Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 09 May 2013
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 7 - 2013 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00177

Structural basis of empathy and the domain general region in the anterior insular cortex

  • 1Department of Psychology, Division of Clinical Psychology and Epidemiology, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego (UCSD), La Jolla, California, USA
  • 3Department of Psychology, Division of Molecular Neuroscience, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Zurich University Hospital of Psychiatry, Zürich, Switzerland
  • 5Intracranial EEG and Brain Imaging Research Group, University of Freiburg, Freiburg, Germany

Empathy is key for healthy social functioning and individual differences in empathy have strong implications for manifold domains of social behavior. Empathy comprises of emotional and cognitive components and may also be closely linked to sensorimotor processes, which go along with the motivation and behavior to respond compassionately to another person's feelings. There is growing evidence for local plastic change in the structure of the healthy adult human brain in response to environmental demands or intrinsic factors. Here we have investigated changes in brain structure resulting from or predisposing to empathy. Structural MRI data of 101 healthy adult females was analyzed. Empathy in fictitious as well as real-life situations was assessed using a validated self-evaluation measure. Furthermore, empathy-related structural effects were also put into the context of a functional map of the anterior insular cortex (AIC) determined by activation likelihood estimate (ALE) meta-analysis of previous functional imaging studies. We found that gray matter (GM) density in the left dorsal AIC correlates with empathy and that this area overlaps with the domain general region (DGR) of the anterior insula that is situated in-between functional systems involved in emotion–cognition, pain, and motor tasks as determined by our meta-analysis. Thus, we propose that this insular region where we find structural differences depending on individual empathy may play a crucial role in modulating the efficiency of neural integration underlying emotional, cognitive, and sensorimotor information which is essential for global empathy.

Introduction

Empathy has strong implications for manifold domains of social behavior and it may constitute an integral part of emotional intelligence (Goleman, 1998). In the psychological literature, empathy has been defined as the ability to feel what another person is feeling (emotional component), and knowing what a person is feeling (cognitive component), i.e., to achieve a cognitive understanding of other feelings (Eisenberg and Miller, 1987; Decety and Jackson, 2004). Empathy may also include sensorimotor processes, which go along with the motivation and behavior to respond compassionately to another person's feelings (Preston, 2007; Zaki and Ochsner, 2012). It has been shown that empathetic ability, however, is not always going along with prosocial behavior (Eisenberg and Miller, 1987). The cognitive component of empathy may be closely linked to “theory of mind,” that is the meta-cognitive ability to represent mental states such as beliefs, intentions, and desires of other people (Premack and Woodruf, 1978). According to Dymond, an empathic person can imaginatively take the role of another and can understand and predict that person's thoughts, feelings, and actions (Dymond, 1949). Together, these definitions suggest that the human ability of empathy is more than a pure emotional process but also includes cognitive aspects such as perspective taking and may also involve the sensorimotor system for responding appropriately and compassionately to another person's feelings. There is growing appreciation that there are stable differences between individuals in the level of empathy which has a high impact on social behavior (Zaki and Ochsner, 2012) and that these differences can be reliably measured (Miller and Eisenberg, 1988; Marangoni et al., 1995; Singer et al., 2004). Recent studies have shown that there is capacity for local plastic change in the structure of the healthy adult human brain in response to environmental demands or intrinsic factors (Johansen-Berg, 2012). There is increasing interest in investigating the neuroanatomical basis underlying individual differences in human behavior and cognition (Kanai and Rees, 2011), including empathy (Banissy et al., 2012). However, the structural basis of empathy in particular in the insular cortex has remained unclear. Accumulating evidence indicates a crucial role of the insular cortex in empathy: in particular the anterior insular cortex (AIC)—a brain region which is situated in the depth of the Sylvian fissure and anatomically highly interconnected to many other cortical regions (Nieuwenhuys, 2012)—is part of the functional neural network that plays an essential role in mediating social-emotional processing (Mutschler et al., 2009, 2012) including empathy (Singer et al., 2004; Seeley et al., 2012).

Deficits in empathy have been reported in different neuropsychiatric conditions such as in borderline personality disorder with a history of childhood trauma and co-morbid posttraumatic stress disorder (Roepke et al., 2012), autistic spectrum disorders (Decety and Moriguchi, 2007), in psychopathy/antisocial personality disorder (Decety and Moriguchi, 2007; Shirtcliff et al., 2009), in conduct disorder (Sterzer et al., 2007), and in schizophrenia (Bora et al., 2008). In several of these disorders structural abnormities in the anterior insula have been shown such as in adolescents with conduct disorder (Sterzer et al., 2007), in adults with schizophrenia (Makris et al., 2006), and in individuals with psychopathy (de Oliveira-Souza et al., 2008). More recently, a study in combat veterans with traumatic brain injury reports that lesions in several brain regions, particularly in the insula, was associated with deficits in empathy (Driscoll et al., 2012).

On this background we used voxel-based morphometric (VBM) analysis of high-resolution structural MRI to investigate the correlation between local gray matter (GM) density and inter-individual differences in empathy in a large sample of healthy adult females (n = 101). VBM allows objective structural analysis across the whole brain in an unbiased way and with no a priori regions-of-interest (ROIs). A purely female sample was investigated because there is increasing evidence for sex differences in empathy (Hoffman, 1977; Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004).

Because of the increasing evidence indicating a crucial role of the insular cortex in empathy a further aim of this study was to put empathy-related structural effects into the context of a functional map of the AIC determined by activation likelihood estimate (ALE) meta-analysis of previous functional imaging studies. While the anatomical diversity of AIC is long recognized (Mesulam and Mufson, 1982; Mufson and Mesulam, 1982), functional studies have often treated the AIC as a single, homogenous region. Recently, however, a differentiated functional map of the AIC in the human brain is emerging, primarily based on meta-analytic summaries of neuroimaging studies (Mutschler et al., 2007, 2009, 2012) and connectivity studies (Cauda et al., 2011; Deen et al., 2011; Chang et al., 2013; Touroutoglou et al., 2012). These studies together clearly indicate that the AIC contains several functionally specialized parts, related to pain experience, auditory processing and language, and sensorimotor functions, among others. An further functional subregion in the AIC is the domain general region (DGR) (Dosenbach et al., 2006). It is outstanding that this region is activated across a broad range of tasks typically investigated in functional imaging studies (Craig, 2009) and has been proposed that it represents a potential high-level insular integration hub possibly involved in task-set representation (Dosenbach et al., 2006), awareness (Craig, 2009), rule-based evaluation of sensory information (Mutschler et al., 2009), and evaluation of the salience of (internal and external) sensory information (Wiech et al., 2010; Mutschler et al., 2012; Borsook et al., 2013).

Because empathy also crucially requires high-level integration of emotional, cognitive, and social components as well as of behavioral control, we were specifically interested whether there are structural differences in the insular DRG related to individual empathy and more generally, how empathy-related structural effects map onto functionally defined insular areas related to pain-processing, emotion, and sensorimotor functions. To this end, in the present study VBM findings were related to a recently developed functional map of the AIC and this map was extended to by meta-analysis of previous neuroimaging studies investigating empathy for pain and emotional processing applying the ALE method (Turkeltaub et al., 2002).

Materials and Methods

Subjects

One hundred and one healthy females (mean age = 23.6 years, range = 18–35 years) with no history of psychiatric or neurological diseases (based on a clinical interview by a trained psychologist) were recruited from the local University, ensuring a comparable/similar educational background. All participants were right-handed according to the Edinburgh Handedness Questionnaire (Oldfield, 1971): mean = 83.7%, range = 58.3–100%. This study was approved by the local ethics committee for medical research in Basel. Before participation, subjects signed written informed consent. They received a modest monetary compensation for participation.

Data Acquisition

T1-weighted high-resolution images were collected with a 3 T scanner (Siemens Magnetom Allegra syngo MR 2004A, Erlangen, Germany) using a MPRAGE sequence (resolution: 1 mm isotropic, matrix: 256 × 256 × 176, TR: 2000 ms, TI: 1000 ms, 7° flip angle). There were no structural abnormalities in any of the MRIs. To measure individual differences in trait empathy we used the E-Scale, a commonly used and validated 25-item self-evaluation measure by (Leibetseder et al., 2001). Twenty-two of the items are worded positively so that agreement indicates higher empathy. The other three items are worded negatively so that disagreement indicates higher empathy. The technique of balancing positively and negatively worded items helps to control for acquiescence bias. Thirteen items assess empathy in fictitious and 12 items empathy in real-life situations. Each items has to be answered on a 7-point Likert scale. The total score is computed by adding up the participant's responses to the 22 positively and 3 negatively worded items. This German version has been developed from English instruments assessing empathy (Mehrabian and Epstein, 1972; Stotland et al., 1978; Davis, 1983). For a more detailed discussion of the psychometric properties of the measure and how it was developed see reference Leibetseder et al. (2001). A more recent study shows that the E-Scale assesses cognitive and emotional components of empathy and shows that this measure possesses high reliability and validity for the assessment of trait empathy (Leibetseder et al., 2007).

Voxel-Based Morphometry (VBM) Analysis

DARTEL-based VBM analyses of combined gray-white maps were performed on the structural MRIs. Data were processed using the VBM8 toolbox (http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm8/) and the SPM8 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) software package. The images were segmented into GM, white matter (WM) using SPM8, and then normalized with the Dartel normalization, using the VBM8 Dartel template (Ashburner, 2007), including a modulation step. A manual quality control and inspection of the processing steps and outputs was performed. Finally, normalized images were smoothed with a 6-mm full-width at half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. Statistical analysis was carried out by means of voxelwise whole brain correlation of MR signal intensities with the individuals' empathy scores. Similar to several previous VBM studies (Kim et al., 2008; Labate et al., 2008; Peters et al., 2009; Heuser et al., 2011; Herringa et al., 2012), we report our results at p < 0.001, uncorrected, combined with a conservative threshold on cluster size, requiring more than 150 contiguous significant voxels (k > 150). Normal distribution of the questionnaire data was tested with the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, and analyzed using SPSS (Version 19.0, Chicago, IL, USA).

Activation Likelihood Estimate (ALE) Meta-Analysis

Search criteria

A search of Medline (Medical Literature Analysis and Retrieval System Online) and PsycINFO (Psychological Information Database) was performed. No start data limit on the search criteria if the databases were set but the end date was end of December 2011. The search keywords in title and abstracts were “empathy,” “pain,” “noxious stimuli,” “emotion,” “brain imaging,” “insula,” “insular cortex,” “functional magnetic resonance imaging” (fMRI”), “positron emission tomography” (“PET”).

Inclusion criteria

(1) The article had to be published in a peer-reviewed journal; (2) The study investigated empathy for pain, or emotional processing or experimentally induced physical pain in individuals using fMRI or PET; (3) Reported insular peaks that lay within 5 mm of the insular border (for more details see below); (4) Provided Talairach or Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates; (5) Included hand movement (e.g., button press) only if two experimental conditions assumed to have equal movement-related activity were contrasted; this was selected to rule out movement-related effects, which are known to activate a region in the AIC (Mutschler et al., 2007, 2009); (6) Examined healthy adult individuals that did not suffer from any neurological or psychiatric disorder. The studies included in the ALE analyses according to these inclusion criteria are listed in the Appendix.

Analyses

Talairach coordinates were translated to the MNI space using the tal2mni Matlab script from http://eeg.sourceforge.net/doc_m2html/bioelectromagnetism/tal2mni.html. Subsequently, an ALE, given by the union of the probabilities associated with the different foci, was calculated for an area comprising the whole Y and Z extent of the insular cortex. The ALE was calculated across studies such that the summed ALE of all peaks of each study was normalized to unity to ensure that studies reporting large numbers of peaks cannot disproportionally dominate the resulting ALE maps. The average extent of the insular cortex was determined from the T1 multi-subject template provided with SPM5. The anatomical boundaries of the insula are described in the study by Makris et al. (2006). The procedure as described so far generates ALE maps for each of the modalities/categories investigated. Peaks that lay more than 5 mm outside the border of the insula were regarded as outliers and were excluded. The spatial union of all ALE values finally could be portrayed as an ALE-value map which differentially describes the reproducibility of an effect within different insular subregions. Statistical significance was assessed using a (non-parametric) single threshold permutation test as already described in previous works (Turkeltaub et al., 2002; Laird et al., 2005). ALE scores inferred from the distribution of activation foci reported in the included studies of one functional category was compared against a single critical ALE score derived from an empirical null distribution. This null distribution resulted from 10,000 permutations of an equal number of foci equal to the sample used to generate the ALE that were distributed randomly throughout the insular cortex. The threshold was set at p < 0.01. This means that only ALE-values that extend a critical ALE value corresponding to a significance level of α = 0.01 are indicated in the ALE maps.

Definition of the Domain General Region (DGR)

The DGR in the AIC (red dashed circle in Figure 2) was defined based on the study of Dosenbach and colleagues who, based on mixed design fMRI experiments using 10 different tasks, found the bilateral anterior insula/frontal operculum region to show reliable start-cue and sustained activations across all or nearly all tasks (Dosenbach et al., 2006) which was interpreted as a representation of task sets. This generalized type of activation was found in the dorsal part of the AIC (black dot in Figure 2). It is clear that the DGR cannot be reduced to a single point corresponding to the reported peak coordinates. On the other hand, the exact size/spatial extent of the DRG is currently unclear. We defined the DGR large enough to encompass a peak related to meta-analytically defined, supramodal aesthetic appraisal (Brown et al., 2011) (back triangle in Figure 2A), as this supramodal processing may be an aspect of the domain general function defining the DGR. Our conclusions, however, do not critically depend on the size assigned to the DGR, because the peak reported by Dosenbach et al. (2006) directly falls into the region with empathy-related VBM effects (Figure 2B) and, therefore, an overlap between any DGR defined based on the study (Dosenbach et al., 2006) with our VBM effects exists in any case, independent of the exact radius assumed for the DGR.

Results

Overall empathy scores (M = 91.6, SD = 13.3) of the E-Scale (Leibetseder et al., 2001) were normally distributed (Kolmogorov–Smirnov Test, p = 0.58). Means for the overall empathy score (M = 3.66, SD = 0.53) were similar to a previous study investigating a comparable sample of healthy individuals using the same questionnaire (M = 3.56, SD = 0.61). For more details see supplement of reference Krach et al. (2011). The VBM analysis revealed a single cluster of 185 voxels in the left AIC showed a significant positive correlation between individual empathy scores of the 101 subjects and GM density (p < 0.001, extent threshold k > 1.0 voxels, Figure 1). There were two peaks within this cluster at Montreal Neurologic Institute (MNI) x/y/z coordinates—30/7.5/4.5 (T = 4.17) and at—24/18/6 (T = 3.65).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Significant correlation in the left dorsal anterior insular cortex (AIC) between individual empathy scores in 101 healthy females and individual cortical gray values (p < 0.001). Axial slice at the (MNI) x/y/z coordinate [−30, 7.5, 4.5] (global, maximal correlation). T-values are color-coded. (B) Correlation of individual gray matter density (arbitrary units) and empathy scores at the peak coordinates from (A). We report these correlations for descriptive purposes. Our conclusions are entirely based on the findings as shown in (A) because one has to keep in mind that the strength of correlation at peak coordinates as shown in (B) may overestimate the true effect (Vul et al., 2009).

Sixteen fMRI studies on empathy for pain (comprising 344 individuals, 191 females, 42 peaks, 23 in the left insula), 44 studies on emotion in healthy subjects (comprising 756 subjects and 46 peaks in the left insula) and 57 studies on physical pain in healthy individuals (comprising 690 subjects and 88 peaks in the left insula) fulfilled the inclusion criteria for the ALE meta-analyses (see Materials and Methods). Studies on pain predominantly used thermal heat stimuli (n = 29). Thirteen studies used PET and 44 studies used fMRI. Studies on emotion used PET (N = 13) or fMRI (n = 31). The studies included in the ALE analyses according to our inclusion criteria are listed in Tables AC.

In the left hemisphere, pain-related maximal ALE in healthy subjects were found in the posterior and dorsal mid-anterior insula, whereas in the right hemisphere maximal ALE was located in the dorsal mid-anterior insula. We further found that emotion-related maximal ALE in healthy individuals—excluding activation peaks in the insula related to the measurement of peripheral physiological changes—was located in the dorsal anterior insula. The maximal ALE related to studies investigating empathy for pain predominantly evoked activation in the dorsal part of the AIC. Figure 2 shows ALE findings.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Activation likelihood estimate (ALE) findings in the left insula and the MNI z coordinates (on the y-axis) and y coordinates (on the x-axis) related to the physical pain investigated with fMRI (red), emotion (yellow), empathy for pain (purple), hand (turquoise), and foot movement (green). The solid gray line indicates the mean outline of the left sagittal insula and the dashed gray line the central sulcus of the insula dividing the insula in an anterior and posterior part (see methods for more details). Importantly, studies on emotion in healthy individuals entering this ALE analysis did not include insula-coordinates which were related to the measurement of peripheral physiological changes. In a recent meta-analysis responses related to peripheral physiological changes resulting from emotional experiences were located in the ventral anterior insular cortex (Mutschler et al., 2009). Pain-related maximal ALE were found in the posterior insula (left) and in the dorsal anterior insula [left and right, see reference Mutschler et al. (2012) for more details]. Response peaks related to task-set processing based on conjoint analysis of data from 10 different fMRI tasks are indicated by red dashed circles [that is the approximate position of the domain-general region (DGR) of the insular cortex described in reference Dosenbach et al. (2006)], and peaks related to social norm violation (Sanfey et al., 2003), meta-analytically defined, supra-modal aesthetic appraisal (Brown et al., 2011), and sense of agency of hand movements (Farrer and Frith, 2002) by black squares, triangles, and star, respectively. Sense of agency of hand movements overlapped with sensorimotor processing. (B) Empathy-related voxel-based morphometric (VBM) findings in the left insula (indicated by black squares) overlapped with empathy and emotion and sensorimotor-related ALE, but also with the DGR. Blue dots represent empathy for pain-related coordinates from reference Singer et al. (2004).

Discussion

Our results indicate regionally specific structural differences in the left dorsal AIC related to individual empathy scores in healthy individuals. GM density correlated in a confined region of the left dorsal AIC with empathy in healthy females. The insular region where we find structural differences overlaps with the peaks from a previous functional study correlating empathy-related responses fMRI with individual empathy scores (Singer et al., 2004), Figure 2B. This precise spatial correspondence strongly supports our VBM findings. Studies show that individuals with specialized abilities have markedly developed brain structures in specific regions related to their expertise (Kanai and Rees, 2011). For instance, an investigation by Maguire et al. on London taxi drivers found that GM measures derived from T1-weighted structural MRI are sensitive to experience. The authors showed that taxi drivers, with their knowledge of London's complex street plan, had enlarged posterior hippocampi in comparison to control subjects (Maguire et al., 2000). More recently, a longitudinal study demonstrates that learning to juggle increases GM density in occipito-parietal cortical areas in the adult brain involved in reaching and grasping (Scholz et al., 2009).

The results reveal that our morphometrically identified area in the dorsal AIC related to individual differences in empathy overlaps the DGR. A first indication toward a DGR was provided by Dosenbach et al. who conjointly analyzed mixed design fMRI experiments using 10 different tasks (Dosenbach et al., 2006). They found, among others, the bilateral anterior insula/frontal operculum region to show reliable start-cue and sustained activations across all or nearly all tasks. This generalized type of activation was found in the dorsal part of the AIC (black dot in Figure 2). The idea of a DGR was extended by Craig in his review on awareness and the AIC (Craig, 2009). The author observed that, in imaging studies, the AIC is reported to be activated in an astonishing number of studies from a broad range of topics including all types of subjective feelings, attention, cognitive choices, intentions, music, time perception, awareness of sensations, and movements, of visual and auditory percepts, of the trustworthiness of other individuals and concluded that “No other region of the brain is activated in all of these tasks.” In a subsequent meta-analysis of a wide range of functional imaging studies, the same region of the AIC that showed task-set-related responses in the study by Dosenbach et al. was also reliably activated by a wide range of auditory and language tasks as well as during social norm violation (Mutschler et al., 2009). Importantly, this study was restricted to studies which reported clear insular responses, i.e., ambiguous effects such as “insula/frontal operculum” were excluded, giving strong additional support for the existence of a domain-general region in the AIC. The idea of a DGR that is activated across nearly all kinds of tasks as well as Craig's observation that no other brain region shows such generalized responses was later also confirmed in a meta-analysis (Kurth et al., 2010). However, little is known regarding the exact location and the functional meaning of the DGR in the dorsal AIC. There is good evidence that the DGR is found in the dorsal anterior insula as indicated in Figure 2A. Importantly, compared to a previous interpretation (Brown et al., 2011) our ALE analysis shows that movement-related responses do not (or only to a very small degree) overlap with the domain-general area. The position of the main region with reproducible movement-related responses is located just posterior to the domain-general region (blue area in Figure 2). This movement-related area closely co-localized with insular peaks related to the sense of agency of hand movements (Mutschler et al., 2009). What is the functional meaning of the DGR—in particular for empathy? Several overarching functions have been proposed such as task-set processing (Dosenbach et al., 2006) and a role in awareness (Craig, 2009). Importantly, our findings—that the morphometrically identified area in the dorsal AIC related to individual differences in empathy overlaps the DGR—support the notion that its underlying neuronal substrate may be involved in integrating socio-emotional information during empathy.

We find that emotion-related responses in healthy subjects were preferentially located in the dorsal AIC. This region was distinct from the insular region associated with peripheral physiological changes resulting from emotional experiences and found that this activity was represented in the ventral AIC that was also the most likely site of insular co-activation with the amygdala (Mutschler et al., 2009). In the current study, insula-coordinates associated with emotion-related peripheral physiological changes were excluded. Therefore, our findings suggest that distinct functional insula regions may be involved in different aspects of emotional processing, whereby peripheral physiological correlates of emotional processing are mapped to the ventral anterior regions, while emotion-cognitive processes are mapped to a more dorsal-anterior region. More specifically, we assume that the dorsal anterior insula might play a pivotal role in integrating sensory stimuli with its salience, possibly via connections to the cingulate cortex. This interpretation is supported by the fact that in our study-sample subjects evaluated the emotional content of the presented stimuli and in direct agreement with recent findings showing that the dorsal anterior insula is more consistently involved in human cognition than ventral anterior and posterior networks (Chang et al., 2013), and the observed functional connectivity between the dorsal anterior insula and the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, which that plays a crucial role in cognitive decision-making (Deen et al., 2011). Pain-related maximal ALE were found in the posterior insula and in the dorsal AIC. Both, emotion and pain related ALE findings are discussed in more detail elsewhere (Mutschler et al., 2012) because the aim of this meta-analysis was to relate them to studies on empathy. Recent neuroimaging studies show that the anterior insula and the anterior cingulate cortex are engaged during both, the experience and observation of pain (Singer et al., 2004; Lamm et al., 2011). It has been suggested that neural responses involved in both conditions might reflect a neuronal substrate which is related to the affective but not sensory aspect of pain (Singer et al., 2004). Together, our present ALE-findings support this notion and suggest that the posterior insula is involved in processing sensory aspect of pain, whereas the dorsal anterior insula is involved in emotion. In summary, we argue that the dorsal AIC plays a pivotal role in empathy (similarly as during emotion processing and pain) by integrating sensory stimuli with its salience, possibly via connections to the cingulate cortex. This assumption is also supported by the fact that ALE-findings related to emotion and empathy for pain and also the DGR—which has been associated with cognition—overlap in the dorsal anterior insula, suggesting that these functions share a common neural substrate (Dosenbach et al., 2006). As mentioned above we assume that the overall role of the morphometrically identified area in the dorsal AIC related to individual differences in empathy which overlaps the DGR might be involved in integrating information which is relevant for socio-emotional and cognitive processing. Thus, we assume that empathy is not (only) related to a specific “socio-emotional” interaction area, but to a superordinate “domain-general” area, in line with concepts of empathy that include not only social and emotional, but also cognitive aspects (Eisenberg and Miller, 1987; Decety and Jackson, 2004). Whether our findings in the dorsal AIC have also a relation to the “von Economo neurons” [VENs, (Von Economo, 1926)] remains to be determined. VENs have been hypothesized to play are role in social-emotional processing including empathy (Evrard et al., 2012; Seeley et al., 2012).

In the following paragraph potential limitations of this study are discussed and suggestions are made for future research. Similarly to previous imaging studies (e.g., Singer et al., 2004) we have investigated correlates of overall empathy in our study by using a widely applied and validated self-evaluation measure (Leibetseder et al., 2001, 2007). There is a potential concern about the influence of the social desirability bias—which refers to the tendency of subjects to answer self report items in a self-favoring manner—on the validity of questionnaire-based research (Edwards, 1957). It is discussed whether social desirability scales may be used to detect, minimize, and correct for social desirability bias in order to improve the validity of questionnaire-based research (e.g., Uziel, 2010). In a future study it would be interesting to measure empathy experimentally—e.g., by investigating the impact of compassion-based meditation on empathy (Mascaro et al., 2013)—and to relate functional activity to GM properties.

In this study, brain structure changes resulting from or predisposing to empathy have been investigated in a large sample of females because of increasing evidence for sex differences in empathy. Females score on average higher than males on self-report measures of empathy (Hoffman, 1977; Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004). Singer et al. observed in an fMRI study on empathy for pain that in males but not females empathetic reactions were absent for persons who were perceived as behaving unfairly (Singer et al., 2006). Recently, Van Honk et al. (2011) showed that the administration of the androgen hormone testosterone—which represents the largest hormonal difference between the sexes—impaired cognitive empathy in healthy females. As mentioned, the insular region where we find structural differences exactly overlaps with the peaks from a previous functional study correlating empathy-related responses fMRI with individual empathy scores (Singer et al., 2004). Notably, in this study also only females were investigated. A recent study found in a sample with mixed gender individual differences in trait empathy dimensions correlating with morphological differences in several brain areas including the anterior cingulated cortex and the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Banissy et al., 2012). More specifically, based on an analysis with ROIs around peaks from previous functional studies on empathy, Banissy et al. report structural changes in the ventral-most part of the insular cortex, several centimeters apart from the area characterized in the present study. Interpretational difficulties however arise because (1) the peak at MNI coordinates −39, 9, −21 as reported by Banissy et al. (that was used to define the ventral insular ROI) is pain-related and not empathy-related as was assumed by the authors (see Appendix to Singer et al., 2004) and (2) a second, empathic concern-related peak reported to be in the insular region by Banissy et al. at MNI coordinates −48, 6, 18 is according to the probabilistic assignment obtained from the SPM Anatomy Toolbox (Eickhoff et al., 2005) located in Brodmann Area 44. In this study, structural changes specific to males or females were however not addressed and only changes that were consistent across the whole (mixed) sample were reported which may possibly explain the different results of their and our study. Alternatively, the different results between both studies may also be explained by the fact that in our study the focus was on global empathy whereas in the study by Banissy et al. on the relationship between components of empathy (empathic concern, personal distress, perspective taking, and fantasizing) and brain structure using a different measure. Because empathy crucially requires high-level integration of emotional, cognitive, and social components as well as of behavioral control the goal of our study was not to investigate different components of empathy but to explore the neural substrate that may underlay its neural integration. Our interpretation—that the dorsal AIC where we find structural differences depending on individual empathy might play a crucial role in modulating the efficiency of neural integration underlying emotional, cognitive, and sensorimotor information which is essential for global empathy—is in agreement with previous studies reporting that reduced GM volume in the AIC was associated with a lack of empathy in neuropsychiatric disorders such as in conduct disorder (Sterzer et al., 2007) and in psychopathy (de Oliveira-Souza et al., 2008). Further, as mentioned above, a recent study in combat veterans with traumatic brain injury shows that lesions in several brain regions, particularly in the insula, was associated with deficits in empathy (Driscoll et al., 2012). In future research based on larger samples it would be interesting to investigate the role of empathy subscales such as cognitive and emotional components (Leibetseder et al., 2007), and their relation to functional and structural data. The emotional component of empathy has been for instance closely linked to activation in the inferior frontal gyrus (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009; Banissy et al., 2012).

Finally, longitudinal studies are needed to clarify whether the empathy-related structural effects in the dorsal AIC that we find are due to a pre-existing brain characteristic or to empathy-experience, or whether it indicates both.

In addition, all neuroimaging studies entering our meta-analysis reported that only healthy individuals free of any neurological and psychiatric disorders were investigated. However, for future meta-analyses it would be important that neuroimaging studies specify more clearly the procedure regarding how they assessed and excluded individuals with neurological and psychiatric disorders [e.g., whether a structured clinical interview for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual IV (DSM-IV) axis I and axis II personality disorders was used]. Furthermore, it is also important to note that our meta-analysis on empathy included females and males. An interesting topic for future meta-analyses would be to examine whether there are functional differences between male and female samples with respect to empathy. Finally, future meta-analyses should also investigate whether the dorsal AIC is rather involved in empathy for pain (Singer et al., 2004) and social rejection (Eisenberger et al., 2003) than in empathy for positive emotions and if yes why this could be the case. Only few brain imaging studies to date have examined empathy for positive emotions (Jabbi et al., 2007; Mobbs et al., 2009; Morelli et al., 2012), therefore ALE meta-analysis could not be performed. If the dorsal AIC is related to empathy and is essential for high-level integration, it should be activated during empathy for all types of emotions. However, the few studies on empathy for positive emotions suggest that the dorsal AIC might not be involved (Mobbs et al., 2009; Morelli et al., 2012), but a meta-analytic analysis based on a large sample of studies would be required to resolve this issue. In future it would be also important to meta-analytically examine studies on empathy for negative emotions which show insula activation, such as empathy for disgust (Wicker et al., 2003), embarrassment (Krach et al., 2011), social rejection (Masten et al., 2011), and anxiety (Morelli et al., 2012), as well as studies on functional components of empathy.

In summary, the dorsal AIC where we find structural differences depending on individual empathy may be key in modulating the efficiency of neural integration underlying emotional, cognitive, and sensorimotor information which is essential for empathy. Furthermore, our results support a functional subdivision of the human insula in functionally distinct regions. These include the ventral anterior insula which is involved in mapping peripheral physiological information during emotional experiences and the dorsal AIC which plays a crucial role in integrating sensory stimuli with salience possibly via connections to the cingulate cortex. The dorsal anterior insula constitutes an auditory and language area and the mid anterior insula plays a pivotal role in sensorimotor processing. Finally, the posterior insula may be involved in processing sensory aspects of nociceptive information and the dorsal AIC may have an integrative role during emotional-cognitive evaluation of a noxious stimuli and the associated sensorimotor response. Together, these findings provide new important insights into the functional organization of the human insular cortex in healthy individuals, and the functional map may be helpful for understanding dysfunction in conditions affecting empathy such as borderline personality disorder with co-morbid posttraumatic stress disorder, autistic spectrum disorders, psychopathy/antisocial personality disorder, conduct disorder, and schizophrenia.

Funding

We gratefully acknowledge financial support by the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF fellowship to Isabella Mutschler).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge Dr. Bud Craig for his advice on insula anatomy and Markus Klarhöfer and Birgit Wieckhorst for their help recording the structural images.

References

Ashburner, J. (2007). A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage 38, 95–113.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Banissy, M. J., Kanai, R., Walsh, V., and Rees, G. (2012). Inter-individual differences in empathy are reflected in human brain structure. Neuroimage 62, 2034–2039.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baron-Cohen, S., and Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 163–175.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bora, E., Gokcen, S., and Veznedaroglu, B. (2008). Empathic abilities in people with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 160, 23–29.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Borsook, D., Edwards, R., Elman, I., Becerra, L., and Levine, J. (2013). Pain and analgesia: the value of salience circuits. Prog. Neurobiol. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.02.003. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brown, S., Gao, X., Tisdelle, L., Eickhoff, S. B., and Liotti, M. (2011). Naturalizing aesthetics: brain areas for aesthetic appraisal across sensory modalities. Neuroimage 58, 250–258.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cauda, F., D'Agata, F., Sacco, K., Duca, S., Geminiani, G., and Vercelli, A. (2011). Functional connectivity of the insula in the resting brain. Neuroimage 55, 8–23.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chang, L. J., Yarkoni, T., Khaw, M. W., and Sanfey, A. G. (2013). Decoding the role of the insula in human cognition: functional parcellation and large-scale reverse inference. Cereb. Cortex 23, 739–749.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 59–70.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual-differences in empathy—evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126.

Decety, J., and Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 3, 71–100.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Decety, J., and Moriguchi, Y. (2007). The empathic brain and its dysfunction in psychiatric populations: implications for intervention across different clinical conditions. Biopsychosoc. Med. 1:22. doi: 10.1186/1751-0759-1-22

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Deen, B., Pitskel, N. B., and Pelphrey, K. A. (2011). Three systems of insular functional connectivity identified with cluster analysis. Cereb. Cortex 21, 1498–1506.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

de Oliveira-Souza, R., Hare, R. D., Bramati, I. E., Garrido, G. J., Azevedo Ignacio, F., Tovar-Moll, F., et al. (2008). Psychopathy as a disorder of the moral brain: fronto-temporo-limbic grey matter reductions demonstrated by voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage 40, 1202–1213.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dosenbach, N. U., Visscher, K. M., Palmer, E. D., Miezin, F. M., Wenger, K. K., Kang, H. C., et al. (2006). A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron 50, 799–812.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Driscoll, D. M., Dal Monte, O., Solomon, J., Krueger, F., and Grafman, J. (2012). Empathic deficits in combat veterans with traumatic brain injury: a voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping study. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 25, 160–166.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dymond, R. F. (1949). A scale for the measurement of empathic ability. J. Consult. Psychol. 13, 127–133.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Edwards, A. L. (1957). The Social Desirability Variable in Personality Assessment and Research. New York, NY: Dryden.

Eickhoff, S. B., Stephan, K. E., Mohlberg, H., Grefkes, C., Fink, G. R., Amunts, K., et al. (2005). A new SPM toolbox for combining probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps and functional imaging data. Neuroimage 25, 1325–1335.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eisenberg, N., and Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychol. Bull. 101, 91–119.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., and Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science 302, 290–292.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Evrard, H. C., Forro, T., and Logothetis, N. K. (2012). Von economo neurons in the anterior insula of the macaque monkey. Neuron 74, 482–489.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Farrer, C., and Frith, C. D. (2002). Experiencing oneself vs another person as being the cause of an action: the neural correlates of the experience of agency. Neuroimage 15, 596–603.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Goleman, D. (1998). Working with Emotional Intelligence. New York, NY: Bantam Books.

Herringa, R., Phillips, M., Almeida, J., Insana, S., and Germain, A. (2012). Post-traumatic stress symptoms correlate with smaller subgenual cingulate, caudate, and insula volumes in unmedicated combat veterans. Psychiatry Res. 203, 139–145.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Heuser, M., Thomann, P. A., Essig, M., Bachmann, S., and Schroder, J. (2011). Neurological signs and morphological cerebral changes in schizophrenia: an analysis of NSS subscales in patients with first episode psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 192, 69–76.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hoffman, M. L. (1977). Sex differences in empathy and related behaviors. Psychol. Bull. 84, 712–722.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Jabbi, M., Swart, M., and Keysers, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex. Neuroimage 34, 1744–1753.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Johansen-Berg, H. (2012). The future of functionally-related structural change assessment. Neuroimage 62, 1293–1298.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kanai, R., and Rees, G. (2011). The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 231–242.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, J. H., Suh, S. I., Seol, H. Y., Oh, K., Seo, W. K., Yu, S. W., et al. (2008). Regional grey matter changes in patients with migraine: a voxel-based morphometry study. Cephalalgia 28, 598–604.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Krach, S., Cohrs, J. C., de Echeverria Loebell, N. C., Kircher, T., Sommer, J., Jansen, A., et al. (2011). Your flaws are my pain: linking empathy to vicarious embarrassment. PLoS ONE 6:e18675. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018675

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kurth, F., Zilles, K., Fox, P. T., Laird, A. R., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). A link between the systems: functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 519–534.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Labate, A., Cerasa, A., Gambardella, A., Aguglia, U., and Quattrone, A. (2008). Hippocampal and thalamic atrophy in mild temporal lobe epilepsy: a VBM study. Neurology 71, 1094–1101.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laird, A. R., Fox, P. M., Price, C. J., Glahn, D. C., Uecker, A. M., Lancaster, J. L., et al. (2005). ALE meta-analysis: controlling the false discovery rate and performing statistical contrasts. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 155–164.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lamm, C., Decety, J., and Singer, T. (2011). Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage 54, 2492–2502.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leibetseder, M., Laireiter, A. R., and Koller, T. (2007). Structural analysis of the E-scale. Pers. Individ. Dif. 42, 547–561.

Leibetseder, M., Laireiter, A.-R., and Riepler, A. (2001). E-Scale: questionnaire for the assessment of empathy in fictitious as well as real-life situations—Description and psychometric properties. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie 22, 70–85.

Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., et al. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 4398–4403.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Makris, N., Goldstein, J. M., Kennedy, D., Hodge, S. M., Caviness, V. S., Faraone, S. V., et al. (2006). Decreased volume of left and total anterior insular lobule in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 83, 155–171.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marangoni, C., Garcia, S., Ickes, W., and Teng, G. (1995). Empathic accuracy in a clinically relevant setting. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 854–869.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mascaro, J. S., Rilling, J. K., Tenzin Negi, L., and Raison, C. L. (2013). Compassion meditation enhances empathic accuracy and related neural activity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 48–55.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Masten, C. L., Morelli, S. A., and Eisenberger, N. I. (2011). An fMRI investigation of empathy for ‘social pain’ and subsequent prosocial behavior. Neuroimage 55, 381–388.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mehrabian, A., and Epstein, N. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. J. Pers. 40, 525–543.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mesulam, M. M., and Mufson, E. J. (1982). Insula of the old world monkey. III: efferent cortical output and comments on function. J. Comp. Neurol. 212, 38–52.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miller, P. A., and Eisenberg, N. (1988). The relation of empathy to aggressive and externalizing/antisocial behavior. Psychol. Bull. 103, 324–344.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mobbs, D., Yu, R., Meyer, M., Passamonti, L., Seymour, B., Calder, A. J., et al. (2009). A key role for similarity in vicarious reward. Science 324, 900.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morelli, S. A., Rameson, L. T., and Lieberman, M. D. (2012). The neural components of empathy: predicting daily prosocial behavior. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss088. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mufson, E. J., and Mesulam, M. M. (1982). Insula of the old world monkey. II: afferent cortical input and comments on the claustrum. J. Comp. Neurol. 212, 23–37.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mutschler, I., Ball, T., Wankerl, J., and Strigo, I. A. (2012). Pain and emotion in the insular cortex: evidence for functional reorganization in major depression. Neurosci. Lett. 520, 204–209.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mutschler, I., Schulze-Bonhage, A., Glauche, V., Demandt, E., Speck, O., and Ball, T. (2007). A rapid sound-action association effect in human insular cortex. PLoS ONE 2:e259. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000259

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mutschler, I., Wieckhorst, B., Kowalevski, S., Derix, J., Wentlandt, J., Schulze-Bonhage, A., et al. (2009). Functional organization of the human anterior insular cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 457, 66–70.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nieuwenhuys, R. (2012). The insular cortex: a review. Prog. Brain Res. 195, 123–163.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Peters, J., Dauvermann, M., Mette, C., Platen, P., Franke, J., Hinrichs, T., et al. (2009). Voxel-based morphometry reveals an association between aerobic capacity and grey matter density in the right anterior insula. Neuroscience 163, 1102–1108.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Premack, D., and Woodruf, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav. Brain Sci. 1, 515–526.

Preston, S. D. (2007). “A perception-action model for empathy,” in Empathy in Mental Illness, eds T. Farrow and P. Woordruff (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 428–447.

Roepke, S., Vater, A., Preissler, S., Heekeren, H. R., and Dziobek, I. (2012). Social cognition in borderline personality disorder. Front. Neurosci. 6:195. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00195

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., and Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science 300, 1755–1758.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Scholz, J., Klein, M. C., Behrens, T. E., and Johansen-Berg, H. (2009). Training induces changes in white-matter architecture. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1370–1371.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Seeley, W. W., Merkle, F. T., Gaus, S. E., Craig, A. D., Allman, J. M., Hof, P. R., et al. (2012). Distinctive neurons of the anterior cingulate and frontoinsular cortex: a historical perspective. Cereb. Cortex 22, 245–250.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., and Perry, D. (2009). Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain 132, 617–627.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shirtcliff, E. A., Vitacco, M. J., Graf, A. R., Gostisha, A. J., Merz, J. L., and Zahn-Waxler, C. (2009). Neurobiology of empathy and callousness: implications for the development of antisocial behavior. Behav. Sci. Law 27, 137–171.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature 439, 466–469.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sterzer, P., Stadler, C., Poustka, F., and Kleinschmidt, A. (2007). A structural neural deficit in adolescents with conduct disorder and its association with lack of empathy. Neuroimage 37, 335–342.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stotland, E., Kenneth, E., Mathews, J., Sherman, S. E., Hansson, R. O., and Richardson, B. (1978). Empathy, Fantasy and Helping. (Beverly Hills: Sage Library of Social Research), 27–45.

Touroutoglou, A., Hollenbeck, M., Dickerson, B. C., and Feldman Barrett, L. (2012). Dissociable large-scale networks anchored in the right anterior insula subserve affective experience and attention. Neuroimage 60, 1947–1958.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Turkeltaub, P. E., Eden, G. F., Jones, K. M., and Zeffiro, T. A. (2002). Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word reading: method and validation. Neuroimage 16, 765–780.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Uziel, L. (2010). Rethinking social desirability scales: from impression management to interpersonally oriented self-control. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 5, 243–262.

Van Honk, J., Schutter, D. J., Bos, P. A., Kruijt, A. W., Lentjes, E. G., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). Testosterone administration impairs cognitive empathy in women depending on second-to-fourth digit ratio. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 3448–3452.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Von Economo, C. (1926). Eine neue Art Spezialzellen des Lobus cinguli and Lobus insulae. Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 100, 706–712.

Vul, E., Harris, C., Winkielman, P., and Pashler, H. (2009). Puzzlingly high correlations in fmri studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 4, 274–290.

Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in My Insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40, 655–664.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wiech, K., Lin, C. S., Brodersen, K. H., Bingel, U., Ploner, M., and Tracey, I. (2010). Anterior insula integrates information about salience into perceptual decisions about pain. J. Neurosci. 30, 16324–16331.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zaki, J., and Ochsner, K. (2012). The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 675–680.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Appendix

TABLE A
www.frontiersin.org

Table A. Sixteen fMRI imaging studies reporting anterior insula activity related to empathy for pain (comprising 344 individuals, 191 females, and 42 peaks, 23 left and 19 right).

TABLE B
www.frontiersin.org

Table B. Fifty-seven neuroimaging studies investigating pain in healthy subjects entered the ALE meta-analyses.

TABLE C
www.frontiersin.org

Table C. Fourty-four imaging studies on emotional processing in healthy subjects.

References

Akitsuki, Y., and Decety, J. (2009). Social context and perceived agency affects empathy for pain: an event-related fMRI investigation. Neuroimage 47, 722–734.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Albuquerque, R. J., de Leeuw, R., Carlson, C. R., Okeson, J. P., Miller, C. S., and Andersen, A. H. (2006). Cerebral activation during thermal stimulation of patients who have burning mouth disorder: an fMRI study. Pain 122, 223–234.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Anand, A., Li, Y., Wang, Y., Wu, J., Gao, S., Bukhari, L., et al. (2005). Activity and connectivity of brain mood regulating circuit in depression: a functional magnetic resonance study. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1079–1088.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bartels, A., and Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love. Neuroreport 11, 3829–3834.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bartels, A., and Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. Neuroimage 21, 1155–1166.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Becerra, L., Breiter, H. C., Wise, R., Gonzalez, R. G., and Borsook, D. (2001). Reward circuitry activation by noxious thermal stimuli. Neuron 32, 927–946.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bingel, U., Quante, M., Knab, R., Bromm, B., Weiller, C., and Buchel, C. (2003). Single trial fMRI reveals significant contralateral bias in responses to laser pain within thalamus and somatosensory cortices. Neuroimage 18, 740–748.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bingel, U., Schoell, E., Herken, W., Buchel, C., and May, A. (2007). Habituation to painful stimulation involves the antinociceptive system. Pain 131, 21–30.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blood, A. J., and Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 11818–11823.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Botvinick, M., Jha, A. P., Bylsma, L. M., Fabian, S. A., Solomon, P. E., and Prkachin, K. M. (2005). Viewing facial expressions of pain engages cortical areas involved in the direct experience of pain. Neuroimage 25, 312–319.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Britton, J. C., Phan, K. L., Taylor, S. F., Fig, L. M., and Liberzon, I. (2005). Corticolimbic blood flow in posttraumatic stress disorder during script-driven imagery. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 832–840.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Britton, J. C., Taylor, S. F., Sudheimer, K. D., and Liberzon, I. (2006). Facial expressions and complex IAPS pictures: common and differential networks. Neuroimage 31, 906–919.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brooks, J. C., Nurmikko, T. J., Bimson, W. E., Singh, K. D., and Roberts, N. (2002). fMRI of thermal pain: effects of stimulus laterality and attention. Neuroimage 15, 293–301.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brooks, J. C., Zambreanu, L., Godinez, A., Craig, A. D., and Tracey, I. (2005). Somatotopic organisation of the human insula to painful heat studied with high resolution functional imaging. Neuroimage 27, 201–209.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Calder, A. J., Beaver, J. D., Davis, M. H., van Ditzhuijzen, J., Keane, J., and Lawrence, A. D. (2007). Disgust sensitivity predicts the insula and pallidal response to pictures of disgusting foods. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 3422–3428.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carlsson, K., Andersson, J., Petrovic, P., Petersson, K. M., Ohman, A., and Ingvar, M. (2006). Predictability modulates the affective and sensory-discriminative neural processing of pain. Neuroimage 32, 1804–1814.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Casey, K. L., Morrow, T. J., Lorenz, J., and Minoshima, S. (2001). Temporal and spatial dynamics of human forebrain activity during heat pain: analysis by positron emission tomography. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 951–959.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cheng, Y., Chen, C., Lin, C. P., Chou, K. H., and Decety, J. (2010). Love hurts: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 51, 923–929.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cheng, Y., Lin, C. P., Liu, H. L., Hsu, Y. Y., Lim, K. E., Hung, D., et al. (2007). Expertise modulates the perception of pain in others. Curr. Biol. 17, 1708–1713.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Christmann, C., Koeppe, C., Braus, D. F., Ruf, M., and Flor, H. (2007). A simultaneous EEG-fMRI study of painful electric stimulation. Neuroimage 34, 1428–1437.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cole, L. J., Farrell, M. J., Gibson, S. J., and Egan, G. F. (2010). Age-related differences in pain sensitivity and regional brain activity evoked by noxious pressure. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 494–503.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L. L., Parvizi, J., et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1049–1056.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Danziger, N., Faillenot, I., and Peyron, R. (2009). Can we share a pain we never felt? Neural correlates of empathy in patients with congenital insensitivity to pain. Neuron 61, 203–212.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Decety, J., Echols, S., and Correll, J. (2010). The blame game: the effect of responsibility and social stigma on empathy for pain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 985–997.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

de Leeuw, R., Albuquerque, R. J., Andersen, A. H., and Carlson, C. R. (2006a). Influence of estrogen on brain activation during stimulation with painful heat. J. Oral Maxillofac. Surg. 64, 158–166.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

de Leeuw, R., Davis, C. E., Albuquerque, R., Carlson, C. R., and Andersen, A. H. (2006b). Brain activity during stimulation of the trigeminal nerve with noxious heat. Oral. Surg. Oral. Med. Oral. Pathol. Oral. Radiol. Endod. 102, 750–757.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Derbyshire, S. W., and Jones, A. K. (1998). Cerebral responses to a continual tonic pain stimulus measured using positron emission tomography. Pain 76, 127–135.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Derbyshire, S. W., Jones, A. K., Gyulai, F., Clark, S., Townsend, D., and Firestone, L. L. (1997). Pain processing during three levels of noxious stimulation produces differential patterns of central activity. Pain 73, 431–445.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Derbyshire, S. W., and Osborn, J. (2009). Offset analgesia is mediated by activation in the region of the periaqueductal grey and rostral ventromedial medulla. Neuroimage 47, 1002–1006.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Donix, M., Poettrich, K., Weiss, P. H., Werner, A., von Kummer, R., Fink, G. R., et al. (2010). Age-dependent differences in the neural mechanisms supporting long-term declarative memories. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 25, 383–395.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dube, A. A., Duquette, M., Roy, M., Lepore, F., Duncan, G., and Rainville, P. (2009). Brain activity associated with the electrodermal reactivity to acute heat pain. Neuroimage 45, 169–180.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ducreux, D., Attal, N., Parker, F., and Bouhassira, D. (2006). Mechanisms of central neuropathic pain: a combined psychophysical and fMRI study in syringomyelia. Brain 129, 963–976.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ethofer, T., Kreifelts, B., Wiethoff, S., Wolf, J., Grodd, W., Vuilleumier, P., et al. (2009). Differential influences of emotion, task, and novelty on brain regions underlying the processing of speech melody. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 1255–1268.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eugene, F., Levesque, J., Mensour, B., Leroux, J. M., Beaudoin, G., Bourgouin, P., et al. (2003). The impact of individual differences on the neural circuitry underlying sadness. Neuroimage 19, 354–364.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fink, G. R., Markowitsch, H. J., Reinkemeier, M., Bruckbauer, T., Kessler, J., and Heiss, W. D. (1996). Cerebral representation of one's own past: neural networks involved in autobiographical memory. J. Neurosci. 16, 4275–4282.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Fitzgerald, D. A., Posse, S., Moore, G. J., Tancer, M. E., Nathan, P. J., and Phan, K. L. (2004). Neural correlates of internally-generated disgust via autobiographical recall: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Neurosci. Lett. 370, 91–96.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frankenstein, U. N., Richter, W., McIntyre, M. C., and Remy, F. (2001). Distraction modulates anterior cingulate gyrus activations during the cold pressor test. Neuroimage 14, 827–836.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Freund, W., Stuber, G., Wunderlich, A. P., and Schmitz, B. (2007). Cortical correlates of perception and suppression of electrically induced pain. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 24, 203–212.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Garrett, A. S., and Maddock, R. J. (2006). Separating subjective emotion from the perception of emotion-inducing stimuli: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 33, 263–274.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

George, M. S., Ketter, T. A., Parekh, P. I., Herscovitch, P., and Post, R. M. (1996). Gender differences in regional cerebral blood flow during transient self-induced sadness or happiness. Biol. Psychiatry 40, 859–871.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gracely, R. H., Petzke, F., Wolf, J. M., and Clauw, D. J. (2002). Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence of augmented pain processing in fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheum. 46, 1333–1343.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gu, X., Liu, X., Guise, K. G., Naidich, T. P., Hof, P. R., and Fan, J. (2010). Functional dissociation of the frontoinsular and anterior cingulate cortices in empathy for pain. J. Neurosci. 30, 3739–3744.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gundel, H., O'Connor, M. F., Littrell, L., Fort, C., and Lane, R. D. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of grief: an FMRI study. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 1946–1953.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gundel, H., Valet, M., Sorg, C., Huber, D., Zimmer, C., Sprenger, T., et al. (2008). Altered cerebral response to noxious heat stimulation in patients with somatoform pain disorder. Pain 137, 413–421.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Han, S., Fan, Y., Xu, X., Qin, J., Wu, B., Wang, X., et al. (2009). Empathic neural responses to others' pain are modulated by emotional contexts. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 3227–3237.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hofbauer, R. K., Fiset, P., Plourde, G., Backman, S. B., and Bushnell, M. C. (2004). Dose-dependent effects of propofol on the central processing of thermal pain. Anesthesiology 100, 386–394.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hofer, A., Siedentopf, C. M., Ischebeck, A., Rettenbacher, M. A., Verius, M., Felber, S., et al. (2006). Gender differences in regional cerebral activity during the perception of emotion: a functional MRI study. Neuroimage 32, 854–862.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hutcherson, C. A., Goldin, P. R., Ochsner, K. N., Gabrieli, J. D., Barrett, L. F., and Gross, J. J. (2005). Attention and emotion: does rating emotion alter neural responses to amusing and sad films? Neuroimage 27, 656–668.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Immordino-Yang, M. H., McColl, A., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A. (2009). Neural correlates of admiration and compassion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 8021–8026.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jackson, P. L., Brunet, E., Meltzoff, A. N., and Decety, J. (2006). Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain. Neuropsychologia 44, 752–761.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Klucken, T., Kagerer, S., Schweckendiek, J., Tabbert, K., Vaitl, D., and Stark, R. (2009). Neural, electrodermal and behavioral response patterns in contingency aware and unaware subjects during a picture-picture conditioning paradigm. Neuroscience 158, 721–731.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kong, J., White, N. S., Kwong, K. K., Vangel, M. G., Rosman, I. S., Gracely, R. H., et al. (2006). Using fMRI to dissociate sensory encoding from cognitive evaluation of heat pain intensity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 27, 715–721.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kotz, S. A., Meyer, M., Alter, K., Besson, M., von Cramon, D. Y., and Friederici, A. D. (2003). On the lateralization of emotional prosody: an event-related functional MR investigation. Brain Lang. 86, 366–376.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kurata, J., Thulborn, K. R., Gyulai, F. E., and Firestone, L. L. (2002). Early decay of pain-related cerebral activation in functional magnetic resonance imaging: comparison with visual and motor tasks. Anesthesiology 96, 35–44.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., and Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 42–58.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lamm, C., Meltzoff, A. N., and Decety, J. (2010). How do we empathize with someone who is not like us? A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 362–376.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Ahern, G. L., Schwartz, G. E., and Davidson, R. J. (1997). Neuroanatomical correlates of happiness, sadness, and disgust. Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 926–933.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Laureys, S., Faymonville, M. E., Peigneux, P., Damas, P., Lambermont, B., Del Fiore, G., et al. (2002). Cortical processing of noxious somatosensory stimuli in the persistent vegetative state. Neuroimage 17, 732–741.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Levesque, J., Eugene, F., Joanette, Y., Paquette, V., Mensour, B., Beaudoin, G., et al. (2003). Neural circuitry underlying voluntary suppression of sadness. Biol. Psychiatry 53, 502–510.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Liotti, M., Mayberg, H. S., Brannan, S. K., McGinnis, S., Jerabek, P., and Fox, P. T. (2000). Differential limbic—cortical correlates of sadness and anxiety in healthy subjects: implications for affective disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 48, 30–42.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Lorenz, I. H., Egger, K., Schubert, H., Schnurer, C., Tiefenthaler, W., Hohlrieder, M., et al. (2008). Lornoxicam characteristically modulates cerebral pain-processing in human volunteers: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Br. J. Anaesth. 100, 827–833.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lorenz, J., Cross, D. J., Minoshima, S., Morrow, T. J., Paulson, P. E., and Casey, K. L. (2002). A unique representation of heat allodynia in the human brain. Neuron 35, 383–393.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lui, F., Duzzi, D., Corradini, M., Serafini, M., Baraldi, P., and Porro, C. A. (2008). Touch or pain? Spatio-temporal patterns of cortical fMRI activity following brief mechanical stimuli. Pain 138, 362–374.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maihofner, C., and Handwerker, H. O. (2005). Differential coding of hyperalgesia in the human brain: a functional MRI study. Neuroimage 28, 996–1006.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maihofner, C., Herzner, B., and Otto Handwerker, H. (2006). Secondary somatosensory cortex is important for the sensory-discriminative dimension of pain: a functional MRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 1377–1383.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maihofner, C., Ringler, R., Herrndobler, F., and Koppert, W. (2007). Brain imaging of analgesic and antihyperalgesic effects of cyclooxygenase inhibition in an experimental human pain model: a functional MRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 26, 1344–1356.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marci, C. D., Glick, D. M., Loh, R., and Dougherty, D. D. (2007). Autonomic and prefrontal cortex responses to autobiographical recall of emotions. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 243–250.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mathews, A., Yiend, J., and Lawrence, A. D. (2004). Individual differences in the modulation of fear-related brain activation by attentional control. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1683–1694.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mobascher, A., Brinkmeyer, J., Warbrick, T., Musso, F., Wittsack, H. J., Saleh, A., et al. (2009a). Laser-evoked potential P2 single-trial amplitudes covary with the fMRI BOLD response in the medial pain system and interconnected subcortical structures. Neuroimage 45, 917–926.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mobascher, A., Brinkmeyer, J., Warbrick, T., Musso, F., Wittsack, H. J., Stoermer, R., et al. (2009b). Fluctuations in electrodermal activity reveal variations in single trial brain responses to painful laser stimuli–a fMRI/EEG study. Neuroimage 44, 1081–1092.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mochizuki, H., Sadato, N., Saito, D. N., Toyoda, H., Tashiro, M., Okamura, N., et al. (2007). Neural correlates of perceptual difference between itching and pain: a human fMRI study. Neuroimage 36, 706–717.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Eslinger, P. J., Bramati, I. E., Mourao-Miranda, J., Andreiuolo, P. A., et al. (2002). The neural correlates of moral sensitivity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of basic and moral emotions. J. Neurosci. 22, 2730–2736.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, F. T., Ignacio, F. A., Bramati, I. E., Caparelli-Daquer, E. M., et al. (2005). The moral affiliations of disgust: a functional MRI study. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 18, 68–78.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Moriguchi, Y., Decety, J., Ohnishi, T., Maeda, M., Mori, T., Nemoto, K., et al. (2007). Empathy and judging other's pain: an fMRI study of alexithymia. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2223–2234.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nemoto, H., Toda, H., Nakajima, T., Hosokawa, S., Okada, Y., Yamamoto, K., et al. (2003). Fluvoxamine modulates pain sensation and affective processing of pain in human brain. Neuroreport 14, 791–797.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Noriuchi, M., Kikuchi, Y., and Senoo, A. (2008). The functional neuroanatomy of maternal love: mother's response to infant's attachment behaviors. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 415–423.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ochsner, K. N., Ludlow, D. H., Knierim, K., Hanelin, J., Ramachandran, T., Glover, G. C., et al. (2006). Neural correlates of individual differences in pain-related fear and anxiety. Pain 120, 69–77.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ogino, Y., Nemoto, H., Inui, K., Saito, S., Kakigi, R., and Goto, F. (2007). Inner experience of pain: imagination of pain while viewing images showing painful events forms subjective pain representation in human brain. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1139–1146.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paulson, P. E., Minoshima, S., Morrow, T. J., and Casey, K. L. (1998). Gender differences in pain perception and patterns of cerebral activation during noxious heat stimulation in humans. Pain 76, 223–229.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Peyron, R., Kupers, R., Jehl, J. L., Garcia-Larrea, L., Convers, P., Barral, F. G., et al. (2007). Central representation of the RIII flexion reflex associated with overt motor reaction: an fMRI study. Neurophysiol. Clin. 37, 249–259.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ploghaus, A., Narain, C., Beckmann, C. F., Clare, S., Bantick, S., Wise, R., et al. (2001). Exacerbation of pain by anxiety is associated with activity in a hippocampal network. J. Neurosci. 21, 9896–9903.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Ploner, M., Lee, M. C., Wiech, K., Bingel, U., and Tracey, I. (2010). Prestimulus functional connectivity determines pain perception in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 355–360.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Reiman, E. M., Lane, R. D., Ahern, G. L., Schwartz, G. E., Davidson, R. J., Friston, K. J., et al. (1997). Neuroanatomical correlates of externally and internally generated human emotion. Am. J. Psychiatry 154, 918–925.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rolls, E. T., O'Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Francis, S., Bowtell, R., and McGlone, F. (2003). Representations of pleasant and painful touch in the human orbitofrontal and cingulate cortices. Cereb. Cortex 13, 308–317.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schafer, A., Schienle, A., and Vaitl, D. (2005). Stimulus type and design influence hemodynamic responses towards visual disgust and fear elicitors. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 57, 53–59.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schienle, A., Stark, R., Walter, B., Blecker, C., Ott, U., Kirsch, P., et al. (2002). The insula is not specifically involved in disgust processing: an fMRI study. Neuroreport 13, 2023–2026.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Schmahl, C., Bohus, M., Esposito, F., Treede, R. D., Di Salle, F., Greffrath, W., et al. (2006). Neural correlates of antinociception in borderline personality disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63, 659–667.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Seifert, F., and Maihofner, C. (2007). Representation of cold allodynia in the human brain–a functional MRI study. Neuroimage 35, 1168–1180.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shapira, N. A., Liu, Y., He, A. G., Bradley, M. M., Lessig, M. C., James, G. A., et al. (2003). Brain activation by disgust-inducing pictures in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 751–756.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature 439, 466–469.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Singer, T., Snozzi, R., Bird, G., Petrovic, P., Silani, G., Heinrichs, M., et al. (2008). Effects of oxytocin and prosocial behavior on brain responses to direct and vicariously experienced pain. Emotion 8, 781–791.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stammler, T., De Col, R., Seifert, F., and Maihofner, C. (2008). Functional imaging of sensory decline and gain induced by differential noxious stimulation. Neuroimage 42, 1151–1163.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stark, R., Zimmermann, M., Kagerer, S., Schienle, A., Walter, B., Weygandt, M., et al. (2007). Hemodynamic brain correlates of disgust and fear ratings. Neuroimage 37, 663–673.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Straube, T., Preissler, S., Lipka, J., Hewig, J., Mentzel, H. J., and Miltner, W. H. (2010). Neural representation of anxiety and personality during exposure to anxiety-provoking and neutral scenes from scary movies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 36–47.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Straube, T., Schmidt, S., Weiss, T., Mentzel, H. J., and Miltner, W. H. (2009). Sex differences in brain activation to anticipated and experienced pain in the medial prefrontal cortex. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 689–698.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Svensson, P., Johannsen, P., Jensen, T. S., Arendt-Nielsen, L., Nielsen, J., Stodkilde-Jorgensen, H., et al. (1998). Cerebral blood-flow changes evoked by two levels of painful heat stimulation: a positron emission tomography study in humans. Eur. J. Pain 2, 95–107.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Symonds, L. L., Gordon, N. S., Bixby, J. C., and Mande, M. M. (2006). Right-lateralized pain processing in the human cortex: an FMRI study. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 3823–3830.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Takahashi, H., Matsuura, M., Yahata, N., Koeda, M., Suhara, T., and Okubo, Y. (2006). Men and women show distinct brain activations during imagery of sexual and emotional infidelity. Neuroimage 32, 1299–1307.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Taylor, S. F., Liberzon, I., and Koeppe, R. A. (2000). The effect of graded aversive stimuli on limbic and visual activation. Neuropsychologia 38, 1415–1425.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Taylor, S. F., Phan, K. L., Decker, L. R., and Liberzon, I. (2003). Subjective rating of emotionally salient stimuli modulates neural activity. Neuroimage 18, 650–659.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Teasdale, J. D., Howard, R. J., Cox, S. G., Ha, Y., Brammer, M. J., Williams, S. C., et al. (1999). Functional MRI study of the cognitive generation of affect. Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 209–215.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Tolle, T. R., Kaufmann, T., Siessmeier, T., Lautenbacher, S., Berthele, A., Munz, F., et al. (1999). Region-specific encoding of sensory and affective components of pain in the human brain: a positron emission tomography correlation analysis. Ann. Neurol. 45, 40–47.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Tseng, M. T., Tseng, W. Y., Chao, C. C., Lin, H. E., and Hsieh, S. T. (2010). Distinct and shared cerebral activations in processing innocuous versus noxious contact heat revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 743–757.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Valet, M., Sprenger, T., Boecker, H., Willoch, F., Rummeny, E., Conrad, B., et al. (2004). Distraction modulates connectivity of the cingulo-frontal cortex and the midbrain during pain—an fMRI analysis. Pain 109, 399–408.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Dillen, L. F., Heslenfeld, D. J., and Koole, S. L. (2009). Tuning down the emotional brain: an fMRI study of the effects of cognitive load on the processing of affective images. Neuroimage 45, 1212–1219.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Boly, M., Balteau, E., Schnakers, C., Moonen, G., Luxen, A., et al. (2009). Pain and non-pain processing during hypnosis: a thulium-YAG event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 47, 1047–1054.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vogt, B. A., Derbyshire, S., and Jones, A. K. (1996). Pain processing in four regions of human cingulate cortex localized with co-registered PET and MR imaging. Eur. J. Neurosci. 8, 1461–1473.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

von Leupoldt, A., Sommer, T., Kegat, S., Baumann, H. J., Klose, H., Dahme, B., et al. (2009). Dyspnea and pain share emotion-related brain network. Neuroimage 48, 200–206.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wagner, K. J., Sprenger, T., Kochs, E. F., Tolle, T. R., Valet, M., and Willoch, F. (2007). Imaging human cerebral pain modulation by dose-dependent opioid analgesia: a positron emission tomography activation study using remifentanil. Anesthesiology 106, 548–556.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Watson, A., El-Deredy, W., Iannetti, G. D., Lloyd, D., Tracey, I., Vogt, B. A., et al. (2009). Placebo conditioning and placebo analgesia modulate a common brain network during pain anticipation and perception. Pain 145, 24–30.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Waugh, C. E., Wager, T. D., Fredrickson, B. L., Noll, D. C., and Taylor, S. F. (2008). The neural correlates of trait resilience when anticipating and recovering from threat. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 3, 322–332.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wright, P., He, G., Shapira, N. A., Goodman, W. K., and Liu, Y. (2004). Disgust and the insula: fMRI responses to pictures of mutilation and contamination. Neuroreport 15, 2347–2351.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Xu, X., Fukuyama, H., Yazawa, S., Mima, T., Hanakawa, T., Magata, Y., et al. (1997). Functional localization of pain perception in the human brain studied by PET. Neuroreport 8, 555–559.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Zald, D. H., and Pardo, J. V. (2002). The neural correlates of aversive auditory stimulation. Neuroimage 16, 746–753.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zeki, S., and Romaya, J. P. (2008). Neural correlates of hate. PLoS ONE 3:e3556. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003556

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ziv, M., Tomer, R., Defrin, R., and Hendler, T. (2010). Individual sensitivity to pain expectancy is related to differential activation of the hippocampus and amygdala. Hum. Brain Mapp. 31, 326–338.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: social neuroscience, individual differences, emotion, pain, sensorimotor integration, auditory perception, language, voxel-based morphometry

Citation: Mutschler I, Reinbold C, Wankerl J, Seifritz E and Ball T (2013) Structural basis of empathy and the domain general region in the anterior insular cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:177. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00177

Received: 07 February 2013; Accepted: 20 April 2013;
Published online: 09 May 2013.

Edited by:

John J. Foxe, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA

Reviewed by:

Michael Banissy, Goldsmiths University of London, UK
Graeme Fairchild, University of Southampton, UK

Copyright © 2013 Mutschler, Reinbold, Wankerl, Seifritz and Ball. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Isabella Mutschler, Department of Psychology, Division of Clinical Psychology and Epidemiology, University of Basel, Missionsstrasse 62a, 4055 Basel, Switzerland. e-mail: isabella.mutschler@unibas.ch

Download