Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 27 July 2012
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 6 - 2012 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00224

What can other animals tell us about human social cognition? An evolutionary perspective on reflective and reflexive processing

  • 1Graduate Neuroscience Program, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
  • 2Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
  • 3Center for Translational Social Neuroscience, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
  • 4Department of Philosophy, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, USA
  • 5Decision Neuroscience Laboratory, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, USA
  • 6Beckman Institute for Advanced Science and Technology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA
  • 7Department of Internal Medicine, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, USA
  • 8Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, USA
  • 9Department of Speech and Hearing Science, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, USA
  • 10Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, USA

Human neuroscience has seen a recent boom in studies on reflective, controlled, explicit social cognitive functions like imitation, perspective-taking, and empathy. The relationship of these higher-level functions to lower-level, reflexive, automatic, implicit functions is an area of current research. As the field continues to address this relationship, we suggest that an evolutionary, comparative approach will be useful, even essential. There is a large body of research on reflexive, automatic, implicit processes in animals. A growing perspective sees social cognitive processes as phylogenically continuous, making findings in other species relevant for understanding our own. One of these phylogenically continuous processes appears to be self-other matching or simulation. Mice are more sensitive to pain after watching other mice experience pain; geese experience heart rate increases when seeing their mate in conflict; and infant macaques, chimpanzees, and humans automatically mimic adult facial expressions. In this article, we review findings in different species that illustrate how such reflexive processes are related to (“higher order”) reflexive processes, such as cognitive empathy, theory of mind, and learning by imitation. We do so in the context of self-other matching in three different domains—in the motor domain (somatomotor movements), in the perceptual domain (eye movements and cognition about visual perception), and in the autonomic/emotional domain. We also review research on the developmental origin of these processes and their neural bases across species. We highlight gaps in existing knowledge and point out some questions for future research. We conclude that our understanding of the psychological and neural mechanisms of self-other mapping and other functions in our own species can be informed by considering the layered complexity these functions in other species.

Introduction: How can an Evolutionary Perspective Inform Human Neuroscience?

Students of evolutionary neuroscience may be familiar with the metaphor of an old apartment building for brain evolution. At first, the building is heated by a series of wood-burning fireplaces. Later, a coal-fueled steam system is added in the chimneys and hearths. Later still, an HVAC system is installed, with electrical wiring grafted to the old hot water pipes. Every time something goes wrong with the heat, someone has to determine whether the problem is due to a wiring problem in the HVAC system, damage to the old hot water pipes along which those wires run, or a structural problem in the old chimneys that house the whole apparatus. Like the addition of new heating systems to the apartment building, evolution adds new functions to the brain by building on the pre-existing architecture. Thus the old systems don't disappear: their new functions are integrated with their pre-existing ones, and the continued function of the new systems relies on the soundness of the old ones.

A prominent instantiation of this idea was MacLean's triune brain theory (MacLean, 1990), which posited that instinctual behavior is controlled by the brain's “reptilian complex” (basal ganglia), basic social behavior by the “paleomammalian complex” (limbic system), and higher cognitive function by the “neomammalian complex” (cerebral neocortex). Later anatomical work showed this model to be overly simplistic, but the basic concept of hierarchical processing is echoed by the recent proliferation in dual process models in neuroscience and psychology. Current models tend to make a two-way distinction. One type of system is described as unconscious or preconscious, implicit, automatic, low effort, rapid, perceptually driven, while another is described as conscious, explicit, controlled, high effort, slow, and analytic or reflective [reviewed in Evans (2008)]. For the sake of simplicity, we will refer to the first sort of system as “reflexive” and the second as “reflective,” although this two-way distinction is likely also overly simplistic.

For some time, it was assumed that reflective social cognitive processes were evolutionary “upgrades” unique to humans, or perhaps humans and our closest living relatives, and much behavioral research focused on identifying which skills are “uniquely human” (Evans, 2008; de Waal and Ferrari, 2010). However, there are reasons not to assume that humans' most advanced forms of social cognition lack correlates in other species. Like the upgrades to the apartment heating system, human social cognitive “upgrades” must achieve the same basic purpose as their simpler predecessors—interacting with other individuals in the environment in an adaptive way. Evolution modifies previously existing forms to create new ones (for example, wings are modifications of limbs), and the new forms retain some features of the old ones (bone structure). These adaptations must arise in the context of a previously working social cognitive system, and as such, must incorporate with it. New neural mechanisms must function within the organism's existing social cognitive framework, or else the organism's social behavior will be impaired and its chances of survival will be reduced. Therefore, neural adaptations for new social cognitive functions are likely to involve some of the same neural architecture as preexisting systems.

Furthermore, functions that were once attributed only to humans are increasingly being identified in other species. Thus, reflective social cognition is probably uniquely developed in humans, but not unique to us (Evans, 2008). It is important to remember that all life on earth has been evolving for the same amount of time and the phylogenic tree has no “top.” Differences in function represent adaptation to different niches, not higher or lower position in a scala naturae. A growing number of researchers in the field of comparative behavior stress the explanatory utility of viewing most behavior as phylogenically continuous (de Waal and Ferrari, 2010), a position that was espoused by Darwin (1872).

All of this argues that studying animals can tell us something about human social cognition. Human neuroscience is currently very interested in the brain's “most modern upgrades”—reflective processes like theory of mind, or thinking about what another person is thinking (Premack and Woodruff, 1978), as well as related processes like imitation, perspective-taking, and empathy. Understanding these functions is relevant for understanding and treating disorders of social cognition like autism in which they are impaired. But like the heating in the old apartment building, these functions aren't stand-alone systems. Deficits in the higher level functions may even be due to underlying, less obvious deficits in the lower level functions. In such cases understanding the interplay between higher- and lower-level functions is essential for understanding and treating deficits and disease affecting higher level social functions.

In this review, we explore the interplay between higher- and lower-level functions, as well as the question of what in particular the study of animals can tell us about human social cognition. We do so in the context of self-other matching, defined as any phenomenon in which the observation of another's behavior or state causes the observer's behavior or state to become congruent with it. We have chosen this domain for several reasons. First, the operational definition allows phenomena to be categorized by easily observable output. In many species, comparable behavioral data is available but data about underlying physiology or neural substrates is not (or it is available but contentious, as in the question of whether human imitation involves or relies on the mirror system). Grouping results by behavioral output allows for cross-species comparisons without any a priori perspective about underlying physiological processes. We will, however, draw connections to underlying physiological and neural substrates when possible. Second, self-other matching can occur in a reflexive manner, but this reflexive processing can have measurable effects on reflective processes. Third, self-other matching phenomena are present in varying degrees of complexity across a wide range of phyla. In this review, we limit our scope to vertebrates. We focus heavily on primates, since they are most closely related to humans and also the subject of a large body of comparative research, but we also discuss some research in canids, rodents, birds, and reptiles. In humans, self-other matching encompasses phenomena like motor resonance, mimicry, imitation, emulation, empathy, and perspective taking (defined in Table 1), which likely rely on partially discrete and partially overlapping neural and psychological mechanisms. Comparing which of these functions are present in which other species can help us to structure our thinking about the organization of these processes within our own species.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Terms and definitions.

Experimental Results: A Compendium of Self-Other Matching Phenomena Across Species

Self-Other Matching in the Motor Domain: Somatomotor Movements

Somatomotor self-other matching can occur at a reflexive level via motor resonance. Motor resonance is a general idea implicating the activation of common neural or psychological substrates for observed and executed action—e.g., observing another's action causes my motor system to “resonate” with theirs. When motor resonance causes the overt output of an observed action, this is termed “motor contagion”. A well-known example of motor contagion occurs during infancy. For a brief period in development, neonatal macaques, humans, and chimpanzees copy observed orofacial movements (Meltzoff and Moore, 1977, 1983; Heimann et al., 1989; Myowa-Yamakoshi et al., 2004; Ferrari et al., 2006; Bard, 2007; Ferrari et al., 2009a,b; Paukner et al., 2011). Human infants also copy observed finger movements (Nagy et al., 2005). This effect disappears sometime around age 2 weeks in macaques, 2 months in chimpanzees, and 3 months in humans (Meltzoff and Moore, 1977, 1983; Heimann et al., 1989; Myowa-Yamakoshi et al., 2004; Ferrari et al., 2006). The fact that this period lasts longer in humans may be relevant to species differences in adult social cognition, although this idea awaits exploration. In adult humans, motor contagion in everyday social interactions is sometimes called the “chameleon effect”—the tendency to mimic others' postures, mannerisms, facial expressions, and behaviors. It increases liking, smoothes social interactions, and is more common in empathic people (Chartrand and Bargh, 1999). Orangutans spontaneously and rapidly mimic facial expressions during play (Davila Ross et al., 2008), chimpanzees experience contagion for aggressive and affliative social interactions (Videan et al., 2005), and macaques are more likely to eat when seeing or hearing another monkey eat (Ferrari et al., 2005). In Paukner et al. (2011), human experimenters imitated capuchin monkeys' actions on a ball, such as poking or mouthing it. The monkeys later preferred to spend more time in proximity to imitator versus non-imitator humans, and also preferred to interact with them in a task where tokens could be exchanged for food. This suggests that motor contagion may play a role in their naturalistic social interactions and may be important for establishing affiliative relationships and prosocial behavior.

In addition to facilitating the production of actions congruent to others', motor resonance can interfere with the production of non-congruent actions. This is termed “motor interference” and is measured by a reduction in movement accuracy while observing a non-congruent movement. In humans, motor interference appears around age 4–5, is influenced by prior knowledge or experience of the individual performing the observed action, is weakened by self-focus, and is stronger when the subject has practiced the observed action and when the demonstrator is similar to the subject (Marshall et al., 2010; Saby et al., 2011). Observing a sinusoidal arm movement interferes with the observer's own movement more if the observed movement is directed toward a goal, suggesting that goal directed actions are more contagious than non-goal-directed actions (Bouquet et al., 2010). To our knowledge, motor interference has not been studied in other species, although like motor resonance, it seems to be an easily addressable topic. For example, in a paradigm used to study reach-to-grasp movements, macaque monkeys grasp a bar in an apparatus that measures the force, velocity, and direction of their arm movements [e.g., (Kalaska et al., 1989)]. This could be used to measure perturbations to a monkey's movements while watching congruent versus incongruent movements by another monkey.

In humans, evidence for a shared physiological basis of action execution and observation at a low level comes from electrophysiological experiments. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to motor cortex can be used to produce motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) in the periphery—e.g., stimulation to the thumb area of primary motor cortex evokes a measurable electrophysiological effect in the thumb muscles. In these TMS experiments, MEPs are greater during observation of movements involving those muscles; this effect occurs for both goal-directed and non-goal directed movements (Fadiga et al., 1995; Maeda et al., 2002).

Furthermore, the timecourse of MEPs follows the timecourse of the observed action, showing that the human motor system matches the individual, component movements of an observed action (Gangitano et al., 2001). Additionally, electrical stimulation to a nerve produces activation (twitching) in monosynaptically-connected muscle fibers, called the H-reflex. Baldissera et al. (2001) elicited H-reflexes from flexor finger muscles while subjects viewed a hand either opening or closing. Activation of the flexor muscles was greater when subjects observed a hand opening, which is the opposite of what occurs during actual hand-opening execution (flexors close the hand) and also opposite to the resonant excitability that occurs through stimulation at the level of the cortex (i.e., the TMS experiments above). This implies that motor resonance in the brain is somehow inhibited in the periphery. Because the H-reflex is known to be monosynaptic, this indicates that this inhibition occurs at the level of the spinal cord.

Human electrophysiology experiments have also found a shared basis for action execution and observation. Humans show suppression of sensorimotor cortical EEG rhythms during both action observation and execution, measurable with either EEG or MEG (Pineda, 2005; Hari, 2006). This occurs during observation of facial expressions as well as both transitive and intransitive limb movements and is distributed somatotopically over sensorimotor cortex according to the body part being observed (Muthukumaraswamy and Johnson, 2004; Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2004, 2006; Oberman et al., 2005; Moore et al., 2011). The effect is stronger for reach-to-grasp actions that are directed toward an object than those that are not (Muthukumaraswamy and Johnson, 2004; Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2004).

These types of TMS, EEG, and MEG experiments have not been performed in macaques, but single-cell recordings show that mirror neurons in ventral premotor area F5 and inferior parietal areas PF/PFG respond to both the execution and observation of similar movements, including both manual actions and orofacial movements (Gallese et al., 1996; Rizzolatti et al., 1996; Ferrari et al., 2003). However, macaque mirror neurons only respond to observed manual actions which are object- or goal-directed; they do not respond to observed mimed (intransitive) actions (Gallese et al., 1996; Rizzolatti et al., 1996; Ferrari et al., 2003). The human homologues of macaque F5 and PF/PFG are Brodman areas 44 and 40 (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). In neuroimaging experiments, these regions are active during observation and execution of similar actions in a somatotopic manner (Buccino et al., 2001). Motor contagion in humans has been proposed to rely on a mirror system homologous to that in macaques (Blakemore and Frith, 2005). If this is true, then motor contagion and motor interference should occur in macaques (as well as any other species that have a mirror system), although to our knowledge this has not been tested.

In addition to the reflexive phenomena described above, individuals can also copy each other's behavior in a less automatic, more controlled manner. Many species are capable of using observational learning to copy another's goal-directed action. Rats can learn to run a maze by observing another rat (Zentall and Levine, 1972). Some birds socially learn each other's songs (Zentall, 2004). Guppy fish can socially learn foraging innovations (Laland and Reader, 1999). Wild macaques learn to wash sand off sweet potatoes by watching other macaques (Kawamura, 1959). Both capuchin monkeys and chimpanzees learn to use tools by watching conspecifics (Fragaszy and Visalberghi, 1989; Inoue-Nakamura and Matsuzawa, 1997).

Undoubtedly, not all of these phenomena need to be understood as involving reflective processing. When considering the impressive variety of social learning across species, it is important to recognize that the same general function—copying another's behavior—can result from different psychological and neurophysiological mechanisms in different species. Various schemas exist for categorizing different types of social learning behavior [e.g., (Whiten et al., 2004; Zentall, 2006)]. In general, the types of social learning behavior that are most widespread across species do not involve a representational understanding of the goal behind an observed action; for example, observers' attention may be drawn to particular objects or locations in the environment, facilitating their own independent discovery of how to produce an action involving that object (stimulus enhancement); they may learn about the positive or negative value of an object or event (valence learning); or they may reflexively copy aspects an observed action's movements without reflective understanding of its goal (mimicry). Many of these behavioral phenomena may occur reflexively, without representational understanding of the observed action's goal.

Forms of controlled social learning that involve an understanding of the observed goal are more rare, but are well-studied in primates. Most primate social learning is classed as emulation (copying an action's goal or result but not specific movements or methods) rather than imitation (copying both the goal and methods) (Whiten et al., 2009). While some studies report imitation in other species [e.g., chimpanzees: (Hayes and Hayes, 1952; Custance et al., 1995; Horner and Whiten, 2005); marmosets: (Voelkl and Huber, 2000)], none of these species use it so profusely and complexly as humans. In particular, a decades-long body of behavioral research describes a bias toward emulation in chimpanzees, and a bias toward imitation in humans (Whiten et al., 2009). For example, in one task (Horner and Whiten, 2005), the experimenter demonstrates a complex series of actions that open a puzzle box (pulling levers, pressing buttons, etc.). When the puzzle box is opaque and the relationship between these maneuverings and the opening of the box is not perceptible, both chimpanzees and human children copy these actions with high fidelity. However, if a transparent box is used, it becomes obvious that some of the demonstrator's actions do not contribute to opening the box. Chimpanzees dispense with these useless actions and use the most efficient method to open the box. Human children, on the other hand, persist with these actions, even when instructed not to reproduce any “useless” or “silly” actions, and even when they verbally report that they understand that they are useless (Lyons et al., 2007). This is termed “overimitation,” and it is even stronger in adults than in children (McGuigan et al., 2011).

Developmentally, copying of goal directed actions emerges in humans improves over the first two years of life, and in chimpanzees during the first four years (Inoue-Nakamura and Matsuzawa, 1997; Elsner, 2007; Elsner et al., 2007). Human infants are more likely to reproduce actions that have goals than those that do not (Elsner, 2007), and when preschool children copy a goal-directed movement, they tend to use movements that are less congruent with the demonstrator's than if there is no goal (Bekkering et al., 2000). It is interesting to note that motor interference effects are not observable until the age of four to five years (Marshall et al., 2010; Saby et al., 2011), suggesting that motor resonance, which would otherwise cause interference, may be somehow damped during the time that goal-directed copying is developing. However, children show electrophysiological correlates of motor resonance (mu suppression) as early as 6 months and seem to do so throughout development (Lepage and Theoret, 2006; Nystrom, 2008; van Elk et al., 2008). An important area of future research will be the developmental relationship of reflexive motor resonance phenomena with more controlled social learning phenomena.

To date, the neural correlates of goal-directed behavioral copying have only been studied in humans. In humans, two recent meta-analyses of functional neuroimaging studies on imitation found that it involves the homologues of the macaque mirror regions (Brodman areas 44 and 40), as well as broader regions of superior parietal lobe, inferior parietal lobe, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and premotor cortex (Caspers et al., 2010; Molenberghs et al., 2011). Lesions to either frontal or parietal regions can cause apraxia, a neuropsychological disorder of imitation (Goldenberg, 2009). While the macaque mirror system is activated by the observation of goal-directed actions, notably, monkeys do not imitate according to the definition above (copying both goal and method; Fragaszy and Visalberghi, 2004). However, macaques do recognize when their goal-directed actions are being imitated by a human experimenter (Paukner et al., 2005). Even accepting a looser definition of imitation, it is obvious that macaques' social learning is less profuse and less complex than humans'. Furthermore, the macaque mirror system does not respond to meaningless actions not directed at an object, e.g., mimed grasping, while the human mirror system does (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). This suggests that species differences in the mirror system could be related to species differences in social learning.

One recent study examined the white matter connectivity of the mirror system in macaques, chimps, and humans (Hecht et al., 2012). In macaques and chimps, the bulk of the white matter within the mirror system connects temporal perceptual areas directly to the frontal mirror region and surrounding frontal areas. Since the frontal mirror region is thought to contain a “vocabulary of motor acts” where actions are coded according to their goals or results (Rizzolatti et al., 1988; Bonini et al., 2009), this pathway might underlie macaques' and chimps' bias toward copying an action's results over its movements. In humans, relatively more white matter in the mirror system passes through parietal cortex. Since the parietal mirror region is thought to perform sensorimotor mapping of the spatial and temporal details of observed and executed movements (Rozzi et al., 2008; Bonini et al., 2009), this increased connectivity might allow humans to map observed actions onto their own motor systems with greater kinematic detail, and could be related to our propensity for “overimitation.”

Taken together, research on phylogeny, development, and neural activation suggests that self-other mapping in the somatomotor domain can occur via both reflexive and reflective processes. A reflexive mechanism is in place very early whereby observed movements are automatically reproduced. After a short period—days, weeks, or months depending on the species (with unknown implications of this difference)—an inhibitory process comes online and this automatic mimicry disappears. In human adults, this inhibition seems to be mediated by the spinal cord, perhaps leaving the brain free to mirror observed action uninhibitedly (Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). This direct, low level self-other matching mechanism is thought to result from simple Hebbian synaptic potentiation during development: an individual's own action causes motor and visual neurons to “fire together,” increasing the chances that they will eventually “wire together,” so that after repeated co-activation, activation in one neuron alone can cause activation in the other, creating neurons that activate in response to observed, unexecuted action (Keysers and Perrett, 2004; Brass and Heyes, 2005). Such a mechanism should be widespread across phylogeny, might account for the development of premotor/parietal mirror neurons as well as other, heterogeneous cell types, and might account for motor contagion and mimicry across various species.

On the other hand, a reflective mechanism allowing the reproduction of goal-directed actions emerges later in development and is more limited across phylogeny. In humans, it involves some of the same neural substrates as reflexive motor resonance, as well as other regions more commonly associated with reflective processing, like dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and superior parietal cortex (Caspers et al., 2010; Molenberghs et al., 2011; Koenigs et al., 2009; Barbey et al., 2012a,b). A sub-distinction can be made between copying actions' results versus movements; humans focus on copying movements, while chimpanzees and other primates focus on copying goals. This difference in behavior may be the result of an underlying difference in neural responsivity (whether the mirror system can respond to intransitive action), which itself may be a result of a difference in white matter connectivity (the amount of connectivity with parietal cortex) (Hecht et al., 2012). The idea that copying results and copying movements are semi-dissociable processes is supported by clinical evidence. Goldenberg (2009) argues that lesions to frontal cortex in humans impair imitation of goal-directed actions, while lesions to parietal cortex impair imitation of non-goal-directed, meaningless actions. Furthermore, non-goal-directed imitation may be specifically impaired in autism (Gowen et al., 2008). (Paulus et al., 2011) suggest that developmentally, motor resonance is necessary but not sufficient for social learning of goal directed actions. This holds across phylogeny: reflexive motor resonance and mimicry are seen across a wide variety of species, and seem to be necessary but not sufficient for the development of social learning involving a reflective understanding of observed goals, which is more rare across phylogeny.

Self-Other Matching in the Perceptual Domain: Eye Movements and Cognition About Perception

Individuals can match their own visual perception or attention to that of another by following gaze direction (Emery et al., 1997). It is easy to see how gaze following is a broadly adaptive trait—if something has drawn my conspecific's attention, it likely deserves my attention as well, since we share food sources, predators, prey, and potential mating partners. Bringing one's own perception into congruence with another individual's can also serve as first step toward bringing behavior into congruence. Therefore, it is not surprising that this basic behavior occurs automatically across the animal kingdom, in various species of reptiles, birds, and mammals.

In its simplest form, gaze following is tested by having the subject view a conspecific or human experimenter looking up, down, or to the side, and measuring whether the subject performs a congruent adjustment in visual attention. This test is passed by tortoises, a variety of birds, domestic goats, dogs and wolves, and a variety of primates (Bugnyar et al., 2004; Schloegl et al., 2008; Loretto et al., 2009; Rosati and Hare, 2009; Wilkinson et al., 2010; Kehmeier et al., 2011; Range and Viranyi, 2011; Teglas et al., 2012). Some species, such as macaques (Emery et al., 1997), only follow shifts in head or whole body orientation, while others, such as chimpanzees (Tomasello et al., 2007), can follow shifts in eye gaze alone. Humans' white sclera make our eye movements more apparent than other species', who have darker sclera; this is thought to be a contributing factor in our ability to follow eye movements (Tomasello et al., 2007; Rosati and Hare, 2009).

In a more complex version of this task, the demonstrator individual looks toward an object that is occluded from the subject's view by a barrier. Animals that can pass this task are said to follow gaze “geometrically” and are inferred to have some referential understanding of the content of the demonstrator's perception—i.e., that the demonstrator is “looking at” a particular thing. Animals that fail this task are taken to lack the ability to take the visual perspective of others (Rosati and Hare, 2009). Species currently known to follow gaze geometrically include a subset of those above: spider monkeys and capuchins (Amici et al., 2009), chimpanzees, bonobos, and gorillas (Okamoto-Barth et al., 2007), dogs (Teglas et al., 2012), wolves (Range and Viranyi, 2011), rooks (Schloegl et al., 2008), and ravens (Bugnyar et al., 2004).

In a yet more complex task, perspective-taking is studied in humans and great apes using tasks that test the subject's ability to know that another individual does not know something that the subject does. For example, in the Sally-Anne test (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985), Sally places a toy in her basket and then leaves the room. Anne then enters the room and moves the toy. The subject is asked where Sally will look for her toy when she returns. This measures whether the subject has “theory of mind,” or the ability to attribute mental states or perspectives to others which are separate from one's own (Premack and Woodruff, 1978). Thus it is an explicit measure of a reflective process. However, there is evidence that implicit processing is also involved in this task. Both human adults and children are less accurate at judgments about their own visual perspective when there is another person present with a different physical perspective, suggesting that we reflexively map what others can see and that this uses the same cognitive machinery as awareness of what we can see (Samson et al., 2010; Surtees and Apperly, 2012). Human infants look longer at the correct answer in a Sally-Anne test before they can produce a correct explicit verbal response, suggesting that they have implicit awareness of others' perceptual knowledge (Clements and Perner, 1994). Various experiments suggest that chimpanzees are able to take the perspective of others (Povinelli et al., 1990; Hare et al., 2001; Brauer et al., 2007; Krachun and Call, 2009; Krachun et al., 2009). For example, in one study (Hare et al., 2001), subordinate chimpanzees preferred to approach food behind a barrier, so that a dominant chimpanzee could not see.

The complexity of gaze following behavior changes across development, and this differs between species. In humans, gaze following emerges between 3–18 months (Scaife and Bruner, 1975; Carpenter et al., 1998; Corkum and Moore, 1998). In rhesus macaques, it begins to emerge around 5.5 months; in chimpanzees, between 3–4 years (Rosati and Hare, 2009). At first, infants follow head movements but not eye movements alone, and continue to follow a demonstrator's repeated gazes toward an information-less target (such as a blank ceiling). This suggests a lack of understanding that eyes are the mechanism of perception, and that gaze following behavior is relatively inflexible, automatic, and not affected by learning. Later, infants begin to follow eye movements alone, and later still they can inhibit repeated gaze-follows to a meaningless target. The ability to follow gaze geometrically emerges around this time. This pattern of development is similar in wolves, macaques, chimpanzees, and humans (Scaife and Bruner, 1975; Carpenter et al., 1998; Corkum and Moore, 1998; Ferrari et al., 2000; Rosati and Hare, 2009; Range and Viranyi, 2011).

The neural basis of gaze following has been studied in humans and macaques. In humans, neuroimaging experiments have implicated the superior temporal sulcus, cuneus, inferior parietal lobule, and intraparietal sulcus in perceiving others' looking direction (Puce et al., 1998; Wicker et al., 1998; Hoffman and Haxby, 2000; Pelphrey et al., 2003, 2004; Materna et al., 2008). Superior temporal sulcus is involved in encoding intentions related to gaze (Pelphrey et al., 2003), while intraparietal sulcus may be related to shifts in one's own visual attention regardless of social context (Materna et al., 2008). In macaques, cells in superior temporal sulcus respond to different angles of head orientation (Perrett et al., 1991). Cells in area LIP of the intraparietal sulcus fire both when the monkey looks in the cell's preferred direction and when another monkey looks in the same direction (Shepherd et al., 2009). A second population of cells in this area was suppressed by the observation of other monkeys' gaze. Interestingly, most of F5 mirror neurons are tuned to a particular visual perspective for observed grasping movements, suggesting a role for perspective in the somatomotor self-other matching system (Caggiano et al., 2011).

Considering the neural and behavioral research together across phylogeny, some patterns emerge. There are no species that are capable of following eye movements alone but not head movements, or head movements but not whole body movements. Developmentally, across species, the ability to follow eye movements alone emerges after the ability to follow head or body movements. Additionally, there are no species that follow gaze behind a barrier but not into empty space, and following gaze into empty space always emerges in development before following gaze around a barrier. The ability to follow gaze geometrically co-emerges with the ability to not follow repeated gazes toward an informationless target, such as a blank ceiling. Thus it appears that there are two fairly discrete components to gaze following: an early-developing, egocentric, automatic one, and a later-developing, allocentric, controlled one that takes into account the referential information in the gaze.

It seems likely that these components might rely on at least partially separable neural substrates. Shepherd et al. (2009) suggest that LIP cells are involved in the reflexive mode of gaze following. Similarly, Pelphrey et al. (2003) suggest that human intraparietal sulcus is concerned with egocentric mapping of spatial attention. This suggests the hypothesis that the automatic, implicit mode of gaze following can be mapped to parietal cortex. We wonder whether Shepherd et al. (2009) second population of cells that were suppressed by observed gaze changes might serve to override this automatic “mirroring” of attention, and whether the onset of their inhibition during development might coincide with the onset of the ability to habituate to meaningless gazes. Conversely, Pelphrey et al. (2003) suggest that in humans, the superior temporal sulcus may be more involved with judging the intentionality of others' actions, and has been implicated more broadly in reflective social cognitive processes like theory of mind. Thus we can hypothesize that this region might underlie the referential understanding of the content of others' gaze.

Self-Other Matching in the Autonomic/Emotional Domain

In addition to the somatomotor and oculomotor domains, self-other matching also occurs in the autonomic domain. This can extend to very low-level functions, such as pupil size (Harrison et al., 2006, 2007, 2009) and respiration (Jeannerod and Frak, 1999; Paccalin and Jeannerod, 2000; Mulder et al., 2005; Kuroda et al., 2011). “Contagion” of autonomic states has been well studied across species in the domain of pain, fear, and anxiety. For example, geese have heart rate increases after viewing their mate in conflict (Wascher et al., 2010). Mice have stronger responses to pain after viewing another mouse in pain (Langford et al., 2006; Jeon et al., 2010; Jeon and Shin, 2011). Monkeys exhibit behavioral signs of fear when watching another monkey in fear, even when the observer cannot see the item that is feared (Mineka and Cook, 1993). Crying is contagious in human infants (Geangu et al., 2010). In adult humans, photographs of others in danger or pain induces a freezing postural response (Azevedo et al., 2005; Facchinetti et al., 2006).

Beyond simply “catching” the emotion of fear non-referentially, various species can learn what to fear by watching others through observational learning. For example, in an experiment with crows, adult crows were captured, banded, and released by human experimenters who wore distinctive masks. The offspring of these adult crows, who observed the masked experimenters' actions, later produced alarm calls to humans wearing the same masks, even though they had no interaction with the humans personally (Cornell et al., 2011). Similarly, monkeys can acquire fear of snakes after watching other monkeys' fearful interactions with snakes, without any personal experience with snakes (Cook and Mineka, 1989, 1990). When human adults observe others undergoing a panic attack after a conditioned stimulus, they show greater electrodermal responses and report more fear and anxiety for that stimulus (Kelly and Forsyth, 2007). In humans, observational learning of fear, like Pavlovian conditioning, subsequently produces increased skin conductance measurements in response to a masked (nonconsciously viewed) image, while simple verbal instruction that an item is dangerous does not (Olsson and Phelps, 2004). This suggests that observational learning of fear acts via a reflexive, implicit mechanism rather than a controlled, explicit mechanism.

Individuals of various species can also learn what not to fear by watching others. Attenuation of fear by observational learning has been reported in mice (Guzman et al., 2009), and extinction of avoidance behavior is facilitated by observational learning in rats (Uno et al., 1973). Monkeys that observe other monkeys behaving non-fearfully with snakes are less likely to acquire fear of snakes themselves, and overshadowing can also be achieved through observational learning in monkeys (Mineka and Cook, 1986; Cook and Mineka, 1987). Human children who see their mothers responding positively to a fear-inducing stimulus are less fearful of the stimulus (Gerull and Rapee, 2002; Egliston and Rapee, 2007). For human children learning to overcome a fear of swimming, swimming lessons are more effective when paired with observation of a non-fearful child swimming (Weiss et al., 1998).

Self-other matching for autonomic states seems to rely on the same neural structures that produce those states in the observer. In mice, observational fear learning is blocked by inactivation of the anterior cingulate or the thalamic pain nuclei (both regions involved in the experience of pain), but not thalamic sensory nuclei (Jeon et al., 2010). In humans, felt and seen pain activate anterior cingulate and anterior insula (Lamm et al., 2010). Felt and seen disgust also activate the insula (Wicker et al., 2003; Wright et al., 2004; Jabbi et al., 2008). The amygdala seems to be necessary for not only the experience of fear, but also the perception of fear in others—Adolph's famous patient SM, who suffered bilateral calcification of the amygdala, is both unable to experience fear personally and has difficulty attributing it to others (Adolphs et al., 1994; Feinstein et al., 2010).

Another example of automatic, reflexive self-other matching in this domain is facial expressions. As mentioned previously, orofacial movements are automatically imitated for a brief postnatal period in macaques, chimpanzees, and humans (Meltzoff and Moore, 1977, 1983; Heimann et al., 1989; Myowa-Yamakoshi et al., 2004; Ferrari et al., 2006, 2009a,b; Paukner et al., 2011), and adult orangutans rapidly mimic facial expressions during play (Davila Ross et al., 2008), but no other studies have assessed involuntary facial mimicry in adult animals. In adult humans, viewing another individual's facial expression causes rapid facial reactions, or brief, reflexive, low-intensity mimicry of the expression in one's own face, measureable with EMG (Dimberg and Thunberg, 1998). This occurs even when stimuli are presented to the blind hemisphere of patients with unilateral visual cortex lesions, so it does not require cortical awareness (Tamietto et al., 2009). Interfering with this ability reduces emotion detection accuracy—subjects are less accurate at naming happy facial expressions when holding a pencil in their mouth (Oberman et al., 2007), lesions to somatosensory cortex impair facial expression recognition (Adolphs et al., 2000), and Botox injections decrease emotion recognition across multiple expressions (Neal and Chartrand, 2011). Furthermore, the application of a restricting gel to facial skin, which increases feedback signals, increases emotion perception accuracy (Neal and Chartrand, 2011). This suggests that some part of this implicit, automatic mimicry is informational—i.e., facial feedback from the mimicked expression activates neural representations about the meaning of the expression. However, facial expressions, face-voice combinations, and body expressions all evoke similar EMG responses in the face, suggesting that humans also resonate with the affective meaning of expressions and not just the motor pattern (Magnee et al., 2007).

Motor resonance and contagion for facial expressions seems to rely on some of the same mechanisms as motor resonance and contagion for somatomotor movements. While viewing facial expressions, neonatal macaques show mu suppression, thought to be an EEG index of mirror neuron activity (Ferrari et al., 2012). Adult macaques activate frontal mirror neurons during the observation of facial expressions (Ferrari et al., 2003). Human children (Dapretto et al., 2006) and adults (Molenberghs et al., 2011) activate inferior frontal gyrus, the homologue of macaque F5, during the observation of facial expressions, and also show mu suppression during facial expression observation (Oberman et al., 2005; Moore et al., 2011). Interestingly, infant macaques who imitate facial gestures have more developed reaching-grasping behavior and fine motor control in the hand than their conspecifics who do not, providing further evidence that this phenomenon is linked to motor resonance in the somatomotor domain (Ferrari et al., 2009b).

Yawns are a specific example of a contagious facial expression that is contagious in several species. In addition to humans, macaques (Paukner and Anderson, 2006), gelada baboons (Palagi et al., 2009), chimpanzees (Anderson et al., 2004; Campbell et al., 2009; Campbell and de Waal, 2011), and dogs (Joly-Mascheroni et al., 2008; Harr et al., 2009) also experience contagious yawning. In humans, viewing others' yawns activates precuneus, posterior cingulate, and superior temporal sulcus, all regions that have been associated with “higher-level” forms of social cognition (Platek et al., 2005; Schurmann et al., 2005). Platek (2010) notes that individual humans who are more susceptible to contagious yawning tend to be better at higher-order social cognitive measures like theory of mind processing and self-face recognition, and suggests that yawn contagion may be an evolutionarily old processes that became the basis for these more complex forms of social cognition.

In addition to self-other matching of autonomic states and facial expressions, others' emotions can also be matched in a more explicit, reflective manner. Preston and de Waal (2002) use the term “cognitive empathy” to describe a referential understanding of another's emotional state. Several studies show a link between reflective and reflexive self-other matching of emotion. Subjects who score high in emotional empathy scales have stronger facial mimicry for observed emotions, while low-empathy subjects activate facial muscles incongruent with the observed expression—e.g., “smiling” when seeing an angry face (Sonnby-Borgstrom, 2002). Similarly, high-empathy subjects show greater contagion for pupil size (Harrison et al., 2007). Autism and schizophrenia, both disorders which impair higher-order measures of empathizing, involve abnormal facial mimicry of observed facial expressions (McIntosh et al., 2006; Oberman et al., 2009; Varcin et al., 2010) and a reduction in yawn contagion (Haker and Rossler, 2009; Helt et al., 2010). A better understanding of the interaction between reflexive and reflective forms of emotional self-other matching may provide new directions for treatment in disorders of social cognition, since some problems in higher level social cognition and emotional response might derive from deficits in lower-level, reflexive self-other matching systems.

Another broad area of inquiry for future research is the interaction between self-other matching in the emotional domain with self-other matching in other domains. These interactions undoubtedly exist. For example, in the motor domain, as noted earlier, mimicry of postures, mannerisms, facial expressions, and behaviors increases liking, smoothes social interactions, and is more common in empathic people the chameleon effect (Chartrand and Bargh, 1999; Paulus et al., 2011). This brings up the interesting question of whether targeting or training self-other matching in the somatomotor domain (or another domain) might improve self-other matching in the emotional domain. Given that something like the chameleon effect seems to occur in capuchin monkeys, since monkeys prefer to interact with humans who imitate them (Paukner et al., 2009), research on this topic in other species might be useful for understanding it in our own.

General Discussion and Conclusion

In this review, we have aimed to provide specific examples of how reflective processes are related to reflexive processes in self-other matching across species in three specific domains—in the motor domain (somatomotor movements), in the perceptual domain (eye movements and cognition about visual perception), and in the autonomic/emotional domain. Many unanswered questions remain; we have highlighted a few specific questions, with some potential ways to address them, in Table 2. Despite these unanswered questions, taking a broader perspective and considering these domains together, several patterns emerge.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Some unanswered questions for future research, with some suggestions for ways to address them.

First, in each of these domains, there are early-developing, automatic processes that rapidly match the observer's state to others'. These could emerge based on a simple Hebbian mechanism, as individuals learn associations between observable effects and internal states within the context of their own behavior. As these associations are solidified, observation of only the process's effect (a fearful expression or the perception of an arm movement) can activate representations of the internal state that causes it (the emotion of fear or the motor representation of the arm movement). Since Hebbian learning is a common feature of nervous systems, seen even in mollusks, this type of self-other matching is likely widespread across the animal kingdom.

Second, more complex forms of self-other matching in each domain emerge later in development and are less prevalent across phylogeny. They involve some of the same neural substrates as their related lower-level processes, as well as other neural systems associated with representational thought. The function of the lower-level processes can impact higher-level processes. For example, paralysis of one's own facial muscles impairs recognitions of others' facial expressions (Neal and Chartrand, 2011). In general, these higher-level functions seem not to be present in species that lack the underlying lower-level functions—e.g., to date there are no species that are capable of geometric gaze following but not the simpler form of automatic gaze following into empty space. Many of these higher-level functions are uniquely developed in humans, and some may even be completely unique to humans. However, the longer that comparative psychology investigates which behaviors are uniquely human, the more once-unique functions are found in other species.

Third, the proper function and development of the lower-level systems is often critical for the proper function and later development of the higher-level systems. Because these higher-level functions like imitation, perspective taking, and empathy are more immediately observable and salient, social cognitive deficits are often attributed to dysfunctions in these higher-level functions, but it is important to also address whether there may be a less obvious deficit in an underlying lower-level function. For example, autism was once accepted as primarily a disorder of theory of mind (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985). More recent research, though, has shown that high-functioning autistic individuals can pass tests of theory of mind, albeit using different mechanisms. Current research is increasingly pointing toward cascade effects where early disruptions in lower-level social processes cause derailments of later-developing, higher-level processes. For example, early abnormalities in gaze following may underlie later deficits in perspective taking (Elsabbagh et al., 2012); abnormalities in motor resonance for body movements may lead to deficits in imitation (Gowen et al., 2008); and abnormalities in facial expression mimicry may be related to difficulties with empathy (McIntosh et al., 2006; Oberman et al., 2009).

A comparative, evolutionary approach highlights the role of these underlying, lower-level processes because it frames neural and psychological systems in a way that emphasizes continuity. As evolution produces organisms of increasing complexity, new functions must be integrated into the framework of pre-existing, simpler functions, like new heating systems being grafted onto old ones in an apartment building. An understanding of the normal or disordered function of the new systems would be incomplete without an understanding of the underlying, older systems, and of how the new are related to the old. Thus, our understanding of the psychological and neural mechanisms of self-other mapping, other forms of social cognition, and other functions in general in our own species can be informed by considering the layered complexity these functions in other species.

Of course, it is obvious that there are some things about human behavior and the human brain that are “special.” Some human behaviors or neural features may not have easily identifiable correlates in other species (although we argue that most probably do, to some extent). A comparative perspective can also inform understanding of behavioral abilities that only human have: they must rely on aspects of neural organization that are unique to humans. Unique neural features can only be mapped to unique behavioral features if we have a firm understanding of which neural and behavioral features are shared with other species.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., Cooper, G., and Damasio, A. R. (2000). A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. J. Neurosci. 20, 2683–2690.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A. (1994). Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature 372, 669–672.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amici, F., Aureli, F., Visalberghi, E., and Call, J. (2009). Spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) follow gaze around barriers: evidence for perspective taking? J. Comp. Psychol. 123, 368–374.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Anderson, J. R., Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., and Matsuzawa, T. (2004). Contagious yawning in chimpanzees. Proc. Biol. Sci. 271(Suppl. 6), S468–S470.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Azevedo, T. M., Volchan, E., Imbiriba, L. A., Rodrigues, E. C., Oliveira, J. M., Oliveira, L. F., Lutterbach, L. G., and Vargas, C. D. (2005). A freezing-like posture to pictures of mutilation. Psychophysiology 42, 255–260.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baldissera, F., Cavallari, P., Craighero, L., and Fadiga, L. (2001). Modulation of spinal excitability during observation of hand actions in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 190–194.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., and Grafman, J. (2012a). Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human intelligence. Neuropsychologia. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.05.017. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., Solomon, J., Krueger, F., Forbes, C., and Grafman, J. (2012b). An integrative architecture for general intelligence and executive function revealed by lesion mapping. Brain 135(Pt 4), 1154–1164.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bard, K. A. (2007). Neonatal imitation in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) tested with two paradigms. Anim. Cogn. 10, 233–242.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., and Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21, 37–46.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bekkering, H., Wohlschlager, A., and Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 53, 153–164.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blakemore, S. J., and Frith, C. (2005). The role of motor contagion in the prediction of action. Neuropsychologia 43, 260–267.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bonini, L., Rozzi, S., Serventi, F. U., Simone, L., Ferrari, P. F., and Fogassi, L. (2009). Ventral premotor and inferior parietal cortices make distinct contribution to action organization and intention understanding. Cereb. Cortex 20, 1372–1385.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bouquet, C. A., Shipley, T. F., Capa, R. L., and Marshall, P. J. (2010). Motor contagion: goal-directed actions are more contagious than non-goal-directed actions. Exp. Psychol. 58, 71–78.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brass, M., and Heyes, C. (2005). Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 489–495.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brauer, J., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees really know what others can see in a competitive situation. Anim. Cogn. 10, 439–448.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R. J., Zilles, K., Rizzolatti, G., and Freund, H. J. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 400–404.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bugnyar, T., Stowe, M., and Heinrich, B. (2004). Ravens, Corvus corax, follow gaze direction of humans around obstacles. Proc. Biol. Sci. 271, 1331–1336.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Caggiano, V., Fogassi, L., Rizzolatti, G., Pomper, J. K., Thier, P., Giese, M. A., and Casile, A. (2011). View-based encoding of actions in mirror neurons of area f5 in macaque premotor cortex. Curr. Biol. 21, 144–148.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Campbell, M. W., Carter, J. D., Proctor, D., Eisenberg, M. L., and de Waal, F. B. (2009). Computer animations stimulate contagious yawning in chimpanzees. Proc. Biol. Sci. 276, 4255–4259.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Campbell, M. W., and de Waal, F. B. (2011). Ingroup-outgroup bias in contagious yawning by chimpanzees supports link to empathy. PLoS ONE 6:e18283. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018283

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., and Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 63, i–vi, 1–143.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A. R., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. Neuroimage 50, 1148–1167.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chartrand, T. L., and Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 893–910.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Clements, W. A., and Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cogn. Dev. 9, 377–395.

Cook, M., and Mineka, S. (1987). Second-order conditioning and overshadowing in the observational conditioning of fear in monkeys. Behav. Res. Ther. 25, 349–364.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cook, M., and Mineka, S. (1989). Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 98, 448–459.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cook, M., and Mineka, S. (1990). Selective associations in the observational conditioning of fear in rhesus monkeys. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 16, 372–389.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Corkum, V., and Moore, C. (1998). The origins of joint visual attention in infants. Dev. Psychol. 34, 28–38.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cornell, H. N., Marzluff, J. M., and Pecoraro, S. (2011). Social learning spreads knowledge about dangerous humans among American crows. Proc. Biol. Sci. 279, 499–508.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Custance, D. M., Whiten, A., and Bard, K. A. (1995). Can young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) imitate arbitrary actions? Hayes and Hayes 1952 revisited. Behavior 132, 837–859.

Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., and Iacoboni, M. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 28–30.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, Third Edn reprinted by Oxford University Press, 1998.

Davila Ross, M., Menzler, S., and Zimmermann, E. (2008). Rapid facial mimicry in orangutan play. Biol. Lett. 4, 27–30.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

de Waal, F. B., and Ferrari, P. F. (2010). Towards a bottom-up perspective on animal and human cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 201–207.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dimberg, U., and Thunberg, M. (1998). Rapid facial reactions to emotional facial expressions. Scand. J. Psychol. 39, 39–45.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Egliston, K. A., and Rapee, R. M. (2007). Inhibition of fear acquisition in toddlers following positive modelling by their mothers. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 1871–1882.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Elsabbagh, M., Mercure, E., Hudry, K., Chandler, S., Pasco, G., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., and Johnson, M. H. (2012). Infant neural sensitivity to dynamic eye gaze is associated with later emerging autism. Curr. Biol. 22, 338–342.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Elsner, B. (2007). Infants' imitation of goal-directed actions: the role of movements and action effects. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 124, 44–59.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Elsner, B., Hauf, P., and Aschersleben, G. (2007). Imitating step by step: a detailed analysis of 9- to 15-month-olds' reproduction of a three-step action sequence. Infant Behav. Dev. 30, 325–335.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Emery, N. J., Lorincz, E. N., Perrett, D. I., Oram, M. W., and Baker, C. I. (1997). Gaze following and joint attention in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). J. Comp. Psychol. 111, 286–293.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Evans, J. S. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 255–278.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Facchinetti, L. D., Imbiriba, L. A., Azevedo, T. M., Vargas, C. D., and Volchan, E. (2006). Postural modulation induced by pictures depicting prosocial or dangerous contexts. Neurosci. Lett. 410, 52–56.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., and Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. J. Neurophysiol. 73, 2608–2611.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Feinstein, J. S., Adolphs, R., Damasio, A., and Tranel, D. (2010). The human amygdala and the induction and experience of fear. Curr. Biol. 21, 34–38.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ferrari, P. F., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G., and Fogassi, L. (2003). Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, 1703–1714.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ferrari, P. F., Kohler, E., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (2000). The ability to follow eye gaze and its emergence during development in macaque monkeys. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 13997–14002.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ferrari, P. F., Maiolini, C., Addessi, E., Fogassi, L., and Visalberghi, E. (2005). The observation and hearing of eating actions activates motor programs related to eating in macaque monkeys. Behav. Brain Res. 161, 95–101.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ferrari, P. F., Paukner, A., Ionica, C., and Suomi, S. J. (2009a). Reciprocal face-to-face communication between rhesus macaque mothers and their newborn infants. Curr. Biol. 19, 1768–1772.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ferrari, P. F., Paukner, A., Ruggiero, A., Darcey, L., Unbehagen, S., and Suomi, S. J. (2009b). Interindividual differences in neonatal imitation and the development of action chains in rhesus macaques. Child Dev. 80, 1057–1068.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ferrari, P. F., Vanderwert, R., Paukner, A., Bower, S., Suomi, S. J., and Fox, N. A. (2012). Distinct electroencephalographic amplitude suppression to facial gestures as evidence for a mirror mechanism in newborn monkeys. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 1165–1172.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ferrari, P. F., Visalberghi, E., Paukner, A., Fogassi, L., Ruggiero, A., and Suomi, S. J. (2006). Neonatal imitation in Rhesus macaques. PLoS Biol. 4:e302. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040302

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fragaszy, D., and Visalberghi, E. (2004). Socially biased learning in monkeys. Learn. Behav. 32, 24–35.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fragaszy, D. M., and Visalberghi, E. (1989). Social influences on the acquisition of tool-using behaviors in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J. Comp. Psychol. 103, 159–170.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119(Pt 2), 593–609.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gangitano, M., Mottaghy, F. M., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Phase-specific modulation of cortical motor output during movement observation. Neuroreport 12, 1489–1492.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Geangu, E., Benga, O., Stahl, D., and Striano, T. (2010). Contagious crying beyond the first days of life. Infant Behav. Dev. 33, 279–288.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gerull, F. C., and Rapee, R. M. (2002). Mother knows best: effects of maternal modelling on the acquisition of fear and avoidance behaviour in toddlers. Behav. Res. Ther. 40, 279–287.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Goldenberg, G. (2009). Apraxia and the parietal lobes. Neuropsychologia 47, 1449–1459.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gowen, E., Stanley, J., and Miall, R. C. (2008). Movement interference in autism-spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia 46, 1060–1068.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Guzman, Y. F., Tronson, N. C., Guedea, A., Huh, K. H., Gao, C., and Radulovic, J. (2009). Social modeling of conditioned fear in mice by non-fearful conspecifics. Behav. Brain Res. 201, 173–178.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haker, H., and Rossler, W. (2009). Empathy in schizophrenia: impaired resonance. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 259, 352–361.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hare, B., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Anim. Behav. 61, 139–151.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hari, R. (2006). Action-perception connection and the cortical mu rhythm. Prog. Brain Res. 159, 253–260.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harr, A. L., Gilbert, V. R., and Phillips, K. A. (2009). Do dogs (Canis familiaris) show contagious yawning? Anim. Cogn. 12, 833–837.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harrison, N. A., Gray, M. A., and Critchley, H. D. (2009). Dynamic pupillary exchange engages brain regions encoding social salience. Soc. Neurosci. 4, 233–243.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harrison, N. A., Singer, T., Rotshtein, P., Dolan, R. J., and Critchley, H. D. (2006). Pupillary contagion: central mechanisms engaged in sadness processing. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 1, 5–17.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harrison, N. A., Wilson, C. E., and Critchley, H. D. (2007). Processing of observed pupil size modulates perception of sadness and predicts empathy. Emotion 7, 724–729.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hayes, K. J., and Hayes, C. (1952). Imitation in a home-raised chimpanzee. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 45, 450–459.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hecht, E. E., Gutman, D. A., Preuss, T. M., Sanchez, M. M., Parr, L. A., and Rilling, J. K. (2012). Process versus product in social learning: comparative diffusion tensor imaging of neural systems for action execution-observation matching in macaques, chimpanzees, and humans. Cereb. Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs097. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Heimann, M., Nelson, K. E., and Schaller, J. (1989). Neonatal imitation of tongue protrusion and mouth opening: methodological aspects and evidence of early individual differences. Scand. J. Psychol. 30, 90–101.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Helt, M. S., Eigsti, I. M., Snyder, P. J., and Fein, D. A. (2010). Contagious yawning in autistic and typical development. Child Dev. 81, 1620–1631.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hoffman, E. A., and Haxby, J. V. (2000). Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 80–84.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Horner, V., and Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Anim. Cogn. 8, 164–181.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Inoue-Nakamura, N., and Matsuzawa, T. (1997). Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 111, 159–173.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Jabbi, M., Bastiaansen, J., and Keysers, C. (2008). A common anterior insula representation of disgust observation, experience and imagination shows divergent functional connectivity pathways. PLoS ONE 3:e2939. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002939

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jeannerod, M., and Frak, V. (1999). Mental imaging of motor activity in humans. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9, 735–739.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jeon, D., Kim, S., Chetana, M., Jo, D., Ruley, H. E., Lin, S. Y., Rabah, D., Kinet, J. P., and Shin, H. S. (2010). Observational fear learning involves affective pain system and Cav1.2 Ca2+ channels in ACC. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 482–488.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jeon, D., and Shin, H. S. (2011). A mouse model for observational fear learning and the empathetic response. Curr. Protoc. Neurosci. Chapter 8, Unit 8 27.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Joly-Mascheroni, R. M., Senju, A., and Shepherd, A. J. (2008). Dogs catch human yawns. Biol. Lett. 4, 446–448.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kalaska, J. F., Cohen, D. A., Hyde, M. L., and Prud'homme, M. (1989). A comparison of movement direction-related versus load direction-related activity in primate motor cortex, using a two-dimensional reaching task. J. Neurosci. 9, 2080–2102.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Kawamura, S. (1959). The process of sub-culture propagation among Japanese macaques. Primates 2, 43–60.

Kehmeier, S., Schloegl, C., Scheiber, I. B., and Weiss, B. M. (2011). Early development of gaze following into distant space in juvenile Greylag geese (Anser anser). Anim. Cogn. 14, 477–485.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kelly, M. M., and Forsyth, J. P. (2007). Observational fear conditioning in the acquisition and extinction of attentional bias for threat: an experimental evaluation. Emotion 7, 324–335.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keysers, C., and Perrett, D. I. (2004). Demystifying social cognition: a Hebbian perspective. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 501–507.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Koenigs, M., Barbey, A. K., Postle, B. R., and Grafman, J. (2009). Superior parietal cortex is critical for the manipulation of information in working memory. J. Neurosci. 29, 14980–14986.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Krachun, C., and Call, J. (2009). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) know what can be seen from where. Anim. Cogn. 12, 317–331.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Krachun, C., Carpenter, M., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2009). A competitive nonverbal false belief task for children and apes. Dev. Sci. 12, 521–535.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kuroda, T., Masaoka, Y., Kasai, H., Noguchi, K., Kawamura, M., and Homma, I. (2011). Sharing breathlessness: investigating respiratory change during observation of breath-holding in another. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 180, 218–222.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laland, K. N., and Reader, S. M. (1999). Foraging innovation in the guppy. Anim. Behav. 57, 331–340.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lamm, C., Decety, J., and Singer, T. (2010). Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage 54, 2492–2502.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Langford, D. J., Crager, S. E., Shehzad, Z., Smith, S. B., Sotocinal, S. G., Levenstadt, J. S., Chanda, M. L., Levitin, D. J., and Mogil, J. S. (2006). Social modulation of pain as evidence for empathy in mice. Science 312, 1967–1970.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lepage, J. F., and Theoret, H. (2006). EEG evidence for the presence of an action observation-execution matching system in children. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 2505–2510.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Loretto, M. C., Schloegl, C., and Bugnyar, T. (2009). Northern bald ibises follow others' gaze into distant space but not behind barriers. Biol. Lett. 6, 14–17.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., and Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 19751–19756.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

MacLean, P. D. (1990). The Triune Brain in Evolution: Role in Paleocerebral Functions. New York, NY: Springer.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maeda, F., Kleiner-Fisman, G., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2002). Motor facilitation while observing hand actions: specificity of the effect and role of observer's orientation. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 1329–1335.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Magnee, M. J., Stekelenburg, J. J., Kemner, C., and de Gelder, B. (2007). Similar facial electromyographic responses to faces, voices, and body expressions. Neuroreport 18, 369–372.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marshall, P. J., Bouquet, C. A., Thomas, A. L., and Shipley, T. F. (2010). Motor contagion in young children: exploring social influences on perception-action coupling. Neural Netw. 23, 1017–1025.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Materna, S., Dicke, P. W., and Thier, P. (2008). Dissociable roles of the superior temporal sulcus and the intraparietal sulcus in joint attention: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 108–119.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McGuigan, N., Makinson, J., and Whiten, A. (2011). From over-imitation to super-copying: adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 1–18.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McIntosh, D. N., Reichmann-Decker, A., Winkielman, P., and Wilbarger, J. L. (2006). When the social mirror breaks: deficits in automatic, but not voluntary, mimicry of emotional facial expressions in autism. Dev. Sci. 9, 295–302.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198, 75–78.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Dev. 54, 702–709.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mineka, S., and Cook, M. (1986). Immunization against the observational conditioning of snake fear in rhesus monkeys. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 95, 307–318.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mineka, S., and Cook, M. (1993). Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of fear. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 122, 23–38.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Molenberghs, P., Cunnington, R., and Mattingley, J. B. (2011). Brain regions with mirror properties: a meta-analysis of 125 human fMRI studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 341–349.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moore, A., Gorodnitsky, I., and Pineda, J. (2011). EEG mu component responses to viewing emotional faces. Behav. Brain Res. 226, 309–316.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mulder, T., de Vries, S., and Zijlstra, S. (2005). Observation, imagination and execution of an effortful movement: more evidence for a central explanation of motor imagery. Exp. Brain Res. 163, 344–351.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., and Johnson, B. W. (2004). Changes in rolandic mu rhythm during observation of a precision grip. Psychophysiology 41, 152–156.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Johnson, B. W., Gaetz, W. C., and Cheyne, D. O. (2006). Neural processing of observed oro-facial movements reflects multiple action encoding strategies in the human brain. Brain Res. 1071, 105–112.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Johnson, B. W., and McNair, N. A. (2004). Mu rhythm modulation during observation of an object-directed grasp. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 19, 195–201.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., Tomonaga, M., Tanaka, M., and Matsuzawa, T. (2004). Imitation in neonatal chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Dev. Sci. 7, 437–442.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nagy, E., Compagne, H., Orvos, H., Pal, A., Molnar, P., Janszky, I., Loveland, K. A., and Bardos, G. (2005). Index finger movement imitation by human neonates: motivation, learning, and left-hand preference. Pediatr. Res. 58, 749–753.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Neal, D. T., and Chartrand, T. L. (2011). Embodied emotion perception: amplifying and dampening facial feedback modulates emotion perception accuracy. Soc. Psychol. Pers. Sci. 2, 673–678.

Nystrom, P. (2008). The infant mirror neuron system studied with high density EEG. Soc. Neurosci. 3, 334–347.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., and Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 24, 190–198.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Oberman, L. M., Winkielman, P., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). Face to face: blocking facial mimicry can selectively impair recognition of emotional expressions. Soc. Neurosci. 2, 167–178.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Oberman, L. M., Winkielman, P., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2009). Slow echo: facial EMG evidence for the delay of spontaneous, but not voluntary, emotional mimicry in children with autism spectrum disorders. Dev. Sci. 12, 510–520.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Okamoto-Barth, S., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2007). Great apes' understanding of other individuals' line of sight. Psychol. Sci. 18, 462–468.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Olsson, A., and Phelps, E. A. (2004). Learned fear of “unseen” faces after Pavlovian, observational, and instructed fear. Psychol. Sci. 15, 822–828.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paccalin, C., and Jeannerod, M. (2000). Changes in breathing during observation of effortful actions. Brain Res. 862, 194–200.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Palagi, E., Leone, A., Mancini, G., and Ferrari, P. F. (2009). Contagious yawning in gelada baboons as a possible expression of empathy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 19262–19267.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Parr, L. A., Hecht, E., Barks, S. K., Preuss, T. M., and Votaw, J. R. (2009). Face processing in the chimpanzee brain. Curr. Biol. 19, 50–53.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paukner, A., and Anderson, J. R. (2006). Video-induced yawning in stumptail macaques (Macaca arctoides). Biol. Lett. 2, 36–38.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paukner, A., Anderson, J. R., Borelli, E., Visalberghi, E., and Ferrari, P. F. (2005). Macaques (Macaca nemestrina) recognize when they are being imitated. Biol. Lett. 1, 219–222.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paukner, A., Ferrari, P. F., and Suomi, S. J. (2011). Delayed imitation of lipsmacking gestures by infant rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). PLoS ONE 6:e28848. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028848

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paukner, A., Suomi, S. J., Visalberghi, E., and Ferrari, P. F. (2009). Capuchin monkeys display affiliation toward humans who imitate them. Science 325, 880–883.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paulus, M., Hunnius, S., Vissers, M., and Bekkering, H. (2011). Bridging the gap between the other and me: the functional role of motor resonance and action effects in infants' imitation. Dev. Sci. 14, 901–910.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pelphrey, K. A., Singerman, J. D., Allison, T., and McCarthy, G. (2003). Brain activation evoked by perception of gaze shifts: the influence of context. Neuropsychologia 41, 156–170.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pelphrey, K. A., Viola, R. J., and McCarthy, G. (2004). When strangers pass: processing of mutual and averted social gaze in the superior temporal sulcus. Psychol. Sci. 15, 598–603.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Perrett, D. I., Oram, M. W., Harries, M. H., Bevan, R., Hietanen, J. K., Benson, P. J., and Thomas, S. (1991). Viewer-centred and object-centred coding of heads in the macaque temporal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 86, 159–173.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Pineda, J. A. (2005). The functional significance of mu rhythms: translating “seeing” and “hearing” into “doing”. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 50, 57–68.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Platek, S. M. (2010). Yawn, yawn, yawn, yawn; yawn, yawn, yawn! The social, evolutionary and neuroscientific facets of contagious yawning. Front. Neurol. Neurosci. 28, 107–112.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Platek, S. M., Mohamed, F. B., and Gallup, G. G. Jr. (2005). Contagious yawning and the brain. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 23, 448–452.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Povinelli, D. J., Nelson, K. E., and Boysen, S. T. (1990). Inferences about guessing and knowing by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 104, 203–210.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Premack, D. G., and Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav. Brain Sci. 1, 515–526.

Preston, S. D., and de Waal, F. B. (2002). Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav. Brain Sci. 25, 1–20. discussion: 20–71.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Puce, A., Allison, T., Bentin, S., Gore, J. C., and McCarthy, G. (1998). Temporal cortex activation in humans viewing eye and mouth movements. J. Neurosci. 18, 2188–2199.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Range, F., and Viranyi, Z. (2011). Development of gaze following abilities in wolves (Canis lupus). PLoS ONE 6:e16888. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016888

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rilling, J. K., Barks, S. K., Parr, L. A., Preuss, T. M., Faber, T. L., Pagnoni, G., Bremner, J. D., and Votaw, J. R. (2007). A comparison of resting-state brain activity in humans and chimpanzees. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 17146–17151.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rizzolatti, G., Camarda, R., Fogassi, L., Gentilucci, M., Luppino, G., and Matelli, M. (1988). Functional organization of inferior area 6 in the macaque monkey. II. Area F5 and the control of distal movements. Exp. Brain Res. 71, 491–507.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., and Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 3, 131–141.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rosati, A. G., and Hare, B. (2009). Looking past the model species: diversity in gaze-following skills across primates. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 45–51.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rozzi, S., Ferrari, P. F., Bonini, L., Rizzolatti, G., and Fogassi, L. (2008). Functional organization of inferior parietal lobule convexity in the macaque monkey: electrophysiological characterization of motor, sensory and mirror responses and their correlation with cytoarchitectonic areas. Eur. J. Neurosci. 28, 1569–1588.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saby, J. N., Marshall, P. J., Smythe, R., Bouquet, C. A., and Comalli, C. E. (2011). An investigation of the determinants of motor contagion in preschool children. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 138, 231–236.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J., and Bodley Scott, S. E. (2010). Seeing it their way: evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 36, 1255–1266.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Scaife, M., and Bruner, J. S. (1975). The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant. Nature 253, 265–266.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Schloegl, C., Kotrschal, K., and Bugnyar, T. (2008). Do common ravens (Corvus corax) rely on human or conspecific gaze cues to detect hidden food? Anim. Cogn. 11, 231–241.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schurmann, M., Hesse, M. D., Stephan, K. E., Saarela, M., Zilles, K., Hari, R., and Fink, G. R. (2005). Yearning to yawn: the neural basis of contagious yawning. Neuroimage 24, 1260–1264.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shepherd, S. V., Klein, J. T., Deaner, R. O., and Platt, M. L. (2009). Mirroring of attention by neurons in macaque parietal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 9489–9494.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sonnby-Borgstrom, M. (2002). Automatic mimicry reactions as related to differences in emotional empathy. Scand. J. Psychol. 43, 433–443.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Surtees, A. D., and Apperly, I. A. (2012). Egocentrism and automatic perspective taking in children and adults. Child Dev. 83, 452–460.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tamietto, M., Castelli, L., Vighetti, S., Perozzo, P., Geminiani, G., Weiskrantz, L., and de Gelder, B. (2009). Unseen facial and bodily expressions trigger fast emotional reactions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 17661–17666.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Teglas, E., Gergely, A., Kupan, K., Miklosi, A., and Topal, J. (2012). Dogs' gaze following is tuned to human communicative signals. Curr. Biol. 22, 209–212.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tomasello, M., Hare, B., Lehmann, H., and Call, J. (2007). Reliance on head versus eyes in the gaze following of great apes and human infants: the cooperative eye hypothesis. J. Hum. Evol. 52, 314–320.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Uno, T., Greer, S. E., and Goates, L. (1973). Observational facilitation of response prevention. Behav. Res. Ther. 11, 207–212.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

van Elk, M., van Schie, H. T., Hunnius, S., Vesper, C., and Bekkering, H. (2008). You'll never crawl alone: neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy. Neuroimage 43, 808–814.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Varcin, K. J., Bailey, P. E., and Henry, J. D. (2010). Empathic deficits in schizophrenia: the potential role of rapid facial mimicry. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 16, 621–629.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Videan, E. N., Fritz, J., Schwandt, M., and Howell, S. (2005). Neighbor effect: evidence of affiliative and agonistic social contagion in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am. J. Primatol. 66, 131–144.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Voelkl, B., and Huber, L. (2000). True imitation in marmosets. Anim. Behav. 60, 195–202.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wascher, C. A., Fraser, O. N., and Kotrschal, K. (2010). Heart rate during conflicts predicts post-conflict stress-related behavior in greylag geese. PLoS ONE 5:e15751. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015751

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weiss, M. R., McCullagh, P., Smith, A. L., and Berlant, A. R. (1998). Observational learning and the fearful child: influence of peer models on swimming skill performance and psychological responses. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 69, 380–394.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Whiten, A., Horner, V., Litchfield, C. A., and Marshall-Pescini, S. (2004). How do apes ape? Learn. Behav. 32, 36–52.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., and Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 2417–2428.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40, 655–664.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wicker, B., Michel, F., Henaff, M. A., and Decety, J. (1998). Brain regions involved in the perception of gaze: a PET study. Neuroimage 8, 221–227.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wilkinson, A., Mandl, I., Bugnyar, T., and Huber, L. (2010). Gaze following in the red-footed tortoise (Geochelone carbonaria). Anim. Cogn. 13, 765–769.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wright, P., He, G., Shapira, N. A., Goodman, W. K., and Liu, Y. (2004). Disgust and the insula: fMRI responses to pictures of mutilation and contamination. Neuroreport 15, 2347–2351.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Zentall, T. R. (2004). Action imitation in birds. Learn. Behav. 32, 15–23.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Zentall, T. R. (2006). Imitation: definitions, evidence, and mechanisms. Anim. Cogn. 9, 335–353.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zentall, T. R., and Levine, J. M. (1972). Observational learning and social facilitation in the rat. Science 178, 1220–1221.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: reflective processing, reflexive processing, social cognition, empathy, comparative cognition, evolution, motor resonance

Citation: Hecht EE, Patterson R and Barbey AK (2012) What can other animals tell us about human social cognition? An evolutionary perspective on reflective and reflexive processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:224. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00224

Received: 15 March 2012; Accepted: 12 July 2012;
Published online: 27 July 2012.

Edited by:

Chad E. Forbes, University of Delaware, USA

Reviewed by:

Hani Freeman, Lincoln Park Zoo, USA
Jules Panksepp, Oregon Health and Science University, USA

Copyright © 2012 Hecht, Patterson and Barbey. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: A. K. Barbey, Decision Neuroscience Laboratory, 110 Huff Hall, 1206 South Fourth Street, Champaign, IL 61820 MC-586, USA. e-mail: barbey@illinois.edu

Download