Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 26 March 2013
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 7 - 2013 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00096

Neurophysiological correlates of anhedonia in feedback processing

Gabry W. Mies1,2* Ivo Van den Berg3 Ingmar H. A. Franken3 Marion Smits4 Maurits W. Van der Molen5 Frederik M. Van der Veen1,3
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Erasmus MC, University Medical Center Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands
  • 2Department of Developmental Psychology, Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, Netherlands
  • 3Institute of Psychology, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands
  • 4Department of Radiology, Erasmus MC, University Medical Center Rotterdam, Rotterdam, Netherlands
  • 5Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands

Disturbances in feedback processing and a dysregulation of the neural circuit in which the cingulate cortex plays a key role have been frequently observed in depression. Since depression is a heterogeneous disease, instead of focusing on the depressive state in general, this study investigated the relations between the two core symptoms of depression, i.e., depressed mood and anhedonia, and the neural correlates of feedback processing using fMRI. The focus was on the different subdivisions of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Undergraduates with varying levels of depressed mood and anhedonia performed a time-estimation task in which they received positive and negative feedback that was either valid or invalid (i.e., related vs. unrelated to actual performance). The rostral cingulate zone (RCZ), corresponding to the dorsal part of the ACC, was less active in response to feedback in more anhedonic individuals, after correcting for the influence of depressed mood, whereas the subgenual ACC was more active in these individuals. Task performance was not affected by anhedonia, however. No statistically significant effects were found for depressed mood above and beyond the effects of anhedonia. This study therefore implies that increasing levels of anhedonia involve changes in the neural circuitry underlying feedback processing.

Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a serious mental illness, characterized by at least one of two core symptoms: depressed mood and anhedonia (i.e., the loss of pleasure). MDD affects both affective and cognitive functioning. One of the deficits in MDD in which cognition and affect both play a role is impaired feedback processing. Behavioral studies have shown that depressed individuals are hypersensitive to negative feedback. When they make an error or receive negative feedback on their performance, their subsequent performance deteriorates (e.g., Beats et al., 1996; Elliott et al., 1997; Steffens et al., 2001).

In addition to these aberrant behavioral responses, depressed patients have been found to show an increased electrophysiological response to negative feedback, reflected by the feedback-related negativity (FRN; Tucker et al., 2003; Santesso et al., 2008; Mies et al., 2011b), an event-related brain potential (ERP) component that occurs after receiving negative feedback (Miltner et al., 1997). The FRN is presumed to be generated in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004).

The ACC can be divided in two subdivisions: a dorsal part, also known as the midcingulate cortex (MCC), which can be further subdivided into an anterior (aMCC) and posterior (pMCC) part, and a ventral part (ACC), which can be further subdivided into a pregenual (pgACC) and subgenual (sgACC) part (Vogt, 2005; see also Shackman et al., 2011). The aMCC, or more precisely, the rostral cingulate zone (RCZ), has received a lot of attention in the literature on error and feedback processing, since it has repeatedly been found more active during errors, conflict and negative feedback than during correct responses and positive feedback (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004).

In depressed individuals, the MCC, which is thought to be involved in cognitive control, has been found hypoactive, while brain regions primarily involved in emotion processing, such as the amygdala and sgACC, have been found hyperactive (Mayberg, 1997, 2003; Davidson et al., 2002; Pizzagalli, 2011). It is therefore thought that the top-down control of the “cognitive” areas over the “affective” areas is disturbed in depression (e.g., Taylor Tavares et al., 2008). This dysregulation appears to persist in fully recovered patients (Hooley et al., 2009), which may make them vulnerable to a relapse. It is, however, possible that this dysregulation is not a result of the depression, but predisposes an individual to develop a mood disorder such as MDD.

In the present fMRI study, we aimed to identify a relationship between a dysregulated circuit in which the MCC and ACC play a key role, reflected in aberrant feedback processing, and the two core symptoms of depression, depressed mood, and anhedonia. In most studies these symptoms are not separated, although it is known that depressed mood is associated with increased negative affect, while anhedonia is associated with decreased positive affect (Snaith, 1993; Pizzagalli et al., 2005), and that positive and negative affect are two independent constructs (Watson et al., 1988). Anhedonia has been associated with a blunting of behavioral and neural responses to the valence of stimuli (Steele et al., 2007; Dowd and Barch, 2010), whereas depressed mood has been associated with a negativity bias, i.e., the tendency to interpret ambiguous information in a negative way (e.g., Bouhuys et al., 1995). We, therefore, hypothesized that feedback processing would be differentially influenced by anhedonia and depressed mood.

For this purpose we recruited undergraduates who displayed mild depressive symptoms, and let them perform a time-estimation task with two important dimensions of feedback: valence (positive vs. negative feedback) and validity (valid vs. invalid feedback, i.e., feedback that is informative and therefore relevant for behavioral adjustments vs. uninformative/irrelevant feedback). In contrast to most tasks, in which the valence and information value of feedback are highly correlated, this paradigm enables us to disentangle emotion processing (valence processing) from cognitive control (validity processing). We have reported on this task in previous ERP papers including one that involves clinically depressed individuals (Mies et al., 2011b,c). In a previous fMRI study, this time-estimation paradigm showed that the RCZ was primarily sensitive to the validity of the feedback, whereas the pgACC was mainly sensitive to the valence of the feedback (Mies et al., 2011a).

In the present study, we investigated the effects of the core symptoms of depression on these neural correlates of feedback processing. Since anhedonia has been associated with a blunting of neural responses to the valence of stimuli, in e.g., the ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens (Steele et al., 2007; Dowd and Barch, 2010), we hypothesized that anhedonia would be associated with a blunted neural response to the valence of the feedback in this region as well as in the pgACC, i.e., a smaller difference between responses to positive and negative feedback. Depressed mood, on the other hand, was expected to lead to a blunted neural response to the validity of the feedback in the RCZ, i.e., a smaller difference between responses to valid and invalid feedback.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited by means of advertisements on college-wide electronic bulletin boards of the Erasmus University and the Erasmus MC—University Medical Center Rotterdam. The study was approved by the ethics committee of the Erasmus MC and all participants gave written informed consent. Participants received EUR 25 for participation.

Respondents were asked to fill out the Dutch translation of the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck et al., 1961; Bouman et al., 1985) assessing depression severity, and a short questionnaire assessing eligibility for participation in an MRI study. The BDI consists of 21 items, each including four statements (ranging from 0 to 3), assessing several symptoms of depression experienced in the last week. High scores indicate more depressive symptoms. In order to obtain a broad range of scores on our symptoms of interest, i.e., depressed mood and anhedonia, we selected participants on the basis of their overall BDI score at screening. Especially those who had a high score (≥10), indicative of mild depressive symptoms (e.g., Bouman et al., 1985) and those who had a low score (<3) at initial screening were invited to participate and were further screened for eligibility. This resulted in a range of BDI scores between 0 and 26 (M = 7, SD = 7) at the time of scanning, and, importantly, resulted in a broad and continuous range of scores on the questionnaires assessing depressed mood and anhedonia, specifically.

Exclusion criteria were: self-reported neurological illness, severe somatic illness, psychiatric illness other than depression, current treatment for any psychiatric illness (including depression), substance abuse, use of medication which affects the central nervous system (e.g., antidepressants), pregnancy, and any contra-indication for having an MRI-scan. Health criteria were assessed by means of a self-developed questionnaire and contra-indications for MRI were assessed by means of a standard questionnaire from the department of Radiology.

Eventually, 42 healthy volunteers, 26 female, aged between 18 and 32 (M = 23, SD = 3.5), participated in this study.

Questionnaires

To specify the core symptoms of depression, we used the Dutch version of the shortened Profile of Mood States (POMS, McNair et al., 1971; Wald and Mellenbergh, 1990) to assess depressed mood, and the Dutch version of the Snaith–Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS; Snaith et al., 1995; Franken et al., 2007) to assess trait anhedonia.

The visual analog version of the shortened POMS consists of 32 bipolar adjectives to assess current mood. For each pair of adjectives, scores range from 0 to 100, based on how many millimeters from the left participants made a mark on the line. This version of the POMS measures five dimensions: depression, anger, fatigue, tension, and vigor. The dimension “depression” was used as a measure for depressed mood. It consists of 8 items that represents depressed mood including feelings of sadness, unhappiness, hopelessness, loneliness, and worthlessness (Cronbach's alpha = 0.92). Finally, the SHAPS consists of 14 items to be answered on a 1–4 scale, ranging from absolutely agree (1) to absolutely disagree (4) (Cronbach's alpha = 0.83). Higher sum scores indicate higher levels of anhedonia.

Time-Estimation Task

The time-estimation task used in the present study was the same as reported earlier (Mies et al., 2011a,c), and was based on the original time-estimation paradigm developed by Miltner et al. (1997). Participants were instructed to produce 1 s intervals. Each trial started with the presentation of an asterisk (“*”) in the center of a black screen for 2 s. This asterisk was followed by the cue for estimation: a question mark (“?”), which was replaced with another asterisk (1 s) after the estimation. This second asterisk was followed by the feedback stimulus (1 s) (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Trial sequence with an example of the feedback stimulus. Happy facial expressions indicated positive feedback, fearful expressions indicated negative feedback. The gender (male/female) of the face indicated whether the estimation was too short or too long (counterbalanced across participants). The background grid (horizontal/vertical) indicated whether feedback was valid or invalid (counterbalanced across participants).

Participants had to indicate the end of the one-second interval by pressing the button of a response device. Following the button press, they received performance feedback, i.e., positive feedback if their response occurred within a specified window around the target (900–1100 ms), and negative feedback if the response occurred outside the window. Unbeknownst to participants, the window was dynamically adjusted on each trial (±20 ms) to ensure an equal amount of positive and negative feedback stimuli (see Miltner et al., 1997).

Estimates were followed by feedback. The feedback consisted of face stimuli presented against a horizontal or vertical background grid. The background grid communicated the validity of the feedback stimulus to the participants (valid vs. invalid). Valid feedback was based on the participant's performance. Invalid feedback was determined randomly by the computer, with a maximum of three invalid feedback trials in a row. Participants received invalid feedback in 50% of the trials. The emotional expression of the face informed participants that their estimate was correct or incorrect (respectively, a happy vs. a fearful face). Finally, in case of incorrect estimates, the gender of the face indicated whether the estimate was too short (e.g., a male face) or too long (e.g., a female face). The faces used in this study were from the Ekman and Friesen pictures set (Ekman and Friesen, 1978).

Procedure

Participants were seen twice. The first time, participants were asked to fill out the SHAPS and they practiced the two tasks they had to perform in the scanner. The first task participants had to perform was the time-estimation task as described above. Participants were given task instructions and they completed 36 practice trials of the time-estimation task on a computer outside the scanner. The other task was an unrelated task, which is not described in this paper.

Within 4 days of this first meeting (in most instances the next day), participants were scanned. They were asked to abstain from coffee and tobacco for at least 2 h before scanning. Participants first had to fill out the POMS, and were again given task instructions before entering the scanner. When participants were inside the scanner, the visual stimuli were projected on a screen at the end of the scanner bed, which could be viewed by the participant through a small mirror mounted on the head coil. During the time-estimation task participants responded by pressing the button of a response device with their right index finger. Inside the scanner participants performed several practice trials (maximum of 36 trials), after which the first session started, consisting of 120 trials (10 min). After a short break, a second session of the task started which again lasted 10 min. Participants performed 240 trials of the time-estimation task inside the scanner. After these two time-estimation sessions a structural scan was obtained, which lasted about 5 min.

Magnetic Resonance Imaging Data Acquisition

Blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) fMRI data were acquired on a 3T GE Healthcare (Milwaukee, WI) scanner. For the functional scans a single-shot gradient echo echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence was used. The T2*-weighted images were acquired in 26 axial slices (thickness = 3.5 mm, interslice gap = 0.5 mm) with a repetition time (TR) of 2000 ms, echo time (TE) of 30 ms, field of view (FOV) of 220 mm, and voxels of 1.72 × 1.72 × 3.50 mm. The interval between trials was about 5 s. In each session of 120 trials 310 volumes (8060 functional images) were obtained. In addition, five dummy scans were made before the task started in order to obtain a steady-state magnetization.

For anatomical reference, a 3D high-resolution inversion recovery fast spoiled gradient recalled echo T1-weighted sequence was used, which covered the whole brain. One hundred and ninety-two slices were acquired with an effective slice thickness of 0.8 mm, FOV of 250 mm, and voxels of 0.49 × 0.49 × 0.80 mm.

For pre-processing and processing of the fMRI data SPM5 (Statistical Parametric Mapping, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, UK) was used. Preprocessing of the structural data included manual reorienting, unified segmentation using the Montreal Neurological Institute T1 ICBM template for European brains for gray matter, white matter, and CSF, and normalization using the parameters derived from unified segmentation. Preprocessing of the functional data included manual reorienting, slice time correction, realignment using the middle slice as a reference, and unwarping, co-registration (functional images were co-registered to the gray matter structural image derived from unified segmentation), normalization using the parameters derived from unified segmentation, and smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm full width at half maximum, and a high-pass filter of 128 s for temporal smoothing.

Statistical Analyses

Performance data were analyzed by partial correlation analyses assessing the relation between POMS-depression scores and the percentage of correct adjustments after valid negative feedback and the percentage of “correct” adjustments after invalid negative feedback, while correcting for SHAPS-anhedonia scores, and vice versa (examining the relation between SHAPS scores and performance, while correcting for POMS scores).

For the fMRI analyses, a model was made in which the preprocessed fMRI data were coupled to the vectors of feedback onset of each condition (valid positive feedback, valid negative feedback, invalid positive feedback, and invalid negative feedback) in both task sessions. Then two t-contrasts were computed that were used for the whole-brain analyses only: positive—negative feedback (main effect of valence), and valid—invalid feedback (main effect of validity). The individual contrast images resulting from these contrasts were used in a second-level whole-brain analysis.

Whole-brain analyses were performed on the two contrasts. The POMS-depression score and the SHAPS-anhedonia score were added as covariates of interest. The POMS and SHAPS scores were both normally distributed, and were centered by the method of Delaney and Maxwell (1981): the mean of all participants was subtracted from individual scores. Significant voxels and clusters are reported as significant if P < 0.05 corrected with the family-wise error (FWE) approach. The Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002) was used to label the significant clusters and voxels.

Of main interest were, however, the region-of-interest (ROI) analyses. Four ROI analyses were performed using MarsBaR 0.41 (Brett et al., 2002). The left and right RCZ [8 mm sphere around ±8, 30, 32; coordinates adopted from Mars et al., 2005 and implemented in the AAL map of MarsBaR (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002)], the pgACC [8 mm sphere around 0, 40, −2; coordinates adopted from Nieuwenhuis et al. (2005)], the sgACC [8 mm sphere around 1, 32 −6; coordinates adopted from Matthews et al. (2009)], and the nucleus accumbens [NAcc, ±10, 12, −2, coordinates adopted from Knutson et al. (2008)] were defined as ROIs. Figure 2 illustrates the ROIs examined.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Indication of the anatomical location of (A) the three subdivisions of the anterior cingulate cortex: the rostral cingulate zone (RCZ), pregenual anterior cingulate (pgACC), and subgenual anterior cingulate (sgACC), and (B) the bilateral nucleus accumbens (NAcc), displayed on the ch2 template of MRIcron.

For the ROI analyses, beta-values were extracted from the fMRI data for each feedback condition (valid positive feedback, valid negative feedback, invalid positive feedback, and invalid negative feedback) separately. For each ROI, the extracted beta-values of each participant were exported to SPSS, and subsequently analyzed using valence (positive or negative feedback) and feedback-validity (valid or invalid feedback) as within-subjects factors in repeated-measures ANOVAs with mean-centered POMS-depression and SHAPS-anhedonia scores simultaneously added as covariates. Effects of lateralization in the RCZ and NAcc are not reported.

Results

One student did not perform according to task instructions, and was excluded from all analyses. Therefore, 41 participants were included in the analyses.

POMS-depression scores ranged from 8 to 457 (M = 187, SD = 96), and SHAPS scores ranged from 14 to 34 (M = 22, SD = 5). The POMS-depression scores and SHAPS-anhedonia scores correlated positively with each other (r = 0.47, P = 0.002).

Behavioral Results

Participants had a mean estimation time of 1053 ± 87 ms, and, as expected, they adjusted their behavior more often in response to valid negative feedback than to invalid negative feedback (85 ± 8% vs. 52 ± 6%). The partial correlation analyses showed a marginal negative correlation between depressed mood and percentage of correct behavioral adjustments after valid negative feedback (r = −0.28, P = 0.086), indicating that participants with higher scores of depressed mood performed slightly worse after receiving valid negative feedback. No associations were found with anhedonia.

Whole Brain Analyses

The results of the whole brain analyses are shown in Tables 1, 2. At the FWE-corrected threshold of P < 0.05, all contrasts revealed significant activation patterns, except negative feedback minus positive feedback. Importantly, the whole brain analyses did not reveal any significantly different activation patterns for participants with higher levels of depressed mood or anhedonia.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Whole brain analysis for the contrast positive feedback—negative feedback.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Whole brain analysis for the contrasts valid—invalid feedback, and invalid—valid feedback.

Region-of-Interest Analyses

General task effects

In line with our previous study, we found the RCZ more active in response to valid feedback than in response to invalid feedback [F(1, 40) = 8.1, P = 0.007, η2p = 0.17; see Figure 3A], whereas the pgACC was more active in response to positive feedback than in response to negative feedback [F(1, 40) = 30.4, P < 0.001, η2p = 0.43; see Figure 3B]. The NAcc was more active in response to valid feedback than in response to invalid feedback [F(1, 40) = 17.5, P < 0.001, η2p = 0.30], and was more active in response to positive feedback than in response to negative feedback [F(1, 40) = 23.6, P < 0.001, η2p = 0.37]. In addition, valence and validity interacted in the NAcc [F(1, 40) = 15.8, P < 0.001, η2p = 0.28]; the effect of valence was strongest in the valid condition (see Figure 3C). No interaction between valence and validity was found in the RCZ or pgACC. Finally, the sgACC did not respond differently to the different types of feedback.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Bar plots showing the mean beta values for the four predictors (valid positive, valid negative, invalid positive, and invalid negative) for (A) the rostral cingulate zone (averaged across the right and left hemisphere), (B) the pregenual anterior cingulate, and (C) the nucleus accumbens (averaged across both hemispheres).

Effects of depressed mood and anhedonia

The purpose of this study was to examine the separate influences of depressed mood and anhedonia on the four predefined ROIs in relation to these task effects. No statistically significant effects were found for depressed mood above and beyond the effects of anhedonia. Depressed mood only marginally interacted with the effect of validity in the RCZ [F(1, 38) = 3.1, P = 0.084, η2p = 0.08], and with the effect of valence in the NAcc [F(1, 38) = 3.1, P = 0.084, η2p = 0.08].

Anhedonia, on the other hand, did influence feedback processing. In the RCZ we found a main effect of anhedonia, after correction for variation in depressed mood [F(1, 38) = 7.5, P = 0.010, η2p = 0.16]. Higher levels of anhedonia were associated with decreased activity in the RCZ, independent of task condition (Figure 4A). Also in the sgACC a main effect of anhedonia was found. In this area higher levels of anhedonia were associated with increased activity, independent of task condition [F(1, 38) = 4.2, P = 0.048, η2p = 0.10; Figure 4B]. In the pgACC we found a marginal interaction between anhedonia and valence: more hedonic individuals showed a slightly larger difference between positive and negative feedback [F(1, 38) = 3.7, P = 0.064, η2p = 0.09]. Surprisingly, no relationship was found between anhedonia and activity in the NAcc.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Scatter plots of mean beta values representing activity in (A) the rostral cingulate zone (averaged across both hemispheres), and (B) the subgenual anterior cingulate as a function of anhedonia (measured with the SHAPS). Both graphs show values uncorrected for levels of depressed mood.

Discussion

The current study investigated the relationship between the two core symptoms of depression, i.e., depressed mood and anhedonia, and behavioral and neural responses to valid and invalid positive and negative feedback. In this student sample, after correcting for the influence of anhedonia, no statistically significant effects were found of depressed mood on feedback processing. Anhedonia, on the other hand, was negatively correlated with activity in the RCZ, and positively correlated with activity in the sgACC in response to feedback stimuli in general, after correcting for the influence of depressed mood (see Table 3). Anhedonia did not affect the behavioral responses to feedback.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Overview of ROI findings.

The general task effects are in line with our previous studies on feedback processing using this time-estimation task (Mies et al., 2011a,c). At the behavioral level, participants performed according to task instructions: they adjusted their behavior more often in response to valid negative feedback than in response to invalid negative feedback. At the neural level, we again found the RCZ to be sensitive to the validity of the feedback and the pgACC to be sensitive to the valence of the feedback, while the NAcc was sensitive to both valence and validity. The sgACC was neither sensitive to the valence, nor to the validity of the feedback, which implies that it does not play a major role in feedback processing.

Anhedonia was associated with an overall decrease of RCZ-activity in response to feedback. This effect was not hypothesized. Recently, Shackman et al. (2011) postulated the “adaptive control hypothesis,” which suggests that the MCC, in particular the RCZ, uses information with a negative value (punishment, pain, and more abstract forms of negative feedback) to bias responding when the most adaptive course of action is uncertain, and therefore integrates emotion, pain and cognitive control. Our findings are not completely in line with this hypothesis, since no effect of valence was found in the RCZ. This finding implies that the RCZ does not only integrate negative information with cognitive control, but positive information as well. In line with the adaptive control hypothesis, however, is our finding that RCZ activity was influenced by anhedonia, which implies that the RCZ does integrate emotional information, albeit on another level than on stimulus level. Another model of cingulate function, the “predicted response-outcome” (PRO) model, was recently developed by Alexander and Brown (2011), and suggests that activity in the medial PFC (including the RCZ) reflects a learned prediction of the probability and timing of all possible outcomes of an action. According to this model, mPFC/RCZ activity reflects the unexpected non-occurrence or unexpected occurrence of an outcome. This model, therefore, can account for both positive and negative feedback eliciting an RCZ response. Perhaps the general decrease of activity in this brain region in more anhedonic individuals reflects weaker outcome predictions in these individuals, in line with the lack of interest and motivation associated with anhedonia.

Anhedonia was also associated with increased sgACC activity in response to feedback, independent of the type of feedback. A hyperactive sgACC has been rather consistently found in clinically depressed patients (Mayberg, 1997, 2003; Davidson et al., 2002; Pizzagalli, 2011), in healthy persons with high levels of neuroticism or negative affect (Zald et al., 2002; Haas et al., 2007), and in healthy persons subjected to negative mood induction (Mayberg et al., 1999; Berna et al., 2010). This increased sgACC response in combination with a decreased RCZ response implies an imbalance in individuals with higher levels of anhedonia in the neural circuit in which the cingulate cortex plays a key role.

We further expected that higher levels of anhedonia would be associated with a reduced effect of valence in the pgACC and the NAcc. This expected blunted response to positive and negative feedback only marginally reached significance in the pgACC. Surprisingly, in contrast to most studies (Steele et al., 2007; Pizzagalli et al., 2009; Dowd and Barch, 2010), anhedonia was not associated with a blunted response in the NAcc. The discrepancy may lie in the fact that these other studies included clinically depressed or schizophrenic patients in which levels of anhedonia are likely to be higher than in our undergraduates with mild symptoms.

Surprisingly, no significant effects were found of depressed mood on feedback processing, after correction for the influence of anhedonia. We would like to note, however, that there were some trend-level effects, which might be of interest to more closely examine in future studies. Depressed mood appeared to be slightly associated with reduced sensitivity of the RCZ to the validity of feedback. This blunted response to feedback-validity was also expressed at the behavioral level. It should be noted that clinically depressed subjects in a previous ERP study also showed slightly impaired performance after valid negative feedback during the same task (Mies et al., 2011b). These findings suggest that the evaluation of the relevance of the feedback is reduced in individuals with higher levels of depressed mood.

There are several limitations that need to be addressed. First, as expected, anhedonia and depressed mood were correlated, making it difficult to completely disentangle their unique contributions to the present results. When correcting effects of depressed mood for anhedonia, effects did not reach significance, e.g., the interaction between depressed mood and validity of feedback in the RCZ. Although it is unnatural to select participants on the basis of distinct depressed mood and anhedonia scores, it might be useful in future studies to do so. In addition, larger sample sizes may be effective in disentangling their unique effects. Second, we used emotional faces as feedback stimuli, which may cause some concern about whether found effects are due to feedback processing or to emotional face processing per se. Emotional faces are differently processed by depressed than by healthy individuals, that is, depressed individuals show enhanced recognition and recollection of negative emotional expressions such as sadness, and impaired recognition of—or an attentional bias away from—positive expressions (see Leppanen, 2006 for a review). However, we deliberately chose to use easily distinguishable emotional expressions to increase the ecological validity of the feedback stimuli (see also Mies et al., 2011b). By including a valid and invalid feedback condition, we were able to control for the effect of emotion, which is underscored by the RCZ findings. Unfortunately, because the sgACC and pgACC were not sensitive to the validity of feedback, we cannot directly conclude that these areas are responsive to the valence of the feedback rather than to emotion processing per se. On the other hand, one could argue that (aberrant) processing of feedback-valence is inherent to (aberrant) emotion processing.

In conclusion, our findings suggest that anhedonia, rather than depressed mood, affects feedback processing at the neural level. Anhedonia was associated with a decreased response of the RCZ to feedback and with an often-reported hyperactive sgACC in depression. Our results imply that increasing levels of anhedonia involve changes in the neural circuitry underlying feedback processing. These atypical neural responses might render subjects vulnerable to depression.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a grant from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO 400-05-186). The authors wish to thank Maartje Luijten, Cindy Luck, and Vincent Kleinrensink for their help in data collection.

References

Alexander, W. H., and Brown, J. W. (2011). Medial prefrontal cortex as an action-outcome predictor. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 1338–1344.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Beats, B. C., Sahakian, B. J., and Levy, R. (1996). Cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in the elderly depressed. Psychol. Med. 26, 591–603.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., and Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561–571.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berna, C., Leknes, S., Holmes, E. A., Edwards, R. R., Goodwin, G. M., and Tracey, I. (2010). Induction of depressed mood disrupts emotion regulation neurocircuitry and enhances pain unpleasantness. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 1083–1090.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bouhuys, A. L., Bloem, G. M., and Groothuis, T. G. (1995). Induction of depressed and elated mood by music influences the perception of facial emotional expressions in healthy subjects. J. Affect. Disord. 33, 215–226.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bouman, T. K., Luteijn, F., Albersnagel, F. A., and van der Ploeg, F. A. E. (1985). Enige ervaringen met de Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). Gedrag 13, 13–24.

Brett, M., Anton, J.-L., Valabregue, R., and Poline, J.-B. (2002). “Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox,” in 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, (Sendai).

Davidson, R. J., Pizzagalli, D., Nitschke, J. B., and Putnam, K. (2002). Depression: perspectives from affective neuroscience. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 545–574.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Delaney, H. D., and Maxwell, S. E. (1981). On using analysis of covariance in repeated measures designs. Multivariate Behav. Res. 16, 105–123.

Dowd, E. C., and Barch, D. M. (2010). Anhedonia and emotional experience in schizophrenia: neural and behavioral indicators. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 902–911.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial Action Coding System: A Technique for the Measurement of Facial Movement. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Elliott, R., Sahakian, B. J., Herrod, J. J., Robbins, T. W., and Paykel, E. S. (1997). Abnormal response to negative feedback in unipolar depression: evidence for a diagnosis specific impairment. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 63, 74–82.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Franken, I. H., Rassin, E., and Muris, P. (2007). The assessment of anhedonia in clinical and non-clinical populations: further validation of the Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale (SHAPS). J. Affect. Disord. 99, 83–89.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haas, B. W., Omura, K., Constable, R. T., and Canli, T. (2007). Emotional conflict and neuroticism: personality-dependent activation in the amygdala and subgenual anterior cingulate. Behav. Neurosci. 121, 249–256.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hooley, J. M., Gruber, S. A., Parker, H. A., Guillaumot, J., Rogowska, J., and Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2009). Cortico-limbic response to personally challenging emotional stimuli after complete recovery from depression. Psychiatry Res. 172, 83–91.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Knutson, B., Wimmer, G. E., Rick, S., Hollon, N. G., Prelec, D., and Loewenstein, G. (2008). Neural antecedents of the endowment effect. Neuron 58, 814–822.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leppanen, J. M. (2006). Emotional information processing in mood disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging findings. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 19, 34–39.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mars, R. B., Coles, M. G., Grol, M. J., Holroyd, C. B., Nieuwenhuis, S., Hulstijn, W., et al. (2005). Neural dynamics of error processing in medial frontal cortex. Neuroimage 28, 1007–1013.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matthews, S., Simmons, A., Strigo, I., Gianaros, P., Yang, T., and Paulus, M. (2009). Inhibition-related activity in subgenual cingulate is associated with symptom severity in major depression. Psychiatry Res. 172, 1–6.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mayberg, H. S. (1997). Limbic-cortical dysregulation: a proposed model of depression. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 9, 471–481.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mayberg, H. S. (2003). Modulating dysfunctional limbic-cortical circuits in depression: towards development of brain-based algorithms for diagnosis and optimised treatment. Br. Med. Bull. 65, 193–207.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mayberg, H. S., Liotti, M., Brannan, S. K., McGinnis, S., Mahurin, R. K., Jerabek, P. A., et al. (1999). Reciprocal limbic-cortical function and negative mood: converging PET findings in depression and normal sadness. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 675–682.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McNair, D. M., Lorr, M., and Droppleman, L. F. (1971). Manual for the Profile of Mood States. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Services.

Mies, G. W., van der Molen, M. W., Smits, M., Hengeveld, M. W., and van der Veen, F. M. (2011a). The anterior cingulate cortex responds differently to the validity and valence of feedback in a time-estimation task. Neuroimage 56, 2321–2328.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mies, G. W., van der Veen, F. M., Tulen, J. H., Birkenhager, T. K., Hengeveld, M. W., and van der Molen, M. W. (2011b). Drug-free patients with major depression show an increased electrophysiological response to valid and invalid feedback. Psychol. Med. 41, 2515–2525.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mies, G. W., van der Veen, F. M., Tulen, J. H. M., Hengeveld, M. W., and van der Molen, M. W. (2011c). Cardiac and electrophysiological responses to valid and invalid feedback in a time-estimation task. J. Psychophysiol. 25, 131–142.

Miltner, W. H. R., Braun, C. H., and Coles, M. G. H. (1997). Event-related brain potentials following incorrect feedback in a time-estimation task: evidence for a “generic” neural system for error detection. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 788–798.

Nieuwenhuis, S., Slagter, H. A., von Geusau, N. J., Heslenfeld, D. J., and Holroyd, C. B. (2005). Knowing good from bad: differential activation of human cortical areas by positive and negative outcomes. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 3161–3168.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pizzagalli, D. A. (2011). Frontocingulate dysfunction in depression: toward biomarkers of treatment response. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 183–206.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pizzagalli, D. A., Iosifescu, D., Hallett, L. A., Ratner, K. G., and Fava, M. (2009). Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: evidence from a probabilistic reward task. J. Psychiatr. Res. 43, 76–87.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pizzagalli, D. A., Jahn, A. L., and O'shea, J. P. (2005). Toward an objective characterization of an anhedonic phenotype: a signal-detection approach. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 319–327.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., and Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 306, 443–447.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rorden, C., Karnath, H. O., and Bonilha, L. (2007). Improving lesion-symptom mapping. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1081–1088.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Santesso, D. L., Steele, K. T., Bogdan, R., Holmes, A. J., Deveney, C. M., Meites, T. M., et al. (2008). Enhanced negative feedback responses in remitted depression. Neuroreport 19, 1045–1048.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J., and Davidson, R. J. (2011). The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 154–167.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Snaith, P. (1993). Anhedonia: a neglected symptom of psychopathology. Psychol. Med. 23, 957–966.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Snaith, R. P., Hamilton, M., Morley, S., Humayan, A., Hargreaves, D., and Trigwell, P. (1995). A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone the Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale. Br. J. Psychiatry 167, 99–103.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Steele, J. D., Kumar, P., and Ebmeier, K. P. (2007). Blunted response to feedback information in depressive illness. Brain 130, 2367–2374.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Steffens, D. C., Wagner, H. R., Levy, R. M., Horn, K. A., and Krishnan, K. R. (2001). Performance feedback deficit in geriatric depression. Biol. Psychiatry 50, 358–363.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Taylor Tavares, J. V., Clark, L., Furey, M. L., Williams, G. B., Sahakian, B. J., and Drevets, W. C. (2008). Neural basis of abnormal response to negative feedback in unmedicated mood disorders. Neuroimage 42, 1118–1126.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tucker, D. M., Luu, P., Frishkoff, G., Quiring, J., and Poulsen, C. (2003). Frontolimbic response to negative feedback in clinical depression. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 112, 667–678.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tzourio-Mazoyer, N., Landeau, B., Papathanassiou, D., Crivello, F., Etard, O., Delcroix, N., et al. (2002). Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage 15, 273–289.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vogt, B. A. (2005). Pain and emotion interactions in subregions of the cingulate gyrus. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 533–544.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wald, F. D. M., and Mellenbergh, G. J. (1990). De verkorte versie van de Nederlandse vertaling van de Profile of Mood States (POMS). Ned. Tijdschr. Psychol. 45, 86–90.

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Zald, D. H., Mattson, D. L., and Pardo, J. V. (2002). Brain activity in ventromedial prefrontal cortex correlates with individual differences in negative affect. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 2450–2454.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: depression, anhedonia, fMRI, anterior cingulate cortex, feedback processing

Citation: Mies GW, Van den Berg I, Franken IHA, Smits M, Van der Molen MW and Van der Veen FM (2013) Neurophysiological correlates of anhedonia in feedback processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:96. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00096

Received: 03 January 2013; Accepted: 07 March 2013;
Published online: 26 March 2013.

Edited by:

John J. Foxe, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA

Reviewed by:

Alexander J. Shackman, University of Wisconsin-Madison, USA
Diego A. Pizzagalli, McLean Hospital/Harvard Medical School, USA
Kristen P. Morie, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA

Copyright © 2013 Mies, Van den Berg, Franken, Smits, Van der Molen and Van der Veen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Gabry W. Mies, Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, Montessorilaan 3, 6525 HR Nijmegen, Netherlands. e-mail: g.mies@psych.ru.nl

Download