Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 14 March 2014
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00142

Knowledge of facts mediate “continuous improvement” in elite sport: a comment on Stanley and Krakauer (2013)

  • Department of School of Sport, Health and Exercise Sciences, University of Hull, Hull, UK

A commentary on
Motor skill depends on knowledge of facts

by Stanley, J., and Krakauer, J. W. (2013). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:503. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00503

Traditional theories of motor skill learning (e.g., Fitts and Posner, 1967) and many contemporary perspectives in sport psychology (see Masters and Maxwell, 2008) and cognitive neuroscience (see Yarrow et al., 2010) argue that skilled action is guided by procedural or implicit knowledge. Researchers adopting this perspective believe that skilled performance proceeds rapidly, efficiently and without the need for conscious monitoring, or a reliance on propositional knowledge (what some might term declarative or explicit knowledge) to guide an activity. However, in a recent paper published in this journal, Stanley and Krakauer (2013) present a compelling argument which offers a stern challenge to the latter of these assumptions. Drawing on evidence from famous cases in neuroscience (e.g., the case of HM) and other research on the nature of skill, Stanley and Krakauer (2013) suggest that becoming proficient at any motor task, and maintaining and improving one's skill in that activity, is heavily influenced by the use of what they refer to as “knowledge of facts” (i.e., propositional knowledge). The aim of this brief commentary is to build on Stanley and Krakauer's work by pointing to empirical evidence (e.g., Collins et al., 1999), recent theory (e.g., Shusterman, 2009) and phenomenological descriptions (e.g., Cotterill et al., 2010) which suggest that “continuous improvement” at the elite level of sport is mediated by the “ongoing accrual and improving application of knowledge of facts about an activity” (Stanley and Krakauer, 2013, p. 2). More specifically, the current paper presents evidence which demonstrates how performers use knowledge of facts in two distinct sporting situations: (1) in the training context when the performer is seeking to improve “attenuated” movement patterns and (2) during the planning and strategizing that occurs in pre-performance routines during on-line competitive performance.

First, let us consider how elite athletes might use knowledge of facts to alter and improve movements in the practice context. According to Stanley and Krakauer (2013) the “use of knowledge to select actions continues in the skilled state because there is always the possibility to perform new actions based on further knowledge and then develop acuity at these actions” (p. 7). This appears to be precisely what happens in sport where elite level performers are driven by a desire to learn “new and better techniques” (Breivik, 2007, p. 127). For example, Padraig Harrington, the three-time major Golf champion, explained that he consciously sought to improve his technique throughout his entire career because “what I do every day is keep trying to evolve” (Eden, 2013). Indeed, there is a growing body of empirical evidence to suggest that conscious and deliberate attempts to refine and improve one's movement proficiency are a ubiquitous feature of elite performer's training regimes (see Collins et al., 1999; Ravn and Christensen, in press). Skill refinement might require the performer to draw on knowledge of facts on two occasions. Initially, performers may use “somesthetic awareness” (i.e., “a proprioceptive feel of what they are doing”; see Shusterman, 2009, p. 138) to identify problematic movements during competition. Here, the performer will draw on factual knowledge (which is established with the help of a coach in the practice context) about optimal movement positions and compare it to the kinesthetic feedback they receive from their current movement pattern in the performance context. Next, a coach may use “contrast” drills (see Collins et al., 1999) during training to increase the athlete's conscious awareness and understanding (thereby building on their propositional knowledge) of the correct (desired) vs. incorrect (current) movement positioning.

Second, skilled athletes may draw on propositional knowledge when strategizing during the pre-performance routine in closed-skilled sports (e.g., golf). To illustrate, in a naturalistic investigation of the attentional foci adopted by elite golfers during competition, Bernier et al. (2011) found that participants used the pre-shot routine to calculate how certain environmental factors (e.g., the strength of the wind) would influence the shape and trajectory of their tee-shot. Following this, elite golfers may rehearse their swing to establish a “proprioceptive feel” for accurate execution of the stroke (see Nicholls and Polman, 2008). In another study, Cotterill et al. (2010) discovered that elite golfers used the pre-performance routine to identify the risks associated with a particular choice of stroke. Elite golfers appear to bring previous experiences (e.g., those accrued during a practice round) to bear on current problems encountered in the competitive context. Together, these phenomenological descriptions suggest that elite athletes' can retrieve detailed knowledge structures from long-term memory and then re-instate them in working memory to mediate planning and problem-solving during competition. Here performers seem to employ the correct “average action” which, according to Stanley and Krakauer (2013), is “selected from a large potential repertoire… based on ever-accumulating knowledge of the task” (p. 9).

The research literature on the cognitive psychology and neuroscience of skill has tended to present a dichotomized view of the role knowledge plays in guiding skilled performance. According to this body of work, skilled performers engage in intuitive or absorbed coping (see Dreyfus and Dreyfus, 1980) which is predicted to facilitate movement and performance proficiency, or they use declarative knowledge to monitor or control movement which is predicted to have deleterious consequences (see Masters and Maxwell, 2008). Such perspectives appear to represent an unfortunate oversimplification. Indeed, a closer inspection of relevant studies suggests that elite performers are actually quite adept at switching between top-down cognitive processing (i.e., drawing on explicit rules) and bottom-up, embodied feeling and action (i.e., relying on spontaneity) and vice versa. This may happen, for example, when they relinquish spontaneity and use knowledge of facts to alter “attenuated” movement patterns in the practice context or when they are strategizing during competitive performance. Although Stanley and Krakauer (2013) use an example of how skilled performers demonstrate knowledge of what to do to initiate an action, the current paper provides evidence of how skill may be continually improved by proprioceptive knowledge of how to change/alter an action and how performance proficiency may be maintained by propositional knowledge of how best to plan an action during competition.

References

Bernier, M., Codron, R., Thienot, E., and Fournier, J. F. (2011). The attentional focus of expert golfers in training and competition: a naturalistic investigation. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 23, 326–341. doi: 10.1080/10413200.2011.561518

CrossRef Full Text

Breivik, G. (2007). Skillful coping in everyday life and in sport: a critical examination of the views of Heidegger and Dreyfus. J. Philos. Sport 34, 116–134. doi: 10.1080/00948705.2007.9714716

CrossRef Full Text

Collins, D., Morriss, C., and Trower, J. (1999). Getting it back: a case study of skill recovery in an elite athlete. Sport Psychol. 13, 288–298.

Cotterill, S. T., Sanders, R., and Collins, D. (2010). Developing effective pre-performance routines in golf: why don't we ask the golfer? J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 22, 51–64. doi: 10.1080/10413200903403216

CrossRef Full Text

Dreyfus, S. E., and Dreyfus, H. L. (1980). A Five-Stage Model of the Mental Activities Involved in Directed Skill Acquisition. Berkeley, CA: University of California Berkeley operations research center.

Eden, S. (2013). Stroke of Madness: How has Tiger Woods Managed to Overhaul his Swing Three Times? Retrieved from http://espn.go.com/golf/story//id/8865487/tiger-woods-reinvents-golf-swing-third-time-career-espn-magazine

Fitts, P. M., and Posner, M. I. (1967). Human Performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company.

Masters, R. S. W., and Maxwell, J. (2008). The theory of reinvestment. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1, 160–183. doi: 10.1080/17509840802287218

CrossRef Full Text

Nicholls, A. R., and Polman, R. C. J. (2008). Think aloud: acute stress and coping strategies during golf performances. Anxiety Stress Coping 21, 283–294. doi: 10.1080/10615800701609207

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ravn, S., and Christensen, M. K. (in press). Listening to the body? How phenomenological insights can be used to explore a golfer's experiences of the physicality of her body. Qual. Res. Sport Exerc. Health. doi: 10.1080/2159676x.2013.809378

CrossRef Full Text

Shusterman, R. (2009). Body consciousness and performance: somaesthetics east and west. J. Aesth. Art Crit. 67, 133–145. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6245.2009.01343.x

CrossRef Full Text

Stanley, J., and Krakauer, J. W. (2013). Motor skill depends on knowledge of facts. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:503. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00503

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yarrow, K., Brown, P., and Krakauer, J. W. (2010). Inside the brain of an elite athlete: the neural processes that support high achievements in sport. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 585–596. doi: 10.1038/nrn2672

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: skill, knowledge, expertise, embodiment, neuroscience, conscious processing

Citation: Toner J (2014) Knowledge of facts mediate “continuous improvement” in elite sport: a comment on Stanley and Krakauer (2013). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:142. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00142

Received: 07 January 2014; Accepted: 26 February 2014;
Published online: 14 March 2014.

Edited by:

Srikantan S. Nagarajan, University of California, San Francisco, USA

Copyright © 2014 Toner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: john.toner@hull.ac.uk

Download