Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 05 August 2015
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
Volume 9 - 2015 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00446

A kinematic study on (un)intentional imitation in bottlenose dolphins

  • 1Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Università di Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 2Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Università di Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 3Ab.Acus S.r.l., Milano, Italy
  • 4Oceanomare Delphis Onlus, Rimini, Italy
  • 5Centro Interdisciplinare Beniamino Segre, Accademia dei Lincei, Roma, Italy

The aim of the present study was to investigate the effect of observing other’s movements on subsequent performance in bottlenose dolphins. The imitative ability of non-human animals has intrigued a number of researchers. So far, however, studies in dolphins have been confined to intentional imitation concerned with the explicit request to imitate other agents. In the absence of instruction to imitate, do dolphins (un)intentionally replicate other’s movement features? To test this, dolphins were filmed while reaching and touching a stimulus before and after observing another dolphin (i.e., model) performing the same action. All videos were reviewed and segmented in order to extract the relevant movements. A marker was inserted post hoc via software on the videos upon the anatomical landmark of interest (i.e., rostrum) and was tracked throughout the time course of the movement sequence. The movement was analyzed using an in-house software developed to perform two-dimensional (2D) post hoc kinematic analysis. The results indicate that dolphins’ kinematics is sensitive to other’s movement features. Movements performed for the “visuomotor priming” condition were characterized by a kinematic pattern similar to that performed by the observed dolphin (i.e., model). Addressing the issue of spontaneous imitation in bottlenose dolphins might allow ascertaining whether the potential or impulse to produce an imitative action is generated, not just when they intend to imitate, but whenever they watch another conspecific’s behavior. In closing, this will clarify whether motor representational capacity is a by-product of factors specific to humans or whether more general characteristics such as processes of associative learning prompted by high level of encephalization could help to explain the evolution of this ability.

Introduction

The human capacity for imitation is well-known and is considered one of the hallmarks of human cognition and culture (Meltzoff and Prinz, 2002). From infancy on, humans habitually copy behaviors of every type across a variety of contexts (Meltzoff, 1996; Tomasello, 1999). For many decades, those studying imitation in children and animals focused on intentional imitation because it was thought that imitation must be controlled in order to play an important role in cognitive and social development, and to mediate cultural inheritance (Heyes, 2010).

Given the central role of imitation in human cognitive evolution and development, it is perhaps not surprising that a great amount of research has been devoted to ascertain whether this ability is shared among non-human animals (Galef, 1988; Fiorito and Scotto, 1992; Whiten and Ham, 1992; Leggio et al., 2000; Byrne and Bates, 2010).

A surprising aspect stemming from this body of work is that the animal most similar to humans in imitative abilities may not be found among our closest relatives, but rather among cetaceans (Herman, 2002). For instance, bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) exhibit a prominent ability to copy motor behaviors of conspecifics, humans, and other animals (Tayler and Saayman, 1973; Bauer and Johnson, 1994; Herman, 2002; Jaakkola et al., 2010). So far, imitation in dolphins has been chiefly investigated in terms of explicit request to imitate (e.g., Herman, 1980; Herman et al., 1989; Marino, 2002; Kuczaj and Highfill, 2005; Kuczaj and Walker, 2006). Rather, mimicry, imitative behavior that is not intended (Stürmer et al., 2000) and of which the imitator may be unaware (Chartrand and Bargh, 1999), has received little attention. Unintentional imitation is a compatibility effect in which the speed and/or accuracy of behavioral performance is modulated by the relationship between the topographic features of an observed action and the observer’s responses. In the absence of instruction to imitate, movement observation facilitates execution of the observed action. Such facilitation effects have been described in humans as a decrease in reaction time and an increase in average velocity when an observed and a subsequently executed hand action matched (i.e., Visuomotor Priming; see Craighero et al., 1998; Castiello et al., 2002; Edwards et al., 2003; Heyes et al., 2005). It is generally agreed that unintentional imitation effects result from a process in which action observation activates motor representations that are “similar” to the action observed (Heyes, 2011). The interesting question is then: does unintentional imitation depends on learned or genetically prespecified, stimulus-response (S-R) connections? Single-unit recording indicates that monkeys have mirror neurons in the premotor cortex, that is, cells that fire during observation and execution of the same action (di Pellegrino et al., 1992; Gallese et al., 1996). Similarly, the areas of the human brain that are activated by observation and execution of the same actions are sometimes called the “mirror neuron system” (Gazzola and Keysers, 2009; Kilner et al., 2009). Many developmental and comparative psychologists have suggested that nonhuman primates show “mimicry” (Tomasello, 1996; Meltzoff and Moore, 1997) or “response facilitation” (Byrne and Russon, 1998) because of simple, innate S-R links, and it is widely assumed that the perception–action matching properties of “mirror neurons” are present at birth (Ferrari et al., 2009). On the other hand, two theoretical accounts explain unintentional imitation as a by-product of learned associations between perceptual and motor representations: the ideomotor theory of action control (Prinz, 2005; Massen and Prinz, 2009) and the associative sequence learning model of imitation (Heyes et al., 2005; Catmur et al., 2009). In this connection, the extent to which animals can imitate might reflect representational capacity and is a matter of considerable importance (Whiten, 1998; Suddendorf and Whiten, 2001; Kuczaj and Yeater, 2006). This is one reason why studying unintentional imitation in dolphins is particularly significant. Conclusive evidence of unintentional imitation in a species as phylo-genetically distant from primates as dolphins would play a pivotal role in determining whether this motor representational capacity is a by-product of factors specific to primates or whether more general characteristics such as processes of associative learning related to a high level of encephalization could help to explain the evolution of this ability.

To investigate this issue, here we adapted a classic visuomotor priming paradigm. A dolphin “A” observed another dolphin (i.e., model) performing a reach-to-touch (with the rostrum) action towards a spherical object. Subsequently, the dolphin “A” performed the same action towards the same object. A control condition in which the dolphin performed the same movement in the absence of any motor priming was also included. On the basis of previous human findings based on a similar paradigm (e.g., Edwards et al., 2003; Pierno et al., 2008), we hypothesize that if the trailing dolphin is facilitated when primed by a dolphin model, then reach-to-touch movement should exhibit a higher average velocity than when visuomotor priming does not occur. Conversely, if no facilitation occurs, then primed and non-primed movements should not differ from a kinematical perspective.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Two adult bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus, S. and L., male and female, 20 and 21 years respectively) participated in the study. They swam in a pool of a round shape (20 m in diameter; capacity 1300 m3, surface 310 m2 and maximum depth of 5 m; Figure 1). The experimental procedure for the dolphins was approved by the committee for animal research of the University of Padova and adhered to the Ethical Guidelines for the Conduct of Research on Animals by Zoos and Aquariums issued by the World Association of Zoos and Aquariums (WAZA).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Aerial view of the pool (20 m in diameter; capacity 1300 m3, surface 310 m2 and maximum depth of 5 m) and relative position of the target stimulus, video camera and trainer’s platform.

Stimulus and Experimental Set Up

The stimulus was a plastic ball (diameter 20 cm) attached to a pinnacle located to a fixed distance of 10 m from the starting position (Figure 2). Both the dolphins participating in this experiment had already familiarized and played with the plastic ball attached to the pinnacle in previous years. We purposely abstained from introducing a new stimulus in the pool area to avoid possible learning effects.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Schematic section of the pool and field of view of the video camera. The stimulus was a plastic ball (diameter 20 cm) attached to a pinnacle located to a fixed distance of 10 m from the starting position.

Video Recording Technique

A total of 12 h of video footage was filmed during daylight hours between 10.00 a.m. and 12.00 p.m. in the time period between 27th of May and 7th of June, 2013. The video was filmed ad libitum using a A GoPro camera Hero3 (Black Edition Wi-Fi compatible) fastened to a rigid trampoline above the plane of motion. This procedure was utilized to guarantee a constant point of reference during movements taking place on the plane perpendicular to the camera axis. A frame of reference identifying X and Y axes as horizontal and vertical directions was manually set by an operator. In order to calibrate the space of interest, a plank of 324 cm with a perpendicular bar at the distal end (length 78 cm) was placed on three fixed points along the perimeter of the pool. A known length in the camera’s field of view and in the same plane as the movement was used as a measurement reference unit.

Procedure

The trainer stood at the border of the pool facing the dolphins. Then by means of a fixed signal she asked one of the dolphins (i.e., “A”) to reach the stimulus and touch it with the rostrum (Control condition; Figure 2). As soon as the stimulus was touched the trainer called the dolphin back to the starting position by using a whistle. Then the trainer asked another dolphin (i.e., model) to reach towards, touch the stimulus and then return to the starting position. Immediately after, the trainer asked the dolphin “A” to perform again the reach-to-touch action (Visuomotor Priming condition). The “model dolphin” was kept oriented toward the platform by the trainer during performance of dolphin “A” to prevent it from observing its movements. Nonetheless, to exclude the possibility that in some trial “model” dolphin might be influenced by dolphin “A”, all its data were removed from the analysis. The alternating sequence was repeated 10 times per daily session, with the dolphin “A” always following the model (trials: 3, 5, 7, 9, 11) except for the control condition (trial 1). Both dolphins acted as models and experimental subjects, in randomized sessions for 6 days. Notably, only one of them acted as experimental subject during each daily session, since only one of them could perform the control condition (pre-observation of the model dolphin).

Data Processing

Following data collection all videos were reviewed and segmented in order to extract the relevant reach-to-touch movements. For the “visuomotor priming” condition only movements in which the dolphin “A” observed the model performing the considered action before executing it were considered for subsequent analysis. Two independent reviewers who were unaware of the study rationale and blind to the experimental conditions scored each segment. They analyzed the footage frame-by-frame (frame duration: 20 ms) using an in-house software developed to perform post hoc kinematical analysis. Reliability between the two was quite high (Cohen’s κ = 0.86). A marker was placed post hoc on each subject’s rostrum. The starting position was defined as the water area frontal to the trainer’s platform at the border of the pool (2 m2). Movement onset was defined as the time at which the tangential velocity of the marker crossed a threshold (5 mm/s) and remained above it for longer than 500 ms. End of the movement was defined as the time at which the rostrum touched the stimulus. Movement tracking procedures were then performed in order to extract the kinematic parameter of interest. The analysis focused on the velocity profile to evaluate how the observer’s action changed following the observation of a model’s movements (Bisio et al., 2010). Average and Peak velocity, defined as the maximum velocity value occurring between movement onset and offset, were then extracted. Modifications of mean velocity in correspondence to changes of demonstrator’s velocity are usually considered proof of the occurrence of motion contagion.

Statistics

Two mixed-design repeated measures ANOVAs were carried out on average and peak velocity with condition (control, visuomotor priming) as within-subject factor and dolphin identity (S., L.) as between-subjects factor, to control for possible inter-individual differences. The strength and direction of the linear relationship between average and peak velocity exhibited by dolphins “A” and their models for the visuomotor priming condition was determined by means of a correlation coefficient (Pearson’s r). In particular, we correlated the average and peak velocity of each trial of dolphin “A” to the just-observed movement of the model.

Results

Statistical analyses revealed a significant effect of condition for both average (F(1,4) = 8.67, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.68) and peak (F(1,4) = 7.94, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.67) velocity. Dolphins were consistently faster after the visuomotor priming than for the control condition (Table 1). Notably, the interaction condition × dolphin identity was not significant (p > 0.05), suggesting that the priming influenced the motor performance for both dolphins. Motor contagion effects were confirmed by the high level of correlation between “A” dolphins and their models in terms of both average and peak velocity (Table 1; see Figure 3 for a representative session).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Average and peak velocity (m/s) mean values recorded from the “A” and the “model” dolphins.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Trial by trial average velocity (m/s) recorded from the model (upper panel, A) and “A” (lower panel, B) dolphins during a representative session (Day 1). (C) Linear relationship between average velocity exhibited by dolphins “A” and model for the visuomotor priming condition at Day 1.

Discussion

Collectively, these findings provide definite evidence that dolphins considered in this study showed visuomotor priming effects, which might be due directly to activation by movement observation of motor representations coding topographically similar responses. These findings, therefore, offer the first convincing evidence that a non-primate species, the bottlenose dolphin, is capable of unintentional imitation.

Our study reveals interesting similarities between the way dolphins and humans respond to action observation. It suggests that action observation activates processes involved in action execution.

Synchronous behavior exhibited by many dolphin species are classical examples of on-line motor mimicry (Connor et al., 2000; Bauer and Harley, 2001). Rather our results, based on a postponed facilitation effect, suggest a representational system that encodes information about other’s behaviors in the same way it encodes information about one’s own behavior (Gopnik, 1993). These data fit well with theories assuming that the long-term S-R associations mediating unintentional imitation are products of learning (Heyes et al., 2005; Prinz, 2005; Catmur et al., 2009; Massen and Prinz, 2009). In this view, we cautiously suggest that motor representations activated following action observation might reflect an automatic resonance mechanism of motor structures paralleling observed movements. This would imply that the emergence of mirror-like manifestations are not a by-product of factors specific to primates, but instead might be attributable to general processes of associative learning fostered by a high degree of encephalization and cognitive ability. At the same time, the present results cannot exclude a speculative interpretation of unintentional imitation in non-primate species. Mirror neurons fire when monkeys perform an action, but also when the animal observe somebody else performing the same action (Gallese et al., 1996; Rizzolatti et al., 2001). Following this discovery, studies have provided some evidence that similar bimodal cells also exist in the human brain (Mukamel et al., 2010). Here, findings from our study might indicate the presence of a similar mirror mechanism in dolphins too.

In conclusion, we suggest that even when dolphins do not intend to imitate, the perception of action might activate the same neural (mirror neurons or mirror areas) or representational (common codes, shared representations or vertical associations) structures that are involved in the production of the perceived action. This might entail that in dolphins, rather than being distantly related by rules, the perception and the execution of action depend on the same systems, and the potential (or impulse) to produce an imitative action is generated not just when they intend to imitate, but whenever they watch another dolphin’s behavior.

To date, hypotheses about the evolution of unintentional imitation effects, as defined here, have largely confined on primates. Our findings show a striking case of convergence in the face of profound differences in neuroanatomical characteristics and evolutionary history. We are aware that the issue of whether the unintentional imitation effects exhibited by dolphins rely on similar mechanisms as primates remains partially unanswered, but this approach is promising and will shed new light in the debate on the “intentional” nature of imitative ability.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by a grant from Università degli Studi di Padova, Bando Giovani Studiosi 2011, L. n.240/2010 to LS, and from Università degli Studi di Padova, Progetto Strategico (N. 2010XPMFW4) to UC. We thank Rimini Delfinario owner and staff for their support, Serena Catalano and Valeria Mariotti for their collaboration.

References

Bauer, G. B., and Harley, H. E. (2001). The mimetic dolphin. Behav. Brain Sci. 24, 326–327. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x01243969

CrossRef Full Text

Bauer, G. B., and Johnson, C. M. (1994). Trained motor imitation by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Percept. Mot. Skills 79, 1307–1315. doi: 10.2466/pms.1994.79.3.1307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bisio, A., Stucchi, N., Jacono, M., Fadiga, L., and Pozzo, T. (2010). Automatic versus voluntary motor imitation: effect of visual context and stimulus velocity. PLoS One 5:e13506. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, R. W., and Bates, L. A. (2010). Primate social cognition: uniquely primate, uniquely social, or just unique? Neuron 65, 815–830. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, R. W., and Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: a hierarchical approach. Behav. Brain Sci. 21, 667–684. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X98001745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Castiello, U., Lusher, D., Mari, M., Edwards, M. G., and Humphreys, G. W. (2002). “Observing a human or a robotic hand grasping an object: differential motor priming effects,” in Attention and Performance XIX, eds W. Prinz and B. Hommel (Cambridge: MIT Press), 315–333.

Catmur, C., Walsh, V., and Heyes, C. (2009). Associative sequence learning: the role of experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 2369–2380. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chartrand, T. L., and Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 893–910. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connor, R. C., Wells, R., Mann, J., and Read, A. (2000). “The bottlenose dolphin,” in Cetacean Societies: Field Studies of Dolphins and Whales, eds J. Mann, R. C. Connor, P. L. Tyack, and H. Whitehead (Chicago: The University of Chicago Press), 91–126.

Google Scholar

Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., and Umiltà, C. (1998). Visuomotor priming. Vis. Cogn. 5, 109–125. doi: 10.1080/713756780

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Exp. Brain Res. 91, 176–180. doi: 10.1007/bf00230027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, M. G., Humphreys, G. W., and Castiello, U. (2003). Motor facilitation following action observation: a behavioral study in prehensile action. Brain Cogn. 53, 495–502. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00210-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, P. F., Bonini, L., and Fogassi, L. (2009). From monkey mirror neurons to primate behaviors: possible ‘direct’ and ‘indirect’ pathways. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 2311–2323. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiorito, G., and Scotto, P. (1992). Observational learning in octopus vulgaris. Science 256, 545–547. doi: 10.1126/science.256.5056.545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galef, B. J. Jr. (1988). “Imitation in animals: history, definition and interpretation of data from the psychological laboratory,” in Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives, eds T. R. Zentall and B. G. Galef Jr. (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 3–28.

Google Scholar

Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119, 593–609. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.2.593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gazzola, V., and Keysers, C. (2009). The observation and execution of actions share motor and somatosensory voxels in all tested subjects: single-subject analyses of unsmoothed fMRI data. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1239–1255. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gopnik, A. (1993). How we know our minds: the illusion of first-person knowledge of intentionality. Behav. Brain Sci. 16, 1–14. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00028636

CrossRef Full Text

Herman, L. M. (1980). “Cognitive characteristics of dolphins,” in Cetacean behavior: Mechanisms and Functions, ed. L. M. Herman (New York: Wiley Interscience), 363–429.

Herman, L. M. (2002). “Vocal, social and self-imitation by bottlenosed dolphins,” in Imitation in Animals and Artifacts, eds C. Nehaniv and K. Dautenhahn (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 63–108.

Google Scholar

Herman, L. M., Hovancik, J. R., Gory, J. D., and Bradshaw, G. L. (1989). Generalization of visual matching by a bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): evidence for invariance of cognitive performance with visual and auditory materials. J. Exp. Psychol. Animal Behav. Proc. 15, 124–136. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.15.2.124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heyes, C. (2010). Where do mirror neurons come from? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 575–583. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heyes, C. (2011). Automatic imitation. Psychol. Bull. 137, 463–483. doi: 10.1037/a0022288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heyes, C., Bird, G., Johnson, H., and Haggard, P. (2005). Experience modulates automatic imitation. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 22, 233–240. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaakkola, K., Guarino, E., and Rodriguez, M. (2010). Blindfolded imitation in a bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 23, 671–688.

Google Scholar

Kilner, J. M., Neal, A., Weiskopf, N., Friston, K. J., and Frith, C. D. (2009). Evidence of mirror neurons in human inferior frontal gyrus. J. Neurosci. 29, 10153–10159. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2668-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuczaj, S. A., and Highfill, L. E. (2005). Dolphin play: evidence for cooperation and culture? Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 705–706. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x05370129

CrossRef Full Text

Kuczaj, S. A., and Walker, R. T. (2006). “How do dolphins solve problems,” in Comparative Cognition: Experimental Explorations of Animal Intelligence, eds E. A. Wasserman and T. R. Zentall (New York: Oxford University Press), 580–600.

Kuczaj, S. A., and Yeater, D. B. (2006). Dolphin imitation: who, what, when and why? Aquat. Mammals 32, 413–422. doi: 10.1578/am.32.4.2006.413

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leggio, M. G., Molinari, M., Neri, P., Graziano, A., Mandolesi, L., and Petrosini, L. (2000). Representation of actions in rats: the role of cerebellum in learning spatial performances by observation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 97, 2320–2325. doi: 10.1073/pnas.040554297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, L. (2002). Convergence of complex cognitive abilities in cetaceans and primates. Brain Behav. Evol. 59, 21–32. doi: 10.1159/000063731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Massen, C., and Prinz, W. (2009). Movements, actions and tool-use actions: an ideomotor approach to imitation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 2349–2358. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). “The human infant as imitative generalist: a 20-year progress report on infant imitation with implications for comparative psychology,” in Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, eds B. G. Galef and C. M. Heyes (New York: Academic Press), 347–370.

Google Scholar

Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: a theoretical model. Early Dev. Parent. 6, 179–192. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0917(199709/12)6:3/4<179::aid-edp157>3.0.co;2-r

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meltzoff, A. N., and Prinz, W. (Eds). (2002). The Imitative Mind: Development, Evolution and Brain Bases. (Vol. 6). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A. D., Kaplan, J., Iacoboni, M., and Fried, I. (2010). Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Curr. Biol. 20, 750–756. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierno, A. C., Mari, M., Lusher, D., and Castiello, U. (2008). Robotic movement elicits visuomotor priming in children with autism. Neuropsychologia 46, 448–454. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prinz, W. (2005). “Representational foundations of intentional action,” in The Human Body: Perception from the Inside Out, eds G. Knoblich, I. Thornton, M. Grosjean, and M. Shiffrar (New York: Oxford University Press), 399–411.

Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 661–670. doi: 10.1038/35090060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stürmer, B., Aschersleben, G., and Prinz, W. (2000). Correspondence effects with manual gestures and postures: a study of imitation. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 26, 1746–1759. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.26.6.1746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suddendorf, T., and Whiten, A. (2001). Mental evolution and development: evidence for secondary representation in children, great ages and other animals. Psychol. Bull. 127, 629–650. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.5.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tayler, C. K., and Saayman, G. S. (1973). Imitative behavior by Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins (Tursiopsaduncus) in captivity. Behaviour 44, 286–298. doi: 10.1163/156853973x00436

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (1996). “Do apes ape?,” in Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, eds C. M. Heyes and B. G. Galef (New York: Academic Press), 319–346.

Tomasello, M. (1999). The human adaptation for culture. Ann. Rev. Anthropol. 28, 509–529. doi: 10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.509

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whiten, A. (1998). Imitation of the sequential structure of actions by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 112, 270–281.

Google Scholar

Whiten, A., and Ham, R. (1992). “On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: reappraisal of a century of research,” in Advances in the Study of behavior, eds P. J. B. Slater, J. S. Rosenblatt, C. Beer, and M. Milinski (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 239–283.

Google Scholar

Keywords: imitation, mimicry, bottlenose dolphins, mirror neurons, automatic imitation, visuomotor priming

Citation: Sartori L, Bulgheroni M, Tizzi R and Castiello U (2015) A kinematic study on (un)intentional imitation in bottlenose dolphins. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:446. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00446

Received: 19 March 2015; Accepted: 23 July 2015;
Published: 05 August 2015.

Edited by:

Matthias Kliegel, University of Geneva, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Matteo Candidi, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Mariella Pazzaglia, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2015 Sartori, Bulgheroni, Tizzi and Castiello. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Luisa Sartori, Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale and Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Università di Padova, Via Venezia 8, 35131 Padova, Italy, luisa.sartori@unipd.it

Download