Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 28 February 2014
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00101

Humans and great apes share increased neocortical neuropeptide Y innervation compared to other haplorhine primates

Mary Ann Raghanti1* Melissa K. Edler1 Richard S. Meindl1 Jessica Sudduth1 Tatiana Bohush1 Joseph M. Erwin2 Cheryl D. Stimpson2 Patrick R. Hof3,4 Chet C. Sherwood2
  • 1Department of Anthropology, School of Biomedical Sciences, Kent State University, Kent, OH, USA
  • 2Department of Anthropology, The George Washington University, Washington, DC, USA
  • 3Fishberg Department of Neuroscience and Friedman Brain Institute, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA
  • 4New York Consortium in Evolutionary Primatology, New York, NY, USA

Neuropeptide Y (NPY) plays a role in a variety of basic physiological functions and has also been implicated in regulating cognition, including learning and memory. A decrease in neocortical NPY has been reported for Alzheimer's disease, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and depression, potentially contributing to associated cognitive deficits. The goal of the present analysis was to examine variation in neocortical NPY-immunoreactive axon and varicosity density among haplorhine primates (monkeys, apes, and humans). Stereologic methods were used to measure the ratios of NPY-expressing axon length density to total neuron density (ALv/Nv) and NPY-immunoreactive varicosity density to neuron density (Vv/Nv), as well as the mean varicosity spacing in neocortical areas 10, 24, 44, and 22 (Tpt) of humans, African great apes, New World monkeys, and Old World monkeys. Humans and great apes showed increased cortical NPY innervation relative to monkey species for ALv/Nv and Vv/Nv. Furthermore, humans and great apes displayed a conserved pattern of varicosity spacing across cortical areas and layers, with no differences between cortical layers or among cortical areas. These phylogenetic differences may be related to shared life history variables and may reflect specific cognitive abilities.

Introduction

Neuropeptide Y (NPY), a 36-amino acid peptide, has an evolutionarily conserved sequence (Blomqvist et al., 1992), and is expressed at high levels throughout the central nervous system (Tatemoto et al., 1981; Adrian et al., 1983). The NPY that is expressed in the cerebral cortex derives from both intrinsic and extrinsic sources. NPY is often colocalized with GABA in cortical interneurons, and NPY-containing neurons of the olfactory bulb, nucleus of the tractus solitarius, and locus coeruleus send projections to innervate neurons of the cerebral cortex, spinal cord, and hypothalamus (Adrian et al., 1983; Allen et al., 1983; von Bohlen und Halbach and Dermietzel, 2006). The effects of NPY are mediated by at least four recognized receptor subtypes (Michel, 1991; Dumont et al., 1998). Its roles within the cerebral cortex include the regulation of blood flow and synaptic activity, and inhibition of neuronal excitability (Colmers and Bleakman, 1994; Cauli et al., 1997; Estrada and Defelipe, 1998; Bacci et al., 2002; Hamel et al., 2002). NPY is involved in regulating a variety of physiological functions, such as feeding behaviors, sleep regulation, and cardiovascular neuroendocrine functions, but has also been implicated in cognitive functions, including learning and memory (Rangani et al., 2012). In addition, decreased NPY concentrations are associated with conditions such as schizophrenia, depression, bipolar disorder, and Alzheimer's disease (Beal et al., 1986b; Kowall and Beal, 1988; Caberlotto and Hurd, 1999; Kuromitsu et al., 2001; Frederickson et al., 2007; Morales-Medina et al., 2010). Accordingly, evolutionary changes in the synthesis and innervation pattern of NPY may be present among primates related to cognitive and behavioral variability.

Neuroanatomical evidence indicates that the distribution and density of axons selectively expressing several neocortical neurotransmitters exhibit differences among primate species (Raghanti et al., 2008a,b,c). Humans and chimpanzees share increased neocortical innervation by dopaminergic and serotonergic axons and a different laminar pattern of cholinergic afferents selectively within prefrontal cortex (areas 9 and 32) relative to macaques, with no species differences evident in the primary motor cortex (area 4) (Raghanti et al., 2008a,b,c). In primates and other mammals, neocortical neurotransmitter innervation has been demonstrated to have species- and area-specific patterns (Morrison et al., 1982; Morrison and Foote, 1986; Lewis et al., 1987; Berger et al., 1991, 1992), raising the possibility that these systems may provide targets for the evolutionary modification of neuronal processing capacity. To understand the role of neurotransmitters in the evolution of the human brain, each system must be analyzed in a comparative context, so ultimately, broad quantitative comparative analyses of each cortical neurotransmitter system can be integrated and evaluated to reveal species-typical neurochemical phenotypes.

In the present study, we quantified the density of NPY-immunoreactive (ir) axons and varicosities in neocortical areas 10, 24, 44, and 22 among humans, African great apes, Old World monkeys, and New World monkeys. Cortical areas were chosen based on their roles in cognitive functions, including regions involved in language. The goal of the study was to determine if there was an evolutionary shift in cortical NPY innervation that might be associated with the emergence of cognitive or behavioral differences among haplorhine primates.

Materials and Methods

Specimens

Brain samples from the left hemisphere of 46 individuals representing eight haplorhines species were used for this study and included New World monkeys (black-capped squirrel monkeys and tufted capuchins), Old World monkeys (moor macaques, pigtailed macaques, and olive baboons), African great apes (western lowland gorillas and common chimpanzees), and humans (see Table 1 for details). Brain weight was recorded for every specimen either at the time of necropsy or shortly afterwards. All individuals were adults and had no evidence of neuropathological alterations based on inspection of gross specimens. Sexes were balanced within species as much as possible given the opportunistic nature of brain collection. Human brain samples were provided by the Northwestern University Alzheimer's Disease Center Brain Bank. The human subjects exhibited no evidence of cognitive changes before death and all received a score of zero for the CERAD senile plaque grade (Mirra et al., 1991) and the Braak and Braak neurofibrillary tangle stage (Braak and Braak, 1991). Non-human primate brains were acquired from American Zoo and Aquarium-accredited zoos or research institutions and were maintained in accordance with each institution's animal care and use guidelines. All brains except those of the moor macaques were immersion-fixed in 10% buffered formalin for at least 7 days, transferred to a 0.1 M phosphate buffered saline solution containing 0.1% sodium azide, and stored at 4°C until processing. The moor macaques were perfused transcardially with 4% paraformaldehyde as part of unrelated experiments (Duan et al., 2003). Postmortem interval (PMI) did not exceed 17 h for any of the specimens. While all four cortical areas were available for the majority of specimens, some individuals were not represented for every area. All available materials were analyzed for neuron densities and axon length densities. A minimum of four individuals per species (except three for squirrel monkeys) was used to quantify varicosity densities.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Study sample.

Sample Processing

Prior to sectioning, specimens were cryoprotected in a series of sucrose solutions (10, 20, and 30%) until saturated. Brains were frozen in dry ice and cut in the coronal plane into 40 μm-thick sections using a Leica SM2000R freezing sliding microtome. Sections were placed into individual microcentrifuge tubes containing a freezer storage solution (30% of each distilled water, ethylene glycol, and glycerol and 10% 0.244 M phosphate-buffered saline) and numbered sequentially. Sections were stored at −20°C until further processing. Every tenth section was stained for Nissl substance with a solution of 0.5% cresyl violet. Nissl-stained sections were used to identify the cortical areas of interest based on cytoarchitecture.

Identification of Cortical Areas

The identification of each cortical area was performed using descriptions from previous parcellations in a wide range of primate species, including galagos, New World monkeys, Old World monkeys, apes, and humans (Bailey et al., 1950; Rosabal, 1967; Preuss and Goldman-Rakic, 1991b; Watanabe-Sawaguchi et al., 1991; Vogt et al., 1995; Petrides and Pandya, 1999; Paxinos et al., 2000; Sherwood et al., 2003, 2004; Petrides, 2005; Schenker et al., 2008; Spocter et al., 2010). Because the cytoarchitecture of the areas of interest have not been described for all species analyzed in the present study, reports from a diverse array of primates were used to identify the cortical areas using Nissl-stained sections.

Broca's area (area 44) and its homolog have been described in humans (Amunts et al., 1999; Petrides, 2005), chimpanzees (Sherwood et al., 2003), and other great apes (Schenker, 2007). There has been considerable controversy over whether monkeys possess a homolog of human area 44. Petrides et al. (2005) addressed this question using both electrophysiological recordings and quantitative cytoarchitecture in macaque monkeys, demonstrating the presence of a cortical area that is topologically and cytoarchitecturally comparable to that of humans and which is involved in orofacial muscle movement. Coudé et al. (2011) also recently reported that voluntary vocalizations, but not emotional vocalizations, evoked the firing of neurons within the macaque homolog of Broca's area. Area 44 is characterized in each species as being dysgranular with a poorly developed layer IV and large pyramidal cells in the lower portion of layers III and V.

Wernicke's area and its homolog (area 22 or Tpt) is well described for humans (Fullerton and Pandya, 2007), chimpanzees (Spocter et al., 2010), macaques (Preuss and Goldman-Rakic, 1991a; Lewis and Van Essen, 2000; Fullerton and Pandya, 2007; Gannon et al., 2008), and galagos (Preuss and Goldman-Rakic, 1991a). Area Tpt is located in the caudal portion of the superior temporal gyrus and is readily distinguished by its well-developed layer II, a columnar appearance of pyramidal neurons in layers III and V and a thick layer IV (Galaburda and Pandya, 1982; Preuss and Goldman-Rakic, 1991a; Fullerton and Pandya, 2007; Gannon et al., 2008).

Both areas 10 and 24 are present in all haplorhine primate species (Brodmann, 1909; Allman et al., 2001; Semendeferi et al., 2001; Burman et al., 2006; Passingham and Wise, 2012). The anterior cingulate cortex (area 24) is involved in visceral, emotional, and cognitive processes including control of heart rate, blood pressure, vocalizations, and facial expressions (Smith, 1945; Jürgens and Ploog, 1970; Jürgens, 1998). This area also regulates emotional self-control (Damasio et al., 2000), and is activated during cognitively demanding tasks as well as intense drive states in humans (Paus et al., 1993, 1998; Bartels and Zeki, 2000; Bush et al., 2000). The cytoarchitecture of area 24 has been described in humans (Vogt et al., 1995; Öngür and Price, 2000; Öngür et al., 2003), macaques (Hof and Nimchinsky, 1992; Carmichael and Price, 1994; Dombrowski et al., 2001; Vogt et al., 2005), and in a study that compared the cytoarchitecture of humans to that of macaques (Petrides and Pandya, 1994). Area 24 is located in the anterior cingulate gyrus (Carmichael and Price, 1994; Vogt et al., 1995, 2005; Öngür et al., 2003) and the region sampled for this study was anterior to the genu of the corpus callosum, which is agranular.

Area 10 is involved in verbal and non-verbal episodic memory, reward-based decision making, and planning future actions (Buckner, 1996; Okuda et al., 1998; Daffner et al., 2000; Lepage et al., 2000; Burgess et al., 2007). Lesions restricted to area 10 in humans impair the ability to extract meaning from events, and limit creative thinking, artistic expression, and disrupt the ability to plan future actions. Semendeferi et al. (2001) assessed the homology of area 10 among humans, apes, and macaque monkeys. The authors found that area 10 of the frontal pole in human, bonobo, chimpanzee, and orangutan was homologous with the orbital sector of the frontal pole in gibbons. The orbitolateral component of the frontal pole in macaques most closely resembled area 10 of the other species (Semendeferi et al., 2001). Area 10 also has been described in the hamadryas baboon (Watanabe-Sawaguchi et al., 1991), squirrel monkey (Rosabal, 1967), and marmoset (Burman et al., 2006). Area 10 is a granular cortex with a thin layer II, wide layer III, and a sharp border between layer VI and white matter (Semendeferi et al., 2001).

Immunohistochemistry

Every tenth section from each individual and cortical area was processed for NPY immunohistochemistry using the avidin-biotin-peroxidase method as described previously (Raghanti et al., 2013). Briefly, sections were pretreated for antigen retrieval by incubation in 0.05% citraconic acid (pH 7.4) at 85–90°C for 30 min. Endogenous peroxidase was quenched and sections were preblocked, pretreated, and incubated in a polyclonal primary antibody (Abcam, Cambridge, MA, catalog # ab30914) at a dilution of 1:5000 for 24 h at room temperature followed by 24 h at 4°C (Massoner et al., 2013; Raghanti et al., 2013). This antibody is derived from within residues 1 to the C-terminus of pig NPY. NPY has a high degree of sequence homology among vertebrates, with 22 of its 36 positions identical in all sequenced species (Larhammar et al., 1987; Larhammar, 1996), and 100% sequence homology in human, rabbit, rat, guinea pig, and alligator (O'Hare et al., 1988; Larhammar, 1996). After incubation in primary antibody, sections were incubated in a biotinylated secondary antibody (1:200) followed by the avidin-peroxidase complex (PK-6100, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA). A 3,3′-diaminobenzidine-peroxidase (DAB) substrate with nickel enhancement was used as the chromogen (SK-4100, Vector Laboratories).

Data Acquisition

Quantitative analyses were performed using computer-assisted stereology with a system including an Olympus BX-51 photomicroscope equipped with a digital camera and StereoInvestigator software version 10 (MBF Biosciences, Williston, VT). Sampling parameters for each species and variable were determined using subsampling techniques to obtain a coefficient of error below 0.08 (Slomianka and West, 2005). Sampling areas (i.e., layers III and V-VI of each area) were outlined at low magnification (4×, N.A. 0.13). Total axon length was measured utilizing the SpaceBalls probe under Koehler illumination at 60x using the following equation: L = 2 * (v/a) * (Σ is) * 1/asf * 1/ssf * 1/tsf, where v/a is the ratio of sampling frame volume to probe surface area, Σis is the sum of the number of intersections between fibers and sampling hemispheres, asf (area sampling fraction; the fraction of the total area sampled) is the area of the counting frame divided by the total area of the reference space, section sampling fraction (ssf) is the number of sections analyzed divided by the total number of sections through the reference space, and tissue sampling fraction (tsf) is the sampling box height divided by mean mounted section thickness (Calhoun et al., 2004). Mean mounted section thickness was measured at every fifth sampling site. To measure axon length density (ALv), total axon length was divided by the planimetric measurement of the reference volume sampled.

Axonal varicosities, or synaptic boutons, are enlarged areas of an axon that contain release sites for neurotransmitters (Sutoo et al., 2001; Shepherd and Raastad, 2003), and were quantified as a putative measure of neuron-specific innervation to differentiate between axons that are directly innervating cells of a specific area vs. those that are en passant. Varicosities were defined as having a transverse diameter greater than 0.5 μm (Mechawar et al., 2000). Varicosity density (Vv) was quantified using the optical disector probe with a 100× (N.A. 1.4) objective lens. Vv was calculated as the sum of varicosities counted divided by the product of the disector and the volume of the disector (e.g., Sherwood et al., 2007). In addition to varicosity density, mean spacing of varicosities was calculated for each cortical layer and area for each species using the calculated estimates of NPY-ir Vv and ALv. Varicosity spacing has received attention as a putative measure of synaptic density (Shepherd et al., 2002; Shepherd and Raastad, 2003). Here, we aimed to evaluate the variability of varicosity spacing (i.e., mean spacing of varicosities in μm) to determine if this system is globally conserved across cortical regions within a species and among species. Nissl-stained sections were used to obtain neuron density (Nv) with a 60× (N.A. 1.35) objective lens. Nv was calculated in the same manner as Vv.

Statistical Analyses

The variables used to compare cortical NPY innervation among species were ALv/Nv, Vv/Nv, and mean varicosity spacing. The ratio of NPY-ir axon and varicosity density to neuron density allowed for comparison among species with divergent brain sizes. Factorial analyses of variance (ANOVA) with repeated-measures design were used to analyze differences among species for NPY-ir ALv/Nv, Vv/Nv, and mean varicosity spacing. Cortical area and layer were within-subjects factors and species was the between-subjects factor. Repeated-measures ANOVAs also were used to evaluate the uniformity of varicosity spacing within each species with both layer and area as within-subjects factors. Fisher's LSD post hoc analyses were employed to evaluate significant findings. Nonparametric Spearman's correlation coefficient was used to assess whether PMI affected the intensity of immunohistochemical staining in humans. Exact PMIs were not known for other species. Additionally, Spearman's correlation was used to test the effect of age on cortical NPY innervation within each species and the relationship between ALv/Nv and Vv/Nv. Independent t-tests were used to examine differences between sexes. Standard linear regression was used to determine if differences in cortical ALv or Vv were associated with variation in brain mass or encephalization quotient. Brain masses were obtained for each specimen before processing. Because body mass was not available for all individuals, encephalization quotients were taken from Jerison (1973). Neither body mass nor encephalization quotients were available for moor macaques and were omitted from these analyses. Finally, an intraclass correlation coefficient was used to assess inter-observer reliability for quantitative stereological data. Periodically, inter-observer reliability was evaluated utilizing an audit feature within the StereoInvestigator software. The level of significance (α) was set at 0.05 for all statistical tests.

Results

Figures 13 provide examples of cortical NPY immunostaining for each species. The mean number of sampling sites for each layer/area/individual was 109.8 ± 25.4 for Nv (total sampling sites = 39,730; total neurons counted = 238,487), 86.4 ± 11.7 for ALv (total sampling sites = 33,869; total intersections counted = 234,583), and 68.5 ± 6.2 for Vv (total sampling sites = 19,433; total varicosities counted = 148,548). The mean coefficient of error was 0.04 ± 0.01 for Nv and 0.05 ± 0.01 for Vv (Schmitz and Hof, 2000). The intraclass correlation coefficient for inter-observer reliability was 0.86 (n = 24, p < 0.01).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Photomicrographs of NPY immunostaining in layer III of area 44 in (A) squirrel monkey, (B) capuchin, (C) moor macaque, (D) pigtailed macaque, (E) baboon, (F) gorilla, (G) chimpanzee, and (H) human. Scale bar = 50 μm.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Low-powered photomicrographs of NPY immunostaining in layer III (A–H) and layers V–VI (I–P) of area 10. (A,I) squirrel monkey, (B,J) capuchin, (C,K) moor macaque, (D,L) pigtailed macaque, (E,M) baboon, (F,N) gorilla, (G,O) chimpanzee, (H,P) human. Scale bars = 100 μm.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Darkfield photomicrographs of NPY immunostaining in area 24 of (A) capuchin, (B) baboon, (C) gorilla, and (D) chimpanzee. Roman numerals indicate cortical layers, wm, white matter. Scale bars = 250 μm.

Postmortem Interval

We examined the effect of PMI on our measures of cortical NPY in humans, where the most precise data on PMI were available. In humans, PMI was not correlated with NPY-ir ALv/Nv in layers III or V-VI in area 10 (Spearman's rho = −0.23, p = 0.66; −0.12, p = 0.83), area 24 (Spearman's rho = −0.21, p = 0.74; 0.15, p = 0.81), or area 22 (Spearman's rho = 0.56, p = 0.61; 0.31, p = 0.61). PMI was not correlated with NPY ALv/Nv in layer III of area 44 (Spearman's rho = 0.56, p = 0.32), but a significant correlation existed within layers V-VI (Spearman's rho = −0.81, p = 0.03). Given that 87.5% of possible correlations were not significant and PMI did not exceed 17 h for any individual, PMI is not considered a confounding variable for this study.

Sex

Independent samples t-tests were used to determine possible differences in NPY-ir ALv/Nv or Vv/Nv between males and females in each species except squirrel monkeys, as only females were available from this species. No sex differences were detected in capuchins, pigtailed macaques, baboons, chimpanzees, or gorillas. In humans, females exhibited a denser ALv/Nv relative to males only for layers V-VI of area 22 [t(3) = −3.51, p = 0.04]. There was also a sex difference for layer III of area 44 in moor macaques [t(4) = −3.32, p = 0.03], with females having a denser ALv/Nv. Given these results, the overall effect of sex on cortical NPY innervation was not substantial.

Age

To determine if there was a significant effect of age on NPY-ir ALv/Nv and Vv/Nv, each species was analyzed separately, for a total of 64 correlation analyses per measure. Age was not correlated with NPY ALv/Nv in any layer or area for non-human species. In humans, only layer III of area 22 showed an effect of age (Spearman's rho = −0.95, p = 0.01). For Vv/Nv, two significant correlations were found for pigtailed macaques (layer III of area 44, Spearman's rho = −0.90, p < 0.01; layers V-VI of area 22, p = −1.00, p < 0.01) and one for capuchins (layer III of area 10, Spearman's rho = 1.00, p < 0.01). Age within the present was not a significant variable as only 0.02% (ALv/Nv) and 0.05% (Vv/Nv) of the possible correlations were significant.

Brain Mass and Encephalization Quotient

NPY-ir ALv and Vv were each regressed on brain mass and encephalization quotient. There was a significant relationship between ALv and brain mass [F(1, 343) = 28.48, p < 0.01, R2 = 0.08], but not Vv and brain mass [F(1, 289) = 1.66, p = 0.20, R2 = 0.006). Neither measure was significant when regressed on encephalization quotient [ALv: F(1, 296) = 2.70, p = 0.10, R2 = 0.009; Vv: F(1, 250) = 0.05, p = 0.83, R2 = 0.00].

Among-Species Analyses

Across the entire primate sample, NPY-ir ALv/Nv was significantly correlated with Vv/Nv in layers III and V-VI in all cortical areas (Spearman's rho, all p's < 0.05, Figure 4). In some individuals, not every cortical area was represented due to tissue damage during brain extraction or unavailability of tissue. Therefore, analyses were restricted to individuals in which all four cortical areas were represented (see Table 1). Initial repeated-measures ANOVAs revealed no differences among species within the phylogenetic groups of New World monkeys, Old World monkeys, and hominids (great apes and humans) for ALv/Nv (Figure 5), Vv/Nv (Figure 6), and varicosity spacing (Figure 7). Thus, species were collapsed into these categories for further analysis.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Correlations between NPY-ir ALv/Nv and Vv/Nv for (A) area 10, (B) area 24, (C) area 44, and (D) area 22. The outlier in (B) and (D) is one chimpanzee male.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. NPY-ir ALv/Nv for each species for (A) area 10, (B) area 24, (C) area 44, and (D) area 22. Means are shown by bars. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. NPY-ir Vv/Nv for each species in (A) area 10, (B) area 24, (C) area 44, and (D) area 22. Means are shown by bars. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Varicosity spacing in μm for each species in (A) area 10, (B) area 24, (C) area 44, and (D) area 22. Means are shown by bars. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

A repeated-measures ANOVA was used to analyze ALv/Nv among New World monkeys, Old World monkeys, and hominids (Figure 8). There were significant main effects of phylogenetic group [F(2, 34) = 10.34, p < 0.01], cortical area [F(3, 102) = 8.24, p < 0.01], and layer[F(1, 34) = 14.75, p < 0.01]. Additionally, the interaction between layer and phylogenetic group was significant [F(2, 34) = 6.32, p < 0.01]. The interaction terms of area by phylogenetic group, area by layer, and the three-way interaction of area by layer by phylogenetic group were not significant (all p's > 0.05). Fisher's LSD post-hoc analyses revealed that hominids had significantly higher ALv/Nv relative to both New World monkeys and Old World monkeys (all p's < 0.01). No differences were detected between the monkey groups (p = 0.50).

The repeated measures ANOVA for Vv/Nv (Figure 9) revealed significant main effects of phylogenetic group [F(2, 28) = 24.73, p < 0.01] and area [F(3, 84) = 4.94, p < 0.01]. The main effect of layer [F(1, 28) = 0.87, p = 0.36] and interaction terms were not significant (all p's > 0.05). Fisher's LSD post-hoc analyses demonstrated that hominids had significantly higher Vv/Nv relative to both New World monkeys and Old World monkeys (all p's < 0.05). No differences were detected between the monkey groups (p = 0.56).

For varicosity spacing (Figure 10), there were significant main effects of phylogenetic group [F(2, 28) = 17.35, p < 0.01], area [F(3, 84) = 18.19, p < 0.01] and layer [F(1, 28) = 76.19, p < 0.01]. The interaction of area by phylogenetic group [F(6, 27) = 3.07, p < 0.05], layer by phylogenetic group [F(2, 27) = 10.53, p < 0.01], and layer by area [F(3, 27) = 10.47, p < 0.01] were significant. The three-way interaction of phylogenetic group by area by layer was not statistically significant [F(6, 84) = 1.97, p = 0.08]. Post-hoc analyses showed that hominids had smaller inter-varicosity spacing relative to both New and Old World monkeys in area 22 (p = 0.03 and 0.001, respectively). Hominids also had a lower varicosity spacing relative to Old World monkeys in area 44 (p = 0.01). Furthermore, hominids displayed a lower mean varicosity spacing in layer III relative to the Old World monkeys (p = 0.02). No other significant differences were detected.

Within-Species Analysis of Varicosity Spacing

Varicosity spacing was analyzed within each species to determine variability among cortical areas and layers using repeated-measures ANOVAs. A summary of main effects and interaction are listed in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of main effects (area and layer) and interaction term for the within-species repeated-measures ANOVA for varicosity spacing.

For squirrel monkey, post-hoc tests showed that area 22 had higher varicosity spacing relative to the other cortical areas, with area 10 having the lowest varicosity spacing (all p's < 0.05). No significant differences between layers were detected (all p's > 0.05). Capuchin varicosity spacing was higher in area 22 relative to the other areas and was lower in layers V-VI relative to layer III within areas 44 and 22 (all p's > 0.05).

Post-hoc analyses for moor macaques showed that areas 44 and 22 had higher varicosity spacing relative to areas 10 and 24, and layers V/VI displayed lower varicosity spacing than layer III in areas 24 and 44 (all p's < 0.05). In pigtailed macaques, there was increased spacing of varicosities in area 22 relative to area 10 (p = 0.04). Distance between varicosities was lower for layer V-VI relative to layer III (p = 0.02). For baboon, area 22 had higher varicosity spacing relative to areas 10 and 24 (p = 0.02 for each). Post-hoc analyses did not detect a significant difference between layers III and V/VI.

Post-hoc analyses for gorillas did not detect a significant difference between layers (p = 0.13). No significant differences were detected in either the chimpanzee or human.

Discussion

The present report provides a broad quantitative comparative analysis of cortical NPY-ir axon and varicosity densities among haplorhine primate species. NPY is involved in a wide variety of physiological functions as well as cognitive processes, such as learning and memory, and deficits in NPY are associated with neuropathological conditions that exhibit decreases in intellectual abilities (Beal et al., 1986a; Kowall and Beal, 1988; Caberlotto and Hurd, 1999; Kuromitsu et al., 2001; Frederickson et al., 2007; Rangani et al., 2012). Our results demonstrated that humans and African great apes (i.e., hominids) shared a significant increase in cortical NPY-ir axons and varicosities relative to Old World monkeys and New World monkeys, as measured by NPY-ir ALv/Nv and Vv/Nv. Vv/Nv putatively represents a fine-grained measure of innervation as neurotransmitters are released from varicosities. Interestingly, the differences between hominids and monkeys are more apparent in this measure relative to ALv/Nv (see Figures 5, 6, 8, and 9). Although the overall effect of phylogenetic group (i.e., hominid vs. Old World and New World monkeys) was significant, area 24 did not appear dramatically different among the species analyzed here for either ALv/Nv or Vv/Nv. It is notable, however, that we did not find an increase in cortical NPY input for humans relative to the African great apes, chimpanzees and gorillas.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. NPY-ir ALv/Nv collapsed into phylogenetic groups for (A) area 10, (B) area 24, (C) area 44, and (D) area 22. OWM, Old World monkeys; NWM, New World monkeys. Means are shown by bars. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. NPY-ir Vv/Nv collapsed into phylogenetic groups for (A) area 10, (B) area 24, (C) area 44, and (D) area 22. OWM, Old World monkeys; NWM, New World monkeys. Means are shown by bars. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

FIGURE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 10. NPY-ir varicosity spacing collapsed into phylogenetic groups for (A) area 10, (B) area 24, (C) area 44, and (D) area 22. OWM, Old World monkeys; NWM, New World monkeys. Means are shown by bars. Error bars indicate standard deviation.

Increased NPY afferents to cortical regions, such as areas 10, 22, and 44, in humans and great apes could contribute to cortical processing by mediating synaptic inhibition, glutamatergic output, and an increase in cerebral blood flow. Interestingly, glutamatergic gene and protein expression are altered in the frontal pole of hominids relative to other primate species (Muntané et al., 2014), and these modifications may have co-evolved with increased cortical NPY. The most dramatic and consistent phylogenetic differences observed within the current data set were within the language areas (44 and 22), where humans and great apes exhibited higher NPY-ir Vv/Nv (see Figure 6); however, humans clearly deviate from apes in language proficiency. Ape language training experiments have required extensive effort to teach subjects to acquire a limited vocabulary and rudimentary syntax as compared to human children (Nowak and Komarova, 2001; Hauser et al., 2002). In addition to a shared capacity to learn certain aspects of symbolic language, humans and great apes also exhibit increased capacity for behavioral inhibition (Beran and Evans, 2006; Evans and Beran, 2007a,b), cultural transmission of knowledge (Boesch, 1993), episodic memory (Martin-Ordas et al., 2013), and an understanding of the attentional states of others (Tempelmann et al., 2011). Likewise, great apes consistently outperform other non-human primates in several cognitive tests, including mirror self-recognition, tool use, deception, and complex manipulation (Gallup, 1970; Tomasello et al., 2003; Deaner et al., 2006).

NPY may protect cortical neurons from glutamate excitotoxicity by inhibiting glutamate release (Decressac and Barker, 2012). NPY also protects cortical neurons against toxicity mediated by the amyloid β peptide, potentially by increasing the synthesis and release of nerve growth factor (Croce et al., 2012). In addition, NPY plays a significant role in neurogenesis (Decressac and Barker, 2012). It could be argued that these functions of NPY are critical to meet the demands of enlarged brains. However, neocortical NPY innervation density in our samples was not associated with encephalization quotient, and only 0.06–7.7% of the variance in measures of NPY innervation could be explained by brain size. Whereas brain mass varied by six-fold among the monkeys (squirrel monkey to baboon), there were no significant differences among these species in NPY innervation. Similarly, human brains are approximately three times larger than chimpanzees and gorillas, yet NPY density did not vary significantly among hominid species. This suggests that that the increase observed in hominids is not simply associated with encephalization, but rather represents a significant change that occurred in the development of NPY cortical innervation in the stem ancestral ape lineage.

The present analysis of NPY-ir varicosity spacing revealed interesting differences within each species. Notably, there was a conservation of varicosity spacing for humans, chimpanzees, and gorillas across cortical areas and between layers III and V/VI. For both New and Old World monkeys, there was higher varicosity spacing in area 22. There was also decreased varicosity spacing in layers V/VI relative to layer III for both macaque species and the capuchin monkey.

In an earlier study, we reported significant differences among haplorhine primate species in the density of cortical NPY-ir neurons in area 22 that did not clearly conform to a phylogenetic branching pattern (Raghanti et al., 2013). Interestingly, NPY-ir cortical neurons are one of the first affected neuron populations in Alzheimer's disease, suggesting a unique vulnerability among interneurons that are known to be rather resistant to the disease (Beal et al., 1986b; Kowall and Beal, 1988; Hof and Morrison, 1991; Hof et al., 1991, 1993). In our prior report, we found squirrel monkeys possessed the highest percentage of NPY-ir cortical neurons; however, that increase in NPY-ir cortical neurons was not associated with an increased NPY-ir ALv/Nv or Vv/Nv, as illustrated by the present data set. Overall, comparison of the density of NPY-ir cortical neurons with the results presented here indicates that the density of NPY-ir axons or varicosities is not correlated with the density of NPY-ir neurons within the cortex.

Our results indicate that humans and great apes share a significant increase in cortical NPY ALv/Nv and Vv/Nv relative to monkey species with a conserved pattern of NPY-ir varicosity spacing. The increase in cortical NPY innervation may contribute to the differences in behavioral and cognitive flexibility observed in hominids relative to other primates. Alternatively, this phylogenetic difference may be due to the increased lifespans observed within these lineages, since NPY peptide mediates neurodegenerative damage caused by glutamate turnover and amyloid β toxicity. Further research is necessary to determine the role of NPY within cortical language circuits and other executive functions and whether this difference in NPY innervation between hominids and monkeys extends to cortical regions that are not directly associated with cognition.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Science Foundation (NSF BCS-0921079 to Mary Ann Raghanti, BCS-0827531 to Chet C. Sherwood) and the James S. McDonnell Foundation (22002078 to Patrick R. Hof and Chet C. Sherwood and 220020293 to Chet C. Sherwood). We would like to thank Genevieve Sharron and Bridget Wicinski for expert technical assistance. Brain materials for this study were obtained through the Great Ape Aging Project (NIH grant AG014308, “A Comparative Neurobiology of Aging Resource,” J. Erwin, PI), the National Primate Research Center at the University of Washington (NIH grant RR00166), the Northwestern University Alzheimer's Disease Center Brain Bank (supported by an Alzheimer's Disease Core Center grant, P30 AG013854, from the National Institute on Aging to Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois), the University of Texas MD Anderson Cancer Center, Michale E. Keeling Center for Comparative Medicine and Research (NIH P40 RR001254), the Southwest National Primate Research Center (NIH P51 RR013986), and the University of South Alabama.

References

Adrian, T. E., Allen, J. M., Bloom, J. M., Ghatei, M. A., Rosser, M. N., Roberts, G. W., et al. (1983). Neuropeptide Y distribution in the human brain. Nature 306, 584–586. doi: 10.1038/306584a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Allen, Y. S., Adrian, T. E., Allen, J. M., Tatemoto, K., Crow, T. J., Bloom, S. R., et al. (1983). Neuropeptide Y distribution in the rat brain. Science 221, 877–879. doi: 10.1126/science.6136091

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Allman, J. M., Hakeem, A., Erwin, J. M., Nimchinsky, E., and Hof, P. (2001). The anterior cingulate cortex. The evolution of an interface between emotion and cognition. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 935, 107–117. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb03476.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amunts, K., Schleicher, A., Burgel, U., Mohlberg, H., Uylings, H. B., and Zilles, K. (1999). Broca's region revisited: cytoarchitecture and intersubject variability. J. Comp. Neurol. 412, 319–341.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bacci, A., Huguenard, J. R., and Prince, D. A. (2002). Differential modulation of synaptic transmission by neuropeptide Y in rat neocortical neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 17125–17130. doi: 10.1073/pnas.012481899

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bailey, P., Von Bonin, G., and McCulloch, W. S. (1950). The Isocortex of the Chimpanzee. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

Bartels, A., and Zeki, S. (2000). The neural basis of romantic love. Neuroreport 11, 3829–3834. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200011270-00046

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Beal, M. F., Frank, R. C., Ellison, J. B., and Martin, J. B. (1986a). The effects of neuropeptide Y on striatal catecholamines. Neurosci. Lett. 71, 118–123. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(86)90267-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Beal, M. F., Mazurek, M. F., Chattha, G. K., Svendsen, C. N., Bird, E. D., and Martin, J. B. (1986b). Neuropeptide Y immunoreactivity is reduced in cerebral cortex of Alzheimer's disease. Ann. Neurol. 20, 282–288. doi: 10.1002/ana.410200303

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Beran, M. J., and Evans, T. A. (2006). Maintenance of delay of gratification by four chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the effects of delayed reward visibility, experimenter presence, and extended delay intervals. Behav. Processes 73, 315–324. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2006.07.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berger, B., Gaspar, P., and Verney, C. (1991). Dopaminergic innervation of the cerebral cortex: unexpected differences between rodents and primates Trends Neurosci. 14, 21–27. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(91)90179-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berger, B., Gaspar, P., and Verney, C. (1992). Colocalization of neurotensin in the mesocortical dopaminergic system. Restricted regional and laminar distribution in rat, lack of colocalization in human. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 668, 307–310. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb27359.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blomqvist, A. G., Soderberg, C., Lundell, I., Milner, R. J., and Larhammar, D. (1992). Strong evolutionary conservation of neuropeptide Y: sequences of chicken, goldfish, and Torpedo marmorata DNA clones. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 89, 2350–2354. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.6.2350

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boesch, C. (1993). “Aspects of transmission of tool-use in wild chimpanzees,” in Tools, Language and Cognition in Human Evolution, eds. K. R. Gibson and T. Ingold. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 171–184.

Braak, H., and Braak, E. (1991). Neuropathological staging of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 82, 239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brodmann, K. (1909). Vergleichende Lokalisationslehre der Großhirnrinde. Leipzig: Barth. (reprinted as Brodmann's Localisation in the Cerebral Cortex, translated by L. J. Garey, London: Smith-Gordon, 1994).

Buckner, R. (1996). Beyond HERA: contributions of specific prefrontal brain areas to long-term memory retrieval. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 3, 149–158. doi: 10.3758/BF03212413

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burgess, P. W., Gilbert, S. J., and Dumontheil, I. (2007). Function and localization within rostral prefrontal cortex (area 10). Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 362, 887–899. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2095

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burman, K. J., Palmer, S. M., Gamberini, M., and Rosa, M. G. P. (2006). Cytoarchitectonic subdivisions of the dorsolateral frontal cortex of the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus), and their projections to dorsal visual areas. J. Comp. Neurol. 495, 149–172. doi: 10.1002/cne.20837

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bush, G., Luu, P., and Posner, M. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 215–222. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01483-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Caberlotto, L., and Hurd, Y. L. (1999). Reduced neuropeptide Y mRNA expression in the prefrontal cortex of subjects with bipolar disorder. Neuroreport 10, 1747–1750. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199906030-00022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Calhoun, M. E., Mao, Y., Roberts, J. A., and Rapp, P. R. (2004). Reduction in hippocampal cholinergic innervation is unrelated to recognition memory impairment in aged rhesus monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 475, 238–246. doi: 10.1002/cne.20181

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carmichael, S. T., and Price, J. L. (1994). Architectonic subdivision of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 346, 366–402. doi: 10.1002/cne.903460305

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cauli, B., Audinat, E., Lambolez, B., Angulo, M. C., Ropert, N., Tsuzuki, K., et al. (1997). Molecular and physiological diversity of cortical nonpyramidal cells. J. Neurosci. 17, 3894–3906.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Colmers, W. F., and Bleakman, D. (1994). Effects of neuropeptide Y on the electrical properties of neurons. Trends Neurosci. 17, 373–379. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(94)90046-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Coudé, G., Ferrari, P. F., Rodà, F., Maranesi, M., Borelli, E., Veroni, V., et al. (2011). Neurons controlling voluntary vocalization in the macaque ventral premotor cortex. PLoS ONE 6:e26822. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0026822

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Croce, N., Ciotti, M. T., Gelfo, F., Cortelli, S., Federici, G., Caltagirone, C., et al. (2012). Neuropeptide Y protects rat cortical neurons against β-amyloid toxicity and re-establishes synthesis and release of nerve growth factor. ACS Chem. Neurosci. 3, 312–318. doi: 10.1021/cn200127e

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Daffner, K. R., Mesulam, M. M., Scinto, L. F., Acar, D., Calvo, V., Faust, R., et al. (2000). The central role of the prefrontal cortex in directing attention to novel events. Brain 123, 927–939. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.5.927

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L. L., Parvizi, J., et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1049–1056. doi: 10.1038/79871

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Deaner, R. O., Van Schaik, C. P., and Johnson, V. E. (2006). Do some taxa have better domain-general cognition than others? A meta-analysis of nonhuman primate studies. Evol. Psychol. 4, 149–196.

Decressac, M., and Barker, R. A. (2012). Neuropeptide Y and its role in CNS disease and repair. Exp. Neurol. 238, 265–272. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2012.09.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dombrowski, S. M., Hilgetag, C. C., and Barbas, H. (2001). Quantitative architecture distinguishes prefrontal cortical systems in the rhesus monkey. Cereb. Cortex 11, 975–988. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.10.975

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Duan, H., Wearne, S. L., Rocher, A. B., Macedo, A., Morrison, J. H., and Hof, P. R. (2003). Age-realted dendritic and spine changes in corticocortically projecting neurons in macaque monkeys. Cereb. Cortex 13, 950–961. doi: 10.1093/cercor/13.9.950

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dumont, Y., Jacques, D., Bouchard, P., and Quirion, R. (1998). Species differences in the expression and distribution of the neuropeptide Y, Y1, Y2, Y5, and Y5 receptors in rodents, guinea pig, and primate brains. J. Comp. Neurol. 402, 372–384. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19981221)402:3%3C372::AID-CNE6%3E3.3.CO;2-U

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Estrada, C., and Defelipe, J. (1998). Nitric oxide-producing neurons in the neocortex: morphological and functional relationship with intraparenchymal microvasculature. Cereb. Cortex 8, 193–203. doi: 10.1093/cercor/8.3.193

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Evans, T. A., and Beran, M. J. (2007a). Chimpanzees use self-distraction to cope with impulsivity. Biol. Lett. 3, 599–602. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0399

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Evans, T. A., and Beran, M. J. (2007b). Delay of gratification and delay maintenance by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). J. Gen. Psychol. 134, 199–216. doi: 10.3200/GENP.134.2.199-216

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Frederickson, S. O., Ekman, R., Gottfries, C.-G., Widerlov, E., and Jonsson, S. (2007). Reduced concentrations of galanin, arginine vasopressin, neuropeptide Y and peptide YY in the temporal cortex but not in the hypothalamus of brains from schizophrenics. Acta Psychiat. Scand. 83, 273–277. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1991.tb05539.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fullerton, B. C., and Pandya, D. N. (2007). Architectonic analysis of the auditory-related areas of the superior temporal region in human brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 504, 470–498. doi: 10.1002/cne.21432

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Galaburda, A. M., and Pandya, D. N. (1982). “Role of architectonics and connections in the study of primate brain evolution,” in Primate Brain Evolution: Methods and Concepts, eds E. Armstrong and D. Falk (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 203–216.

Gallup, G. G. Jr. (1970). Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science 167, 341–343. doi: 10.1126/science.167.3914.86

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gannon, P. J., Kheck, N., and Hof, P. R. (2008). Leftward interhemispheric asymmetry of macaque monkey temporal lobe language area homologue is evident at the cytoarchitectural, but not gross anatomic level. Brain Res. 1199, 62–73. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.12.041

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hamel, E., Vaucher, E., Tong, X.-K., and St. Georges, M. (2002). Neuronal messengers as mediators of microvascular tone in the cerebral cortex. Int. Congr. Ser. 1235, 267–276. doi: 10.1016/S0531-5131(02)00193-0

CrossRef Full Text

Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., and Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298, 1569–1579. doi: 10.1126/science.298.5598.1569

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hof, P. R., Cox, K., Young, W. G., Celio, M. R., Rogers, J., and Morrison, J. H. (1991). Parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons in the neocortex are resistant to degeneration in Alzheimer's disease. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 50, 451–462. doi: 10.1097/00005072-199107000-00006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hof, P. R., and Morrison, J. H. (1991). Neocortical neuronal subpopulations labeled by a monoclonal antibody to calbindin exhibit differential vulnerability in Alzheimer's disease. Exp. Neurol. 111, 293–301. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(91)90096-U

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hof, P. R., and Nimchinsky, E. A. (1992). Regional distribution of neurofilament and calcium-binding proteins in the cingulate cortex of the macaque monkey. Cereb. Cortex 2, 456–467. doi: 10.1093/cercor/2.6.456

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hof, P. R., Nimchinsky, E. A., Celio, M. R., Bouras, C., and Morrison, J. H. (1993). Calretinin-immunoreactive neocortical interneurons are unaffected in Alzheimer's disease. Neurosci. Lett. 152, 145–148. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(93)90504-E

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jerison, H. J. (1973). Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Jürgens, U. (1998). Neuronal control of mammalian vocalization, with special references to the squirrel monkey. Naturwissenschaften 85, 376–388. doi: 10.1007/s001140050519

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jürgens, U., and Ploog, D. (1970). Cerebral representation of vocalization in the squirrel monkey. Exp. Brain Res. 10, 532–554. doi: 10.1007/BF00234269

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kowall, N. W., and Beal, M. F. (1988). Cortical somatostatin, neuropeptide Y, and NADPH diaphorase neurons, normal anatomy and alterations in Alzheimer's disease. Ann. Neurol. 23, 105–114. doi: 10.1002/ana.410230202

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kuromitsu, J., Yokoi, A., Kawai, T., Nagasu, T., Aizawa, T., Haga, S., et al. (2001). Reduced neuropeptide Y levels in the frontal cortex of people with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Gene Expr. Patterns 1, 17–21. doi: 10.1016/S1567-133X(01)00003-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Larhammar, D. (1996). Evolution of neuropeptide Y, peptide YY and pancreatic polypeptide. Regul. Pept. 62, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/0167-0115(95)00169-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Larhammar, D., Ericsson, A., and Persson, H. (1987). Structure and expression of the rat neuropeptide Y gene. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 84, 2068–2072. doi: 10.1073/pnas.84.7.2068

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lepage, M., Ghaffar, O., Nyberg, L., and Tulving, E. (2000). Prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval mode. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 506–511. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.1.506

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lewis, D. A., Campbell, M. J., Foote, S. L., Goldstein, M., and Morrison, J. H. (1987). The distribution of tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive fibers in primate neocortex is widespread but regionally specific. J. Neurosci. 7, 279–290.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Lewis, J. W., and Van Essen, D. C. (2000). Mapping of architectonic subdivisions in the macaque monkey, with emphasis on parieto-occipital cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 428, 79–111. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20001204)428:1%3C79::AID-CNE7%3E3.0.CO;2-Q

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Martin-Ordas, G., Berntsen, D., and Call, J. (2013). Memory for distant past events in chimpanzees and orangutans. Curr. Biol. 23, 1438–1441. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Massoner, P., Kugler, K. G., Unterberger, K., Kuner, R., Mueller, L. A., Faith, M., et al. (2013). Characterization of transcriptional changes in ERG rearrangement-positive prostate cancer identifies the regulation of metabolic sensors such as neuropeptide Y. PLoS ONE 8:e55207. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055207

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mechawar, N., Cozzari, C., and Descarries, L. (2000). Cholinergic innervation in adult rat cerebral cortex: a quantitative immunocytochemical description. J. Comp. Neurol. 428, 305–318. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20001211)428:2%3C305::AID-CNE9%3E3.0.CO;2-Y

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Michel, M. C. (1991). Receptors for neuropeptide Y: multiple subtypes and multiple second messengers. Trends Neurosci. 12, 389–394. doi: 10.1016/0165-6147(91)90610-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mirra, S., Heyman, A., McKeel, S., Sumi, S., Crain, B., Brownlee, L., et al. (1991). The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer's disease (CERAD). Neurology 41, 479–486. doi: 10.1212/WNL.41.4.479

CrossRef Full Text

Morales-Medina, J. C., Dumont, Y., and Quirion, R. (2010). A possible role of neuropeptide Y in depression and stress. Brain Res. 1314, 194–205. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.077

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morrison, J. H., and Foote, S. L. (1986). Noradrenergic and serotonergic innervation of cortical, thalamic, and tectal visual structures in Old and New World monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 243, 117–138. doi: 10.1002/cne.902430110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morrison, J. H., Foote, S. L., O'Connor, D., and Bloom, F. E. (1982). Laminar, tangential and regional organization of the noradrenergic innervation of monkey cortex: dopamine beta-hydroxylase immunohistochemistry. Brain Res. Bull. 9, 309–319. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(82)90144-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muntané, G., Horvath, J. E., Hof, P. R., Ely, J. J., Hopkins, W. D., Raghanti, M. A., et al. (2014). Analysis of synaptic gene expression in primates reveals evolutionary changes in glutamatergic transmission. Cereb. Cortex doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht354. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nowak, M. A., and Komarova, N. L. (2001). Towards an evolutionary theory of language. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 288–295. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01683-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Hare, M. M., Tenmoku, S., Aakerlund, L., Hilsted, L., Johnsen, A., and Schwartz, T. W. (1988). Neuropeptide Y in guinea pig, rabbit, rat and man. Identical amino acid sequence and oxidation of methionine-17. Regul. Pept. 20, 293–304. doi: 10.1016/0167-0115(88)90064-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Okuda, J., Fiujii, T., Yamadori, A., Kawashima, R., Tsukiura, T., Fukatsu, R., et al. (1998). Participation of the prefrontal cortices in prospective memory: evidence from a PET study in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 253, 127–130. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00628-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Öngür, D., Ferry, A. T., and Price, J. L. (2003). Architectonic subdivision of the human orbital and medial prefrontal cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 460, 425–449. doi: 10.1002/cne.10609

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Öngür, D., and Price, J. L. (2000). The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb. Cortex 10, 206–219. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.206

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Passingham, R. E., and Wise, S. P. (2012). The Neurobiology of the Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Evolution, and the Origin of Insight. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Paus, T., Koski, L., Caramanos, Z., and Westbury, C. (1998). Regional differences in the effects of task difficulty and motor output on blood flow response in the human anterior cingulate cortex: a review of 107 PET activation studies. Neuroreport 9, 37–47. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199806220-00001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paus, T., Petrides, M., Evans, A. C., and Meyer, E. (1993). Role of the human anterior cingulate cortex in the control of oculomotor, manual, and speech responses: a positron emission tomography study. J. Neurophysiol. 70, 453–469.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Paxinos, G., Huang, X. F., and Toga, A. W. (2000). The Rhesus Monkey Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Petrides, M. (2005). Lateral prefrontal cortex: architectonic and functional organization. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 360, 781–795. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1631

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Petrides, M., Cadoret, G., and Mackey, S. (2005). Orofacial somatomotor responses in the macaque monkey homologue of Broca's area. Nature 435, 1235–1238. doi: 10.1038/nature03628

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Petrides, M., and Pandya, D. N. (1994). “Comparative architectonic analysis of the human and macaque frontal cortex,” in Handbook of Neuropsychology, eds F. Boller and J. Grafman (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 17–58.

Petrides, M., and Pandya, D. N. (1999). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and the macaque brain and corticocortical connection patterns. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 1011–1036. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00518.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Preuss, T. M., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1991a). Architectonics of the parietal and temporal association cortex in the strepsirhine primate Galago compared to the anthropoid primate Macaca. J. Comp. Neurol. 310, 475–506. doi: 10.1002/cne.903100403

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Preuss, T. M., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1991b). Myelo- and cytoarchitecture of the granular frontal cortex and surrounding regions in the strepsirhine primate Galago and the anthropoid primate Macaca. J. Comp. Neurol. 310, 429–474. doi: 10.1002/cne.903100402

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raghanti, M. A., Conley, T., Sudduth, J., Erwin, J. M., Stimpson, C. D., Hof, P. R., et al. (2013). Neuropeptide Y-immunoreactive neurons in the cerebral cortex of humans and other haplorhine primates. Am. J. Primatol. 75, 415–424. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22082

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raghanti, M. A., Stimpson, C. D., Marcinkiewicz, J. L., Erwin, J. M., Hof, P. R., and Sherwood, C. C. (2008a). Cholinergic innervation of the frontal cortex: differences among humans, chimpanzees, and macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 506, 409–424. doi: 10.1002/cne.21546

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raghanti, M. A., Stimpson, C. D., Marcinkiewicz, J. L., Erwin, J. M., Hof, P. R., and Sherwood, C. C. (2008b). Cortical dopaminergic innervation among humans, chimpanzees, and macaque monkeys: a comparative study. Neuroscience 155, 203–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.05.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Raghanti, M. A., Stimpson, C. D., Marcinkiewicz, J. L., Erwin, J. M., Hof, P. R., and Sherwood, C. C. (2008c). Differences in cortical serotonergic innervation among humans, chimpanzees, and macaque monkeys: a comparative study. Cereb. Cortex 18, 584–597. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm089

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rangani, R. J., Upadhya, M. A., Nakhate, K. T., Kokare, D. M., and Subhedar, N. K. (2012). Nicotine evoked improvement in learning and memory is mediated through NPY Y1 receptors in rat model of Alzheimer's disease. Peptides 33, 317–328. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2012.01.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rosabal, F. (1967). Cytoarchitecture of the frontal lobe of the squirrel monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 130, 87–108. doi: 10.1002/cne.901300202

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schenker, N. M. (2007). Dissertation: Comparative Analysis of Broca's Area in Hominoids. Ph.D. thesis, San Diego, CA: University of California.

Schenker, N. M., Buxhoeveden, D., Blackmon, W. L., Amunts, K., Zilles, K., and Semendeferi, K. (2008). A comparative quantitative analysis of cytoarchitecture and minicolumnar organization in Broca's area in humans and great apes. J. Comp. Neurol. 510, 117–128. doi: 10.1002/cne.21792

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schmitz, C., and Hof, P. R. (2000). Recommendations for straightforward and rigorous methods of counting neurons based on a computer simulation approach. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 20, 93–114. doi: 10.1016/S0891-0618(00)00066-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Semendeferi, K., Armstrong, E., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., and Van Hoesen, G. W. (2001). Prefrontal cortex in humans and apes: a comparative study of area 10. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 114, 224–241. doi: 10.1002/1096-8644(200103)114:3%3C224::AID-AJPA1022%3E3.3.CO;2-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shepherd, G. M. G., and Raastad, M. (2003). Axonal varicosity distributions along parallel fibers: a new angle on a cerebellar circuit. Cerebellum 2, 110–113. doi: 10.1080/14734220310011407

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Shepherd, G. M. G., Raastad, M., and Andersen, P. (2002). General and variable features of varicosity spacing along unmyelinated axons in the hippocampus and cerebellum. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 6340–6345. doi: 10.1073/pnas.052151299

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sherwood, C. C., Broadfield, D. C., Holloway, R. L., Gannon, P. J., and Hof, P. R. (2003). Variability of Broca's area homologue in African great apes: implications for language evolution. Anat. Rec. 271A, 276–285. doi: 10.1002/ar.a.10046

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sherwood, C. C., Cranfield, M. R., Mehlman, P. T., Lilly, A. A., Garbe, J. A., Whittier, C. A., et al. (2004). Brain structure variation in great apes, with attention to the mountain gorilla (Gorilla beringei beringei). Am. J. Primatol. 63, 149–164. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20048

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sherwood, C. C., Raghanti, M. A., Stimpson, C. D., Bonar, C. J., De Sousa, A. J., Preuss, T. M., et al. (2007). Scaling of inhibitory interneurons in areas V1 and V2 of anthropoid primates as revealed by calcium-binding protein immunohistochemistry. Brain Behav. Evol. 69, 176–195. doi: 10.1159/000096986

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Slomianka, L., and West, M. J. (2005). Estimators of the precision of stereological estimates: an example based on the CA1 pyramidal cell layer of rats. Neuroscience 136, 757–767. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.086

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, W. K. (1945). The functional significance of the rostral cingular cortex as revealed by its response to electrical stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 8, 241–255.

Spocter, M. A., Hopkins, W. D., Garrison, A. R., Bauerfeind, A., Stimpson, C. D., Hof, P. R., et al. (2010). Wernicke's area homologue in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and its relation to the appearance of modern human language. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 277, 2165–2174. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sutoo, D., Akiyama, K., and Yabe, K. (2001). Quantitative imaging of tyrosine hydroxylase and calmodulin in the human brain. J. Neurosci. Res. 63, 369–376. doi: 10.1002/1097-4547(20010301)63:5%3C369::AID-JNR1031%3E3.3.CO;2-V

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tatemoto, K., Carlquist, M., and Mutt, V. (1981). Neuropeptide Y- a novel brain peptide with structural similarities to peptide YY and pancreatic polypeptide. Nature 296, 659–660. doi: 10.1038/296659a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tempelmann, S., Kaminski, J., and Liebal, K. (2011). Focus on the essential: all great apes know when others are being attentive. Anim. Cogn. 14, 433–439. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0378-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tomasello, M., Call, J., and Hare, B. (2003). Chimpanzees understand psychological states- the question is which ones and to what extent. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 153–156. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00035-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vogt, B. A., Nimchinsky, E. A., Vogt, L. J., and Hof, P. R. (1995). Human cingulate cortex: surface features, flat maps, and cytoarchitecture. J. Comp. Neurol. 359, 490–506. doi: 10.1002/cne.903590310

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vogt, B. A., Vogt, L., Farber, N. B., and Bush, G. (2005). Architecture and neurocytology of monkey cingulate gyrus. J. Comp. Neurol. 485, 218–239. doi: 10.1002/cne.20512

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

von Bohlen und Halbach, O., and Dermietzel, R. (2006). Neurotransmitters and Neuromodulators. Handbook of Receptors and Biological Effects. 2nd Edn. Weinheim: Wiley-VCH Verlag GmbH & Co. doi: 10.1002/9783527609963

CrossRef Full Text

Watanabe-Sawaguchi, K., Kubota, K., and Arikuni, T. (1991). Cytoarchitecture and intrafrontal connections of the frontal cortex of the brain of the hamadryas baboon (Papio hamadryas). J. Comp. Neurol. 311, 108–133. doi: 10.1002/cne.903110109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: NPY, Broca's area, Wernicke's area, primate evolution

Citation: Raghanti MA, Edler MK, Meindl RS, Sudduth J, Bohush T, Erwin JM, Stimpson CD, Hof PR and Sherwood CC (2014) Humans and great apes share increased neocortical neuropeptide Y innervation compared to other haplorhine primates. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:101. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00101

Received: 02 December 2013; Accepted: 10 February 2014;
Published online: 28 February 2014.

Edited by:

Suzana Herculano-Houzel, Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Paul Manger, University of the Witwatersrand, South Africa
Todd M. Preuss, Emory University, USA

Copyright © 2014 Raghanti, Edler, Meindl, Sudduth, Bohush, Erwin, Stimpson, Hof and Sherwood. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mary Ann Raghanti, Department of Anthropology, School of Biomedical Sciences, Kent State University, 750 Hilltop Drive, 222 Lowry Hall, Kent, OH 44242, USA e-mail: mraghant@kent.edu

Download