Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 21 June 2016
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
Volume 10 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00313

Using Computational Neuroscience to Define Common Input to Spinal Motor Neurons

  • 1Black Dog Institute, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 2Systems Neuroscience Group, QIMR Berghofer Medical Research Institute, Brisbane, QLD, Australia
  • 3Sobell Department of Motor Neuroscience and Movement Disorders, University College London, London, UK
  • 4National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, London, UK
  • 5Metro North Mental Health Service, Royal Brisbane and Women's Hospital, Brisbane, QLD, Australia

Common input is a widely used concept in motor neurophysiology. It embodies the notion that inputs to individual spinal motor neurons (MNs) are not unique, but partly shared across MNs, and is considered the main explanation for synchronized activity of MNs (Bremner et al., 1991; Farmer et al., 1993; Boonstra and Breakspear, 2012; Farina and Negro, 2015). Motor-unit synchronization was first observed in the time domain using cross-correlation histograms from pairs of MNs (Sears and Stagg, 1976). This was later extended to the frequency domain by estimating coherence between spike trains to reveal the frequency content of common input (Farmer et al., 1993). In addition to measurements of individual MNs, coherence can also be estimated between the surface EMG of different muscles—referred to as intermuscular coherence—to assess common input shared across motor-unit pools (Boonstra and Breakspear, 2012). Common input is considered relevant for motor control as it may provide a mechanism to reduce the dimensionality of the control signal, thereby simplifying motor control (Farmer, 1998).

Recently the existence of common input has been debated: while some argue that synergistic muscles share most of their synaptic input (Laine et al., 2015), others have argued that common input provides no explanation for MN synchronization (Kline and De Luca, 2016). Apart from common input being used to explain different types of MN synchronization, this dispute mainly arises from the absence of a clear definition of common input (cf. Kirkwood, 2016). Initially, Sears and colleagues defined common input structurally, that is, as resulting from branched presynaptic axons (Sears and Stagg, 1976; Kirkwood and Sears, 1978). Later, presynaptic synchronization—synchronization of neurons that project to the MNs—was proposed as an explanation for broad-peak MN synchronization (central peak in cross-correlogram with a duration of 40–60 ms), which is observed following a lesion of the direct mono-synaptic inputs to MNs and cannot be explained by branched presynaptic axons (Kirkwood et al., 1982). This is a functional definition as it involves correlations between input activities, rather than shared anatomical connections. Although the authors themselves did not refer to this mechanism as common input, the term common input has been invoked quite loosely in recent years, reflecting either shared structural or functional inputs.

To resolve this dispute and determine whether MN synchronization is caused by common input, we first need to agree on the definitions of common input and MN synchronization. Computational models are particularly useful as they provide a quantitative and unambiguous description of variables. Computational neuroscience can be used to define common input in terms of a set of equations and determine its effect on MN synchronization (Boonstra, 2013).

We therefore applaud the contributions by Farina and colleagues who provided equations to define common input (Castronovo et al., 2015; Farina and Negro, 2015). They describe the inputs to MNs as

vi(t)=μi+αisC(t)+ni(t),(1)

where vi(t) is the synaptic current to the i-th MN, μi a constant offset, αisC(t) the time-varying input that is common to all MNs (apart from a constant scaling factor αi), and ni(t) is independent white noise. The distinction between μi, αisC(t) and ni(t) is not based on anatomical constraints, i.e., where these inputs originate from, but rather decomposes the total input signal into a DC, a correlated and an uncorrelated component, respectively. This is therefore a functional definition, as common input is defined in terms of correlations between input activities.

Using this formal definition, the effects of common input on force production and MN synchronization can be investigated using analytic and computational approaches. Farina and Negro (2015) show that, when defined in this way, common input is the only input component that influences force generation and that correlated activity of MNs is thus necessary for force control. This is a consequence of the approximately linear input-output relationship of a MN pool (Stegeman et al., 2010; Farina et al., 2014). To generate force muscles require a net excitatory drive and hence the cumulative effects of correlated inputs. This is because uncorrelated inputs average out as they consist of numerous excitatory and inhibitory inputs (Farina et al., 2014; Farina and Negro, 2015). Using this functional definition the notion that MN synchronization is caused by common input becomes therefore true by necessity. Indeed, a coupling between oscillators is necessary for synchronization to occur (Pikovsky et al., 2001). That is, if the outputs of MNs are correlated, the inputs of MNs need to be correlated (functional definition of common input). However, correlated inputs do not necessarily mean these inputs originated from a common source (structural definition of common input).

While a positive step toward standardization, the proposed definition of common input risks tautology and it does not address the underlying neurophysiological cause of correlated inputs. We propose an alternative definition of common input consistent with its original interpretation (cf. Perkel et al., 1967), one that builds on a well-established approach in computational neuroscience, and that is to introduce structural constraints in the definition of common input. Thus we will use a formal biophysical modeling framework and consider a more realistic connection topology. First, we recast Equation (1) within the framework of a spiking MN,

CdVidt=gi(ViEi)+vi(t),(2)

where Vi is the membrane potential of the i-th MN (Boonstra and Breakspear, 2012; Heitmann et al., 2015). Note here that the total synaptic current vi(t) from the LHS of Equation (1) appears here as the second term on the RHS. When this synaptic current is sufficient to push the membrane potential past a threshold (VT, the spiking threshold), the neuron fires a spike and is instantaneously reset to a lower value (VR, the reset potential). This is the simplest (integrate and fire) spiking model—and an abstract model of MNs (see Heckman and Enoka, 2012, for a review of MN physiology)—with a single leaky current given by the first term on the RHS with Nernst potential E and conductance g (Burkitt, 2006). If the summed synaptic currents vi(t) exceed the leaky current, the membrane potential rises toward the firing threshold V = VT whereupon the neuron issues a single spike and is reset to its resting potential: this effect of spiking and resetting essentially incorporates the first order influence of all voltage-dependent channels in more complex models such as the Hodgkin-Huxley model (Kistler et al., 1997).

The next step is to recognize that the total synaptic current vi(t) into the cell soma is a filtered version of all small and transient currents arising at synapses following presynaptic spikes:

CdVidt=gi(ViEi)+αjcijF(t,tj),(3)

where F is a function that represents synapto-dendritic filtering and turns the discrete presynaptic spike times into smooth currents within the soma; cij represent the effective influence by presynaptic neurons j on the post-synaptic MN i (Gerstein et al., 1989), and α is a scaling function that modulates the total sum of all synaptic currents from the dendritic tree into the soma. Negative weights in cij can be used to model inhibitory inputs. For each MN i, cij is a vector weighting all possible sources j projecting to the MN. Presynaptic spikes for each neuron are recorded in the spike train tj and usually only the most recent spike time is recorded, i.e., when a new spike arrives, this is reset to tj = t. The dendritic filter can take several forms, but usually a simple form that allows exponential rise and decay is used,

F(t,tj)=e(tjtτ1)e(tjtτ2)

where τ1 is the rise time and τ2 is the decay time (Kirkwood and Sears, 1978; Burkitt, 2006).

Finally, as with Equation (1), it is usual to introduce a noisy (stochastic) term to acknowledge that there are membrane fluctuations that do not solely reflect input spike trains: these may be due to weak synaptic currents not explicitly modeled as presynaptic spikes, or from truly random events such as the inherently noisy opening and closing of membrane channels (Faisal et al., 2008). Hence we have

CdVidt=gi(ViEi)+αjcijF(t,tj)+ηi(t),(4)

where ηi(t) is (zero mean) white or colored noise, unique to each time step and neuron.

What are the differences between Equations (1) and (4)? First, the influence is modeled not just at the level of synaptic inputs but rather at the cell membrane of MNs, which are the ultimate effectors (through their own spikes) on muscle activity. The more complex formulation of Equation (4) allows the variable influence of spikes on the cell membrane and the balance between incoming and leaky currents. Hence the result is not a passive algebraic addition as in Equation (1), but rather a time dependent process that filters and integrates the weighted synaptic inputs.

Second, a generic coupling matrix C = {cij} allows one to model any potential configuration of inputs, and should be constrained by knowledge of anatomy. The input configuration in Equation (1) can be obtained by considering a single source j1 that projects to all motoneurons i, as reflected by strong weights along the corresponding row of the coupling matrix. Each neuron also has an independent stochastic term ηi and the amount of common input can hence by varied by changing the scaling function α. Comparing Equation (1) and Equation (4), it hence seems that—rather than defining the ratio of common and individual MN input—the model by Farina and colleagues (Castronovo et al., 2015; Farina and Negro, 2015) defines the ratio between input and noise, as the noise term ηi captures weak currents that do not reflect input spike trains.

Equation (4) is very generic and the inputs tj can be temporally homogeneous or oscillatory and uncorrelated or synchronized. Likewise, the coupling matrix C can be extended to include multiple input sources arising from descending and ascending pathways and spinal interneurons (Latash, 2008), thus enabling a more realistic connection topology. Realistic coupling matrices may be high dimensional. To reduce dimensionality and make the system more tractable, a mean-field approximation can be used to consider connectivity between ensembles of neurons (like a motor neuron pool) rather than between individual neurons (Deco et al., 2008). When estimating connectivity from empirical data, standard dimension reduction approaches can be used to obtain a more low dimensional representation (e.g., Roca et al., 2009).

Using Equation (4), independent inputs reflect presynaptic neurons that selectively innervate a single MN, as reflected by a strong connection weight and zeros along the rest of the corresponding row of coupling matrix C. In contrast, common inputs are reflected by strong connection weights along the whole row j. The amount of common input between two MNs can thus be quantified by the correlation between the weights in the two corresponding columns i1 and i2 of cij. This definition of common input is a structural definition of common input, as it quantifies the proportion of anatomical connections shared between MNs. The difference between the functional and structural definition of common input can be illustrated by a simple example: consider a presynaptic neuron j1 innervates MN i1 and a presynaptic neuron j2 innervates MN i2. If the spike trains of j1 and j2 are correlated, this would be considered common input according to Equation (1). In contrast, this is not considered as common input according to Equation (4), as the rows of cij are not correlated.

We hence follow the definition by Sears and Stagg (1976), but extend it to include all structural connections that are shared across MNs. A key attribute of Equation (4) is that it links structure and function, that is, it relates functional connectivity (observed MN synchronization captured by Vi) to effective connectivity (captured by coupling matrix cij). Effective connectivity depends on some model of the influence one neuronal system exerts over another (Friston, 1994; Horwitz, 2003). By providing a structural definition of common input in Equation (4), we position the debate on common input within the broader context of structure-function relationship in computational neuroscience (Honey et al., 2007; Bullmore and Sporns, 2009).

Having a realistic generative model may have important advantages. In particular, MN firing patterns can be “reverse engineered” to identify the underlying organization of synaptic inputs and intrinsic properties of MNs (Heckman and Enoka, 2012). For example, a Bayesian framework can be used to estimate effective connectivity from observed functional connectivity (Friston et al., 2003). We recently showed that functional connectivity between leg muscles reveals a rich structure of connections between muscles (Boonstra et al., 2015). Using model inversion techniques these functional connectivity patterns may be used to uncover structural pathways in the motor system that are difficult to assess directly.

In summary, we propose a structural definition of common input by using standard, well-established models in computational neuroscience. Rather than defining common input as the correlation between presynaptic activities, we suggest to define common input as the correlation between the anatomical connections or structural links that innervate MNs. A structural definition of common input steers the debate toward a pertinent research question: What are the neural pathways that cause the different types of synchronization that are experimentally observed between pairs and groups of MNs?

Author Contributions

TB, SF, and MB contributed to the conception and design of the work. TB drafted the initial version of manuscript. TB, SF, and MB revised the manuscript and approved the final version.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

SF acknowledges funding support from UCLH BRC. MB and TB acknowledge the ARC Centre of Excellence in Integrative Brain Function and NHMRC.

References

Boonstra, T. W. (2013). The potential of corticomuscular and intermuscular coherence for research on human motor control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:855. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, T. W., and Breakspear, M. (2012). Neural mechanisms of intermuscular coherence: implications for the rectification of surface electromyography. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 796–807. doi: 10.1152/jn.00066.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, T. W., Danna-Dos Santos, A., Xie, H.-B., Roerdink, M., Stins, J. F., and Breakspear, M. (2015). Muscle networks: connectivity analysis of EMG activity during postural control. Sci. Rep. 5:17830. doi: 10.1038/srep17830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bremner, F. D., Baker, J. R., and Stephens, J. A. (1991). Variation in the degree of synchronization exhibited by motor units lying in different finger muscles in man. J. Physiol. 432, 381–399. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1991.sp018390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burkitt, A. N. (2006). A review of the integrate-and-fire neuron model: I. Homogeneous synaptic input. Biol. Cybern 95, 1–19. doi: 10.1007/s00422-006-0068-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castronovo, A. M., Negro, F., Conforto, S., and Farina, D. (2015). The proportion of common synaptic input to motor neurons increases with an increase in net excitatory input. J. Appl. Physiol. 119, 1337–1346. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00255.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deco, G., Jirsa, V. K., Robinson, P. A., Breakspear, M., and Friston, K. (2008). The dynamic brain: from spiking neurons to neural masses and cortical fields. PLoS Comput. Biol. 4:e1000092. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faisal, A. A., Selen, L. P., and Wolpert, D. M. (2008). Noise in the nervous system. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 292–303. doi: 10.1038/nrn2258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farina, D., and Negro, F. (2015). Common synaptic input to motor neurons, motor unit synchronization, and force control. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 43, 23–33. doi: 10.1249/JES.0000000000000032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farina, D., Negro, F., and Dideriksen, J. L. (2014). The effective neural drive to muscles is the common synaptic input to motor neurons. J. Physiol. 592, 3427–3441. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.273581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farmer, S. F. (1998). Rhythmicity, synchronization and binding in human and primate motor systems. J. Physiol. 509 (Pt 1), 3–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1998.003bo.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farmer, S. F., Bremner, F. D., Halliday, D. M., Rosenberg, J. R., and Stephens, J. A. (1993). The frequency content of common synaptic inputs to motoneurones studied during voluntary isometric contraction in man. J. Physiol. 470, 127–155. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1993.sp019851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J. (1994). Functional and effective connectivity in neuroimaging: a synthesis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2, 56–78. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460020107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Harrison, L., and Penny, W. (2003). Dynamic causal modelling. Neuroimage 19, 1273–1302. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00202-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerstein, G. L., Bedenbaugh, P., and Aertsen, M. H. (1989). Neuronal assemblies. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 36, 4–14. doi: 10.1109/10.16444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heckman, C. J., and Enoka, R. M. (2012). Motor unit. Compr. Physiol. 2, 2629–2682. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c100087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heitmann, S., Boonstra, T., Gong, P. L., Breakspear, M., and Ermentrout, B. (2015). The rhythms of steady posture: motor commands as spatially organized oscillation patterns. Neurocomputing 170, 3–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neucom.2015.01.088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honey, C. J., Kotter, R., Breakspear, M., and Sporns, O. (2007). Network structure of cerebral cortex shapes functional connectivity on multiple time scales. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 10240–10245. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701519104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horwitz, B. (2003). The elusive concept of brain connectivity. Neuroimage 19, 466–470. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00112-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirkwood, P. A. (2016). The origin of motoneuron synchronization. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 1077–1078. doi: 10.1152/jn.01027.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirkwood, P. A., and Sears, T. A. (1978). The synaptic connexions to intercostal motoneurones as revealed by the average common excitation potential. J. Physiol. 275, 103–134. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirkwood, P. A., Sears, T. A., Tuck, D. L., and Westgaard, R. H. (1982). Variations in the time course of the synchronization of intercostal motoneurones in the cat. J. Physiol. 327, 105–135. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1982.sp014223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kistler, W. M., Gerstner, W., and Vanhemmen, J. L. (1997). Reduction of the Hodgkin-Huxley equations to a single-variable threshold model. Neural Comput. 9, 1015–1045. doi: 10.1162/neco.1997.9.5.1015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, J. C., and De Luca, C. J. (2016). Synchronization of motor unit firings: an epiphenomenon of firing rate characteristics not common inputs. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 178–192. doi: 10.1152/jn.00452.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laine, C. M., Martinez-Valdes, E., Falla, D., Mayer, F., and Farina, D. (2015). Motor neuron pools of synergistic thigh muscles share most of their synaptic input. J. Neurosci. 35, 12207–12216. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0240-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latash, M. L. (2008). Neurophysiological Basis of Movement. Campaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Perkel, D. H., Gerstein, G. L., and Moore, G. P. (1967). Neuronal spike trains and stochastic point processes. II. Simultaneous spike trains. Biophys. J. 7, 419–440. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(67)86597-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pikovsky, A., Rosenblum, M., and Kurths, J. (2001). Synchronization: A Universal Concept in Nonlinear Sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Roca, P., Riviere, D., Guevara, P., Poupon, C., and Mangin, J. F. (2009). Tractography-based parcellation of the cortex using a spatially-informed dimension reduction of the connectivity matrix. Med. Image Comput. Comput. Assist. Interv. 12, 935–942. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-04268-3_115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sears, T. A., and Stagg, D. (1976). Short-term synchronization of intercostal motoneurone activity. J. Physiol. 263, 357–381. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stegeman, D. F., Van De Ven, W. J., Van Elswijk, G. A., Oostenveld, R., and Kleine, B. U. (2010). The alpha-motoneuron pool as transmitter of rhythmicities in cortical motor drive. Clin. Neurophysiol. 121, 1633–1642. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.03.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: common input, motor neurons, connectivity analysis, computational modeling, synchronized oscillations, coherence analysis, spiking neurons

Citation: Boonstra TW, Farmer SF and Breakspear M (2016) Using Computational Neuroscience to Define Common Input to Spinal Motor Neurons. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:313. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00313

Received: 21 April 2016; Accepted: 08 June 2016;
Published: 21 June 2016.

Edited by:

Mikhail Lebedev, Duke University, USA

Reviewed by:

David M. Halliday, University of York, UK
Dario Farina, University Medical Center Göttingen, Georg-August-Universität Göttingen, Germany
Roger Enoka, University of Colorado Boulder, USA

Copyright © 2016 Boonstra, Farmer and Breakspear. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tjeerd W. Boonstra, t.boonstra@unsw.edu.au

Download