo “SENSE” sequence (repetition time = 8.16 ms; TE = 3.7 ms; flip angle = 8°; matrix size = 256 × 256; field of view = 224 mm × 224 mm; slice thickness = 1.0 mm; number of slices = 160). Six additional control subjects were originally scanned, but were excluded from analysis due to motion artifacts in the images.

An additional 138 control subjects were included from a national database. We downloaded high-resolution T1-weighted images of remaining control subjects from the NIH Pediatric MRI database4 with permission. We used a large control cohort, since increasing cohort size is one approach to improve reliability of volume-based analyses in OSA (34). Note that this “population control” group cannot be assumed to have identical characteristics as the nine subjects studied at Chicago, which would affect any sample size power or sensitivity calculations. Full details of recruitment and scanning protocols are available from the project website, including confirmation that consent and assent was obtain as appropriate for the age of the subjects, and that procedures were approved by applicable institutional review boards. In brief, the purpose of the pediatric MRI study of normal brain development includes providing a normative database of the developing brain for comparison with neuroimaging studies of pediatric disease conditions. Participants were recruited from six sites across the United States, and evaluated and screened for health status based on extensive criteria. MRI scans were acquired according to standard protocols, including high-resolution T1-weighted scans at 1 mm isotropic voxel resolution, as used in the present study. Some participants were studied at one or two follow-up visits to obtain longitudinal data. We selected 138 subjects from the database with T1-weighted scans in the age range of our OSA participants. For subjects who fell within the age range during more than one visit, only one recording was used; the choice of which visit was based on ensuring the best age match with the OSA group.

Analysis

Demographic and sleep variables for the Chicago data were analyzed with independent samples t-tests and Chi-square tests.

For brain image assessments, we used the FreeSurfer and MATLAB-based SPM12 software packages for data processing and analysis. SPM12 was used to process the T1-weighted scans, including removal of signal intensity variations due to field inhomogeneities (35). The processed images were imported into FreeSurfer to assess cortical thickness (27). The initial skull stripping and boundary identification were performed, and skull strips of all subjects were manually checked to ensure no brain area was excluded. Similarly, the pial and gray–white matter boundaries were visually checked and, if needed, edits were made to correct misidentified regions. Such edits include correcting misaligned boundaries such as occasions when the automatic calculation would erroneously identify the “gray matter” edge as in the skull region. Minor edits were required to the automatically detected pial boundaries in most subjects, but the skull strip and white matter boundaries did not require any adjustment.

The FreeSurfer processing stream was followed to generate cortical thickness across the brain, except for cerebellar areas. Cortical thickness was calculated as the distance between gray and white matter surfaces. Thinning is considered to reflect atrophy or neurodegenerative processes (or underdevelopment in children), and thickening can reflect swelling due to inflammation or higher-than normal usage. Surface statistics were implemented to assess cortical thinning or thickening in OSA over control subjects, using a general linear model with group and sex as independent variables. Sex was included to account for possible sex differences in brain structure, but was treated as a covariate of no interest, and was not assessed for effects (“nuisance” variable in FreeSurfer terminology). We also assessed correlations of cortical thickness with DAS scores in participants who had these measures. Each hemisphere was analyzed separately, with 10 mm smoothing. We used a threshold of P ≤ 0.05 with false discovery rate correction (FDR) for multiple comparisons. We overlaid the areas of significant difference onto the inflated cortical surface (sulcal and gyral areas displayed as smooth adjacent regions without depth).

Findings show regions of altered cortical thickness. Such regions are termed “clusters” in reference to the multiple adjacent surface points that comprise the region. A cluster is defined as a group of surface points showing a significant thickening or thinning. Clusters are displayed visually, and quantitative measures of mean thickness and statistical differences are reported for each cluster (reporting by individual surface location would result in excessive numerical values.). The FreeSurfer software matches the locations of clusters to a standard atlas, providing standard labels of the regions affected; these labels are indicated in the results.

Results

Sixteen children with OSA and nine age, sex, ethnicity, and BMI Z-score matched controls underwent sleep studies, neurocognitive testing, and MR imaging. Demographic and polysomnography data for those subjects are listed in Table 1, showing any pertinent differences in sleep characteristics and BMI Z scores.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Polysomnographic and demographic data of 16 obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) and 9 control subjects studied at the University of Chicago.

Whole-brain group comparisons of the 16 OSA patients with a control group consisting of the nine controls and the 138 Pediatric MRI subjects revealed cortical thinning associated with OSA in various areas including the superior and medial frontal, prefrontal and parietal cortices, and the occipital cortex (FDR = 0.05). Regions of significantly thicker or thinner cortex in OSA are shown in Table 2 (left hemisphere) and Table 3 (right hemisphere). Representations of these cortical thickness differences between control and OSA groups in the left and right hemispheres are shown in Figure 1 from multiple views overlaid onto the FreeSurfer template pial surface, and a lateral, inflated view to illustrate otherwise hidden structures (i.e., insular cortex, and sulci).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Left hemisphere clusters of significant cortical thickness differences between obstructive sleep apnea and controls [false discovery rate (FDR)] ≤0.05, corresponding to t = 2.15].

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Right hemisphere clusters of significant cortical thickness differences between obstructive sleep apnea and controls [false discovery rate (FDR) ≤0.05, corresponding to t = 2.15].

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Regions of significant cortical thickness differences between obstructive sleep apnea and controls (FDR ≤0.05). Areas are color-coded according to significance level (t-statistic), with cool colors representing thinning and warm colors thickening (see scale). The top panels show regions overlaid onto the pial surface (gray matter boundary) for clear visualization of structures and gyral differences. The lower panel shows regions overlaid onto an inflated view, whereby all regions including sulci and the insular cortices are visible. Light gray shading represents gyral and dark gray sulci. ACC, anterior cingulate cortex. Quantitative measures for each region are shown in Tables 2 and 3.

There were increases in cortical thickness in the bilateral precentral gyrus, the left central gyrus, and regions in bilateral posterior-mid and right anterior insular cortices (Figure 1; Tables 2 and 3). The posterior cingulate and sub-genu of the anterior cingulate cortices, extending into medial prefrontal areas also showed increased thickness in OSA children, as did the temporal cortex and poles, the latter especially on the right side. The left side showed thickening in the precentral gyrus.

The analysis of DAS scores and cortical thickness performed in the 25 children studies at the University of Chicago did not reveal any significant relationships. The small number of subjects meant that the statistical power and hence sensitivity was low for this analysis.

Discussion

Children with OSA exhibit evidence of significant cortical thinning in the frontal (including the superior frontal gyrus), prefrontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital cortices, all of which are consistent with the gray matter decreases previously reported (16, 3638). Thus, the findings further attest to the presence of neural injury in pediatric OSA, extending earlier indications of injury to cortex-specific areas. These cortical regions play important roles in cognition, executive function, and memory. The frontal lobe is the central region for motor function, problem solving, memory, language, impulse control, and social behavior (39). The prefrontal cortex plays a large role in executive function, attention, and personality development (39). The prefrontal cortex continues to mature into late adolescent years and even early 1920s (40, 41), such that damage at a young age could greatly hinder the normative development processes of higher cognitive functions. Both the parietal and occipital cortices also contribute to cognition and behavior; the parietal cortex through spoken and written word comprehension, encoding, consolidating and retrieving memory, and manipulating working memory (4244) and the occipital cortex through memory, visuoconstruction (the ability to manipulate spatial information), counting and arithmetic, and executive tasks (4547). Therefore if damaged, as cortical thinning would suggest, the frontal, parietal, and occipital cortex could exhibit altered function, affecting neurobehavioral development and functioning.

In adult OSA, cortical thinning is often attributed to cell death or neuronal injury, as thinning constitutes the most likely imaging correlate of dramatic volume reduction in a mature brain (17, 4851). Earlier findings of reduced gray matter volume (16) could reflect both atrophy and vascular or cell damage, including injury with atrophy, whereas cortical thinning is specific to atrophy per se. While direct neuronal injury is a likely event in pediatric OSA due to the intermittent hypoxia and sleep fragmentation that characterize this condition, the effect of OSA on brain maturational processes must also be taken into account. In children ages 5–11 years undergoing normal neural development, the frontal and occipital lobes increase in volume by 0.4–1.5 mm every year (52). Therefore, a disruption of neural developmental processes could be the underlying cause of volume reduction in pediatric OSA as opposed to implicitly signifying the presence of neurodegeneration. Thus, OSA could prevent normal developmental brain volume increases through the same aforementioned mechanisms implicated in neurodegeneration, resulting in thinner cortices in children with OSA than in controls.

Cortical thickening was seen in the precentral and left central gyrus, the medial prefrontal cortex, the mid and anterior insular cortex, the posterior and subgenu of the anterior cingulate cortex, and the medial temporal lobe, areas that are involved in cognitive, emotional, autonomic, pain, and motor function. The insular cortex is involved in emotional control, self-awareness and cognitive functioning and motor control (5358). The anterior cingulate cortex also plays a role in emotions as well as reward, decision-making, and autonomic regulation including blood pressure and heart rate (5961), while the posterior cingulate is involved in human awareness and pain and episodic memory retrieval (6264). The medial temporal lobe is a key region for long-term memory (along with the hippocampus region) (65) and the precentral gyrus is the site of the primary motor cortex and the motor strip and controls voluntary skeletal muscle movement (6668). Cortical alterations in these regions could result in the cognitive, autonomic, memory, and psychological symptoms experienced in OSA, including worse performance on cognitive tasks, hypertension and high sympathetic tone, which could eventually cause cardiovascular issues, memory loss, and depressive symptoms, respectively. Nonetheless, the relatively small number of OSA subjects and the fact that some children had evidence of cognitive deficits while others did not may account for the absence of a significant association between cortical thickness and cognitive test scores.

Increases in cortical thickness could also indicate abnormal neural development in pediatric OSA patients. Synaptic pruning is a normally occurring process that is actively regulated through childhood and adolescence, whereby synapses are removed in order to optimize brain function (52, 69). In children ages 5–11 years, cortical thinning occurs in the right dorsal frontal and bilateral parietals regions by about 0.15–0.30 mm per year as a result of synaptic pruning (52, 69). In pediatric OSA, the present findings suggest the normal process of synaptic pruning may be disrupted. In addition, cortical thickness may also increase due to hypoxia-induced neuro-inflammation and glial activation, indicating an immune response in affected cortical areas. Identifying the exact cause will obviously require further investigation with additional complementary imaging approaches such as diffusion tensor imaging, MR spectroscopy, or functional MRI, which would provide information on alterations in regional neurochemical substrates among pediatric OSA subjects when compared to controls.

The cognitive and behavioral symptoms reported in children with OSA are consistent with symptoms reported by sleep deprivation and disruption studies in children (70). Altered sleep duration and continuity may induce behavioral perturbations in children with sleep-disordered breathing, ultimately favoring the occurrence of dis-maturational processes in the brain and finally manifesting as poor performance in school (71). Although we found no significant associations between the structural findings and the cognitive test performances, the absence of such associations was not surprising considering the large heterogeneity in the prevalence of a cognitive deficit phenotype, which would, therefore, require a markedly larger sample size than the present 25 to enable the detection of such correlation. Neuroimaging studies in particular benefit from greater sample sizes (34). Another consideration is that some cortical structures showed multiple subregions affected; in terms of whether one cortical structure can have meaningfully distinct subregions, we have shown that for one area, the insular cortex, that subregions are functionally distinct (72). Thus, within one structure, the different subregions affected in pediatric OSA may have different neuropsychological consequences. Furthermore, cortical thickness changes may reflect a late-stage of brain alterations (since atrophy occurs over many years), and other measures of structure may detect earlier pathology. Since OSA can go for years undetected, it is unclear how long the participants in our study had the disorder, or how long they neuropsychological performance may have been affected. For example, within the hippocampus, changes in structure measured by water diffusivity are correlated with neuropsychological function (37). Whether such subtle structural changes extend to the cortex is unclear, but assessment of water diffusion specifically within the cortex is warranted for future studies.

In summary, we present evidence of regionally defined cortical thinning and thickening in children with OSA compared to healthy controls. These findings clearly indicate the presence of injury to the brain that reflects either the consequences of neuronal cell losses, the imposition of perturbed maturation brain processes, or a combination thereof in those sites with cortical thinning and hypoxia-induced inflammatory changes in sites with cortical thickening. Further studies evaluating the effect of intervention on these findings are needed.

Ethics Statement

The study was approved by the human ethics committee at the University of Chicago (IRB Protocol # 11-0280-CR004), and written informed consent was obtained from the legal caregiver of each participant in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Assent was obtained from children >7 years old.

Author Contributions

PM performed major components of image data analysis and contributed to manuscript editing. LK-G conceptualized the study, recruited subjects, coordinated the database, analyzed data, and drafted portions of the manuscript. JP performed data analyses. RM performed data analyses. RK performed components of data analyses and interpretation and contributed to manuscript editing. MP performed sleep study scoring and interpretation, coordinated and performed the MRI scans. DG participated in the conceptualizing the study and edited the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Funding

This work was supported by NIH grant HL130984 to LK-G. DG is supported by the Herbert T. Abelson Chair in Pediatrics. MP was supported by a Fellowship Educational grant award from the Kingdom of Saudi Arabia. The funders played no role in the study design, data collection, data analysis, interpretation, and writing of the report.

Footnotes

References

1. Bixler EO, Vgontzas AN, Lin HM, Liao D, Calhoun S, Vela-Bueno A, et al. Sleep disordered breathing in children in a general population sample: prevalence and risk factors. Sleep (2009) 32:731–6. doi:10.1093/sleep/32.6.731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Kheirandish-Gozal L, Gozal D. Sleep Disordered Breathing in Children: A Comprehensive Clinical Guide to Evaluation and Treatment. New York: Humana Press (2012).

Google Scholar

3. Marcus CL, Greene MG, Carroll JL. Blood pressure in children with obstructive sleep apnea. Am J Respir Crit Care Med (1998) 157:1098–103. doi:10.1164/ajrccm.157.4.9704080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Kohyama J, Ohinata JS, Hasegawa T. Blood pressure in sleep disordered breathing. Arch Dis Child (2003) 88:139–42. doi:10.1136/adc.88.1.89

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Amin RS, Carroll JL, Jeffries JL, Grone C, Bean JA, Chini B, et al. Twenty-four-hour ambulatory blood pressure in children with sleep-disordered breathing. Am J Respir Crit Care Med (2004) 169:950–6. doi:10.1164/rccm.200309-1305OC

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Gottlieb DJ, Chase C, Vezina RM, Heeren TC, Corwin MJ, Auerbach SH, et al. Sleep-disordered breathing symptoms are associated with poorer cognitive function in 5-year-old children. J Pediatr (2004) 145:458–64. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2004.05.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. O’Brien LM, Mervis CB, Holbrook CR, Bruner JL, Smith NH, Mcnally N, et al. Neurobehavioral correlates of sleep-disordered breathing in children. J Sleep Res (2004) 13:165–72. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2004.00395.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Giordani B, Hodges EK, Guire KE, Ruzicka DL, Dillon JE, Weatherly RA, et al. Neuropsychological and behavioral functioning in children with and without obstructive sleep apnea referred for tonsillectomy. J Int Neuropsychol Soc (2008) 14:571–81. doi:10.1017/S1355617708080776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Owens JA. Neurocognitive and behavioral impact of sleep disordered breathing in children. Pediatr Pulmonol (2009) 44:417–22. doi:10.1002/ppul.20981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Kheirandish-Gozal L, De Jong MR, Spruyt K, Chamuleau SA, Gozal D. Obstructive sleep apnoea is associated with impaired pictorial memory task acquisition and retention in children. Eur Respir J (2010) 36:164–9. doi:10.1183/09031936.00114209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Hunter SJ, Gozal D, Smith DL, Philby MF, Kaylegian J, Kheirandish-Gozal L. Effect of sleep-disordered breathing severity on cognitive performance measures in a large community cohort of young school-aged children. Am J Respir Crit Care Med (2016) 194:739–47. doi:10.1164/rccm.201510-2099OC

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Weissbluth M, Davis AT, Poncher J, Reiff J. Signs of airway obstruction during sleep and behavioral, developmental, and academic problems. J Dev Behav Pediatr (1983) 4:119–21. doi:10.1097/00004703-198306000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Gozal D. Sleep-disordered breathing and school performance in children. Pediatrics (1998) 102:616–20. doi:10.1542/peds.102.3.616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Galland B, Spruyt K, Dawes P, Mcdowall PS, Elder D, Schaughency E. Sleep disordered breathing and academic performance: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics (2015) 136:e934–46. doi:10.1542/peds.2015-1677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Sakamoto N, Gozal D, Smith DL, Yang L, Morimoto N, Wada H, et al. Sleep duration, snoring prevalence, obesity, and behavioral problems in a large cohort of primary school students in Japan. Sleep (2017) 40. doi:10.1093/sleep/zsw082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Philby MF, Macey PM, Ma RA, Kumar R, Gozal D, Kheirandish-Gozal L. Reduced regional grey matter volumes in pediatric obstructive sleep apnea. Sci Rep (2017) 7:44566. doi:10.1038/srep44566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Macey PM, Henderson LA, Macey KE, Alger JR, Frysinger RC, Woo MA, et al. Brain morphology associated with obstructive sleep apnea. Am J Respir Crit Care Med (2002) 166:1382–7. doi:10.1164/rccm.200201-050OC

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Macey PM, Kumar R, Woo MA, Valladares EM, Yan-Go FL, Harper RM. Brain structural changes in obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep (2008) 31:967–77.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

19. Kumar R, Pham TT, Macey PM, Woo MA, Yan-Go FL, Harper RM. Abnormal myelin and axonal integrity in recently diagnosed patients with obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep (2014) 37:723–32. doi:10.5665/sleep.3578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Yadav SK, Kumar R, Macey PM, Woo MA, Yan-Go FL, Harper RM. Insular cortex metabolite changes in obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep (2014) 37:951–8. doi:10.5665/sleep.3668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Macey PM, Sarma MK, Nagarajan R, Aysola R, Siegel JM, Harper RM, et al. Obstructive sleep apnea is associated with low GABA and high glutamate in the insular cortex. J Sleep Res (2016) 25:390–4. doi:10.1111/jsr.12392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Macey PM, Sarma MK, Prasad JP, Ogren JA, Aysola R, Harper RM, et al. Obstructive sleep apnea is associated with altered midbrain chemical concentrations. Neuroscience (2017) 363:76–86. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Smith DL, Gozal D, Hunter SJ, Philby MF, Kaylegian J, Kheirandish-Gozal L. Impact of sleep disordered breathing on behaviour among elementary school-aged children: a cross-sectional analysis of a large community-based sample. Eur Respir J (2016) 48:1631–9. doi:10.1183/13993003.00808-2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Smith DL, Gozal D, Hunter SJ, Kheirandish-Gozal L. Frequency of snoring, rather than apnea-hypopnea index, predicts both cognitive and behavioral problems in young children. Sleep Med (2017) 34:170–8. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2017.02.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Rakic P. Evolution of the neocortex: a perspective from developmental biology. Nat Rev Neurosci (2009) 10:724–35. doi:10.1038/nrn2719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. O’Brien LM, Tauman R, Gozal D. Sleep pressure correlates of cognitive and behavioral morbidity in snoring children. Sleep (2004) 27:279–82. doi:10.1093/sleep/27.2.279

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage (1999) 9:179–94. doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Montgomery-Downs HE, O’Brien LM, Gulliver TE, Gozal D. Polysomnographic characteristics in normal preschool and early school-aged children. Pediatrics (2006) 117:741–53. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-1067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Berry RB, Budhiraja R, Gottlieb DJ, Gozal D, Iber C, Kapur VK, et al. Rules for scoring respiratory events in sleep: update of the 2007 AASM manual for the scoring of sleep and associated events. Deliberations of the sleep apnea definitions task force of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. J Clin Sleep Med (2012) 8:597–619. doi:10.5664/jcsm.2172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Elliott CD. Differential Ability Scales: Introductory and Technical Handbook. San Antonio: Psychological Corp; Harcourt Brace Jovanovich (1990).

Google Scholar

31. Gozal D, Crabtree VM, Sans Capdevila O, Witcher LA, Kheirandish-Gozal L. C-reactive protein, obstructive sleep apnea, and cognitive dysfunction in school-aged children. Am J Respir Crit Care Med (2007) 176:188–93. doi:10.1164/rccm.200610-1519OC

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Barnes ME, Gozal D, Molfese DL. Attention in children with obstructive sleep apnoea: an event-related potentials study. Sleep Med (2012) 13:368–77. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2011.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Elliott CD. The nature and structure of childrens abilities – evidence from the differential ability scales. J Psychoeduc Assess (1990) 8:376–90. doi:10.1177/073428299000800313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Celle S, Delon-Martin C, Roche F, Barthelemy JC, Pepin JL, Dojat M. Desperately seeking grey matter volume changes in sleep apnea: a methodological review of magnetic resonance brain voxel-based morphometry studies. Sleep Med Rev (2016) 25:112–20. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2015.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage (2005) 26:839–51. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Chan KC, Shi L, So HK, Wang D, Liew AW, Rasalkar DD, et al. Neurocognitive dysfunction and grey matter density deficit in children with obstructive sleep apnoea. Sleep Med (2014) 15:1055–61. doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2014.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Cha J, Zea-Hernandez JA, Sin S, Graw-Panzer K, Shifteh K, Isasi CR, et al. The effects of obstructive sleep apnea syndrome on the dentate gyrus and learning and memory in children. J Neurosci (2017) 37:4280–8. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3583-16.2017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Lv J, Shi L, Zhao L, Weng J, Mok VC, Chu WC, et al. Morphometry analysis of basal ganglia structures in children with obstructive sleep apnea. J Xray Sci Technol (2017) 25:93–9. doi:10.3233/XST-16171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Johnson SB, Blum RW, Giedd JN. Adolescent maturity and the brain: the promise and pitfalls of neuroscience research in adolescent health policy. J Adolesc Health (2009) 45:216–21. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Rubia K, Overmeyer S, Taylor E, Brammer M, Williams SC, Simmons A, et al. Functional frontalisation with age: mapping neurodevelopmental trajectories with fMRI. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2000) 24:13–9. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00055-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW. Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat Neurosci (2003) 6:309–15. doi:10.1038/nn1008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Koenigs M, Barbey AK, Postle BR, Grafman J. Superior parietal cortex is critical for the manipulation of information in working memory. J Neurosci (2009) 29:14980–6. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3706-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Brownsett SL, Wise RJ. The contribution of the parietal lobes to speaking and writing. Cereb Cortex (2010) 20:517–23. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Schiffino FL, Zhou V, Holland PC. Posterior parietal cortex is critical for the encoding, consolidation, and retrieval of a memory that guides attention for learning. Eur J Neurosci (2014) 39:640–9. doi:10.1111/ejn.12417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Hartikainen P, Rasanen J, Julkunen V, Niskanen E, Hallikainen M, Kivipelto M, et al. Cortical thickness in frontotemporal dementia, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis (2012) 30:857–74. doi:10.3233/JAD-2012-112060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Santangelo G, Trojano L, Vitale C, Improta I, Alineri I, Meo R, et al. Cognitive dysfunctions in occipital lobe epilepsy compared to temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neuropsychol (2017) 11(2):277–90. doi:10.1111/jnp.12085

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Zhang H, Chen C, Sun Z, Lin J, Zhou W, Zhou X. Early occipital injury affects numerosity counting but not simple arithmetic. Neurocase (2016) 22:12–21. doi:10.1080/13554794.2015.1023316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Yaouhi K, Bertran F, Clochon P, Mezenge F, Denise P, Foret J, et al. A combined neuropsychological and brain imaging study of obstructive sleep apnea. J Sleep Res (2009) 18:36–48. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00705.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Joo EY, Tae WS, Lee MJ, Kang JW, Park HS, Lee JY, et al. Reduced brain gray matter concentration in patients with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Sleep (2010) 33:235–41. doi:10.1093/sleep/33.2.235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Canessa N, Castronovo V, Cappa SF, Aloia MS, Marelli S, Falini A, et al. Obstructive sleep apnea: brain structural changes and neurocognitive function before and after treatment. Am J Respir Crit Care Med (2011) 183:1419–26. doi:10.1164/rccm.201005-0693OC

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Joo EY, Jeon S, Kim ST, Lee JM, Hong SB. Localized cortical thinning in patients with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome. Sleep (2013) 36:1153–62. doi:10.5665/sleep.2876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Toga AW, Thompson PM, Sowell ER. Mapping brain maturation. Trends Neurosci (2006) 29:148–59. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2006.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage (2002) 16:331–48. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Ackermann H, Riecker A. The contribution of the insula to motor aspects of speech production: a review and a hypothesis. Brain Lang (2004) 89:320–8. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00347-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Critchley HD. Neural mechanisms of autonomic, affective, and cognitive integration. J Comp Neurol (2005) 493:154–66. doi:10.1002/cne.20749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Karnath HO, Baier B, Nagele T. Awareness of the functioning of one’s own limbs mediated by the insular cortex? J Neurosci (2005) 25:7134–8. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1590-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Craig AD. How do you feel – now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci (2009) 10:59–70. doi:10.1038/nrn2555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Quarto T, Blasi G, Maddalena C, Viscanti G, Lanciano T, Soleti E, et al. Association between ability emotional intelligence and left insula during social judgment of facial emotions. PLoS One (2016) 11:e0148621. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0148621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Lane RD, Reiman EM, Axelrod B, Yun LS, Holmes A, Schwartz GE. Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness. Evidence of an interaction between emotion and attention in the anterior cingulate cortex. J Cogn Neurosci (1998) 10:525–35. doi:10.1162/089892998562924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Allman JM, Hakeem A, Erwin JM, Nimchinsky E, Hof P. The anterior cingulate cortex. The evolution of an interface between emotion and cognition. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2001) 935:107–17. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb03476.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Bush G, Vogt BA, Holmes J, Dale AM, Greve D, Jenike MA, et al. Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: a role in reward-based decision making. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2002) 99:523–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.012470999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Maddock RJ, Garrett AS, Buonocore MH. Remembering familiar people: the posterior cingulate cortex and autobiographical memory retrieval. Neuroscience (2001) 104:667–76. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00108-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Nielsen FA, Balslev D, Hansen LK. Mining the posterior cingulate: segregation between memory and pain components. Neuroimage (2005) 27:520–32. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Garrison KA, Santoyo JF, Davis JH, Thornhill TAT, Kerr CE, Brewer JA. Effortless awareness: using real time neurofeedback to investigate correlates of posterior cingulate cortex activity in meditators’ self-report. Front Hum Neurosci (2013) 7:440. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Squire LR, Stark CE, Clark RE. The medial temporal lobe. Annu Rev Neurosci (2004) 27:279–306. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Woolsey CN, Settlage PH, Meyer DR, Sencer W, Pinto Hamuy T, Travis AM. Patterns of localization in precentral and “supplementary” motor areas and their relation to the concept of a premotor area. Res Publ Assoc Res Nerv Ment Dis (1952) 30:238–64.

Google Scholar

67. Kwan HC, Mackay WA, Murphy JT, Wong YC. Spatial organization of precentral cortex in awake primates. II. Motor outputs. J Neurophysiol (1978) 41:1120–31. doi:10.1152/jn.1978.41.5.1120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Graziano MS, Taylor CS, Moore T. Complex movements evoked by microstimulation of precentral cortex. Neuron (2002) 34:841–51. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00698-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Stiles J, Jernigan TL. The basics of brain development. Neuropsychol Rev (2010) 20:327–48. doi:10.1007/s11065-010-9148-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Taveras EM, Rifas-Shiman SL, Bub KL, Gillman MW, Oken E. Prospective study of insufficient sleep and neurobehavioral functioning among school-age children. Acad Pediatr (2017) 17(6):625–32. doi:10.1016/j.acap.2017.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Smith DL, Gozal D, Hunter SJ, Kheirandish-Gozal L. Parent-reported behavioral and psychiatric problems mediate the relationship between sleep-disordered breathing and cognitive deficits in school-aged children. Front Neurol (2017) 8:410. doi:10.3389/fneur.2017.00410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Macey PM, Wu P, Kumar R, Ogren JA, Richardson HL, Woo MA, et al. Differential responses of the insular cortex gyri to autonomic challenges. Auton Neurosci (2012) 168:72–81. doi:10.1016/j.autneu.2012.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sleep disordered breathing, cortex, intermittent hypoxia, atrophy, obstructive sleep apnea, cognitive deficits

Citation: Macey PM, Kheirandish-Gozal L, Prasad JP, Ma RA, Kumar R, Philby MF and Gozal D (2018) Altered Regional Brain Cortical Thickness in Pediatric Obstructive Sleep Apnea. Front. Neurol. 9:4. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00004

Received: 23 September 2017; Accepted: 03 January 2018;
Published: 22 January 2018

Edited by:

Ahmed S. BaHammam, King Saud University, Saudi Arabia

Reviewed by:

Leszek Kubin, University of Pennsylvania, United States
Hiroshi Kadotani, Shiga University of Medical Science, Japan

Copyright: © 2018 Macey, Kheirandish-Gozal, Prasad, Ma, Kumar, Philby and Gozal. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Leila Kheirandish-Gozal, lgozal@peds.bsd.uchicago.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.