Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 03 August 2016
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 10 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00392

The Neurological Traces of Look-Alike Avatars

  • 1Microsoft Research, Redmond, WA, USA
  • 2Experimental Virtual Environments for Neuroscience and Technology (EVENT) Laboratory, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 3Institute of Neuroscience, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 4Catalan Institute for Research and Advanced Studies, ICREA, Barcelona, Spain
  • 5Department of Computer Science, University College London, London, UK
  • 6Cognition and Brain Plasticity Group, Bellvitge Biomedical Research Institute—IDIBELL, L’Hospitalet de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain
  • 7Department of Basic Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain

We designed an observational study where participants (n = 17) were exposed to pictures and look-alike avatars pictures of themselves, a familiar friend or an unfamiliar person. By measuring participants’ brain activity with electroencephalography (EEG), we found face-recognition event related potentials (ERPs) in the visual cortex, around 200–250 ms, to be prominent for the different familiarity levels. A less positive component was found for self-recognized pictures (P200) than pictures of others, showing similar effects in both real faces and look-alike avatars. A rapid adaptation in the same component was found when comparing the neural processing of avatar faces vs. real faces, as if avatars in general were assimilated as real face representations over time. ERP results also showed that in the case of the self-avatar, the P200 component correlated with more complex conscious encodings of self-representation, i.e., the difference in voltage in the P200 between the self-avatar and the self-picture was reduced in participants that felt the avatar looked like them. This study is put into context within the literature of self-recognition and face recognition in the visual cortex. Additionally, the implications of these results on look-alike avatars are discussed both for future virtual reality (VR) and neuroscience studies.

Introduction

In the context of object observation and recognition, humans are particularly good at recognizing faces compared to other objects. Previous research has shown that the brain processing of faces differs from that of other objects (Axelrod et al., 2014). Several researchers have studied this particular effect using electroencephalography (EEG) (Bentin et al., 1996; Allison et al., 1999; Eimer, 2000; Caldara et al., 2003), MEG (Liu et al., 2002), and fMRI (Vuilleumier et al., 2001). In particular, EEG studies have associated different event related potential (ERP) N170 components in the visual cortex area during the observation of faces vs. other objects (Bentin et al., 1996; Allison et al., 1999; Axelrod et al., 2014). In healthy subjects faces elicit larger N170 potentials in the visual cortex than other non-face objects (e.g., cars, flowers; Bentin et al., 1996; Zion-Golumbic and Bentin, 2007). This suggests that the encoding of object categories can be signaled by the strength of the N170 (Eimer, 2011), showing a significant more negative voltage for faces just 170 ms after the stimulus onset. The expertise of participants with respect to the presented object seems to affect the ERPs: after a period of observational training of non-face objects the responses of N170 significantly decreased (20%) when compared to subjects who were untrained for those objects (Gauthier et al., 2003; Rossion et al., 2004). A similar training effect has been found for face processing in younger children who do not have the expertise of an adult; children of different ages show different brain potentials during face observation tasks (Taylor et al., 2004). However, the N170 component does not show evidence of face identification, or face memory access (Bentin and Deouell, 2000; Eimer, 2000; Tanaka et al., 2006). According to the literature, memory-related face processing first occurs 200 ms after the stimulus onset (Schweinberger et al., 2004; Kaufmann et al., 2009). Applying transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in the same occipito-parietal part of the brain produced an impairment in face discrimination tasks on healthy participants (Rossion et al., 2003), similar to the deficiencies showed by patients with prosopagnosia who cannot recognize and identify faces (Pitcher et al., 2008, 2011).

In fact, these later brain mechanisms seem to play an important role not only in the distinction between faces and other objects, but also with respect to the evaluation of the emotional state of the face (Vuilleumier et al., 2001; Eimer and Holmes, 2007), and in the classification of faces with different degrees of familiarity (self, familiar, unfamiliar; Bentin and Deouell, 2000; Sui et al., 2006; Platek and Kemp, 2009; Keyes et al., 2010; Ramasubbu et al., 2011). When categorizing faces of different ages, races and genders, a larger P200 is found to outgroups (Ito and Urland, 2003; Ito and Bartholow, 2009; Tanaka and Pierce, 2009; Wiese, 2012); a positive P200 followed by negative N250 deflection has also been related to higher levels of familiarity of the face when observing self, familiar and unfamiliar faces (Tanaka et al., 2006; Kaufmann and Schweinberger, 2008; Wiese, 2012). In these components, self faces show significantly reduced voltage when compared to other people’s faces. Therefore, these mechanisms are sensitive to the owner of the face such that images of our own face are processed differently than the faces of others, i.e., familiarity of the face impacts its processing (Devue and Brédart, 2011). Thus, P200 has been proposed as the earliest component that indexes the stored face representations from the long-term memory, able of self-recognition (Pfütze et al., 2002). The aim of the present study was to investigate further these face recognition traces (Caharel et al., 2002; Tanaka et al., 2006; Kaufmann et al., 2009). In this context previous research with ERPs has found that caricaturing familiar and unfamiliar faces with distortions of up to 30% did not affect the processing of the familiar faces whereas the unfamiliar faces elicited significant effects (Kaufmann and Schweinberger, 2008). Furthermore, in that research the familiarity differences (familiar vs. unfamiliar) remained prominent both in veridical and caricature faces (Kaufmann and Schweinberger, 2008). Additional studies have shown tolerance in the processing of familiar faces even when they are distorted (Sandford and Burton, 2014).

In this article, we exploit previous research on self, familiar and unfamiliar faces to compare the ERP brain processing dynamics when participants are exposed to look-alike avatar faces vs. other avatars. Avatars are widely used in virtual reality (VR) to represent participants and other actors, and multiple behavioral studies have shown that participants responses can vary depending on the external appearance of the avatars (Peck et al., 2013), and whether the avatars looked like the participant (Streuber et al., 2009; Osimo et al., 2015). In this context, we aim to examine the extent to which avatar faces are processed as real faces, and also whether self-recognition on physically similar avatars may elicit measurable neurophysiological effects that relate to the actual subjective experience.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Seventeen neurologically healthy male subjects between the ages of 25 and 41 (M = 33, SD = 4.8), with normal or corrected vision participated in the experiment. Only males were used because of limitations on the pipeline to generate look-alike avatars. Participants were recruited via email from the laboratory mailing list. Since we wanted to study the effects of self-specific processing and familiarity, participants were matched with workmates and friends seen every day by the participants for the stimuli generation. Participants had never been exposed to the specific stimuli used in this experiment at the moment of recruiting and had not participated before on face observation studies. Subjects gave informed consent according to the Declaration of Helsinki, and the experiment was approved by the ethics committee of the Universitat de Barcelona.

Look-Alike Avatars

We took three photographs (front, left and right profile) of each participant in order to create their look-alike avatars. In all cases participants were requested to maintain a neutral facial expression to avoid emotion processing during the observation, since it has been shown that emotions on faces generate different brain traces and activate mirror neurons (Likowski et al., 2012). Avatars were generated in a matter of minutes using the fast creation of look-alike avatars pipeline described in Blom et al. (2014). The resultant avatars were tweaked manually to make minor smoothing and lighting improvements. The avatars were displayed without hair, focusing the experiment on the participant’s pure facial characteristics; none of the participants was actually bald. We used the original frontal picture and a frontal capture of the resulting avatar for the observation task (Figure 1A).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Creation of the look-alike avatar, the three pictures used for the avatar generation on the top. On the bottom, the final pictures used for the experiment as real and virtual. (B) Experimental execution. The six faces (self-real, self-virtual, familiar-real, familiar-virtual, unfamiliar-real, unfamiliar-virtual) were randomly ordered in blocks of 10. Each face was displayed for 300 ms, followed by a variable time of 740–1340 ms in which a fixation cross appeared. After each block there was a short resting period of 2 s for blinking.

The real and the computer generated images were processed to equalize pupil-pupil distance across all, and to ensure that the vertical midline of the image bisected the face; this involved a minor scaling of the entire image, and left the normal proportions of the face invariant. The images were saved at 520 × 520 pixels, in color and with averaged luminance to avoid effects of uncontrolled interstimulus perceptual variance that have been found to influence the N170 component (Thierry et al., 2007). Controlling this particular variance seems to correct the N170 face sensitivity (Bentin et al., 2007).

Stimuli

The experimental design included two factors: Virtuality and Familiarity. The Virtuality factor consisted of either a photographic image of a real person (Real) or an image of the look-alike avatar (Avatar) of that person. The Familiarity factor consisted of the three levels: self (Self), familiar (Familiar) or unfamiliar other (Unfamiliar). The person used for the Unfamiliar condition had no previous relation to any of the participants. Since we wanted to study the effects of self-specific processing rather than habituation to the face—e.g., participants see themselves in mirrors very frequently—the Familiar faces were extracted from friends seen every day by the participants.

In total, subjects were exposed to the six images, 200 times each, in a classical ERP setup resulting in a total of 1200 faces to be observed by each participant during the experiment that lasted approximately 30 min. Habituation to the stimuli was explored to test possible adaptation effects. The Familiarity factor was based on the existing literature on ERPs and self-face processing (Sui et al., 2006; Tanaka et al., 2006; Keyes et al., 2010).

The visual stimuli were implemented and displayed using the XVR programming system (Tecchia et al., 2010; Spanlang et al., 2014) on an Intel Core i7 at screen resolution 1920 × 1080 pixels. The six faces (self-real, self-avatar, friend-real, friend-avatar, unfamiliar-real, unfamiliar-avatar) were pseudo-randomly ordered in blocks of 10. Each face was displayed for 300 ms, followed by a variable time of 740–1340 ms in which a fixation cross appeared (Figure 1B). After each block of 10 pictures there was a resting period of 2 s for blinking. Participants were instructed to maintain the focus on the fixation point and to minimize blinks and eye movements.

Questionnaire

Before mounting the EEG cap and starting the experiment we showed the faces one by one to the participants and administered a short questionnaire in which they had to rate the realism of both the real and avatar pictures from 1 (not alike at all) to 5 (looks completely like the real person). Specifically they were asked to answer whether: “The picture looks like the real person”. The realism of the faces when compared to the real person were not available for the unfamiliar case as participants did not know the real person. Instead they were asked to compare the similarity of the avatar face to the photographic picture face (from 1 to 5): “The avatar face looks like the real face”. This similarity rating was performed for all pairs of real-avatar pictures. In order to detect changes in the scoring that were due to the experimental exposure or adaptation effects, participants were asked to rate again all the faces and avatars after the experiment.

EEG Recording

Continuous EEG was acquired from 64 active electrodes located at standard 10–20 positions with a g.HIamp multichannel amplifier manufactured by g.Tec Medical Engineering. Active ring electrodes (g.LADYbird) were used in a standardized cap (g.GAMMAcap), both from g.Tec. The activity was referenced to the earlobe and the ground electrode was located in the frontal area of the head. Signals where digitalized at 256 Hz frequency rate, a notch Butterworth filter 4th order from 48 to 52 Hz was used to eliminate the AC. Ocular movements were detected from FP1, FP2, AF7 and AF8.

The EEG was segmented offline into 1200 ms epochs starting 200 ms before the stimulus onset. Trials where the faces were consecutively repeated were rejected off-line. There was a repetition rate of 2.99%. Trials in which ocular movements were found (EOG greater than 50 μV) or the absolute amplitude of the signal at any electrode was greater than 150 μV were rejected. The average acceptance rate was of 75 ± 15% trials per participant.

ERPs time-locked to the onset of the stimuli for each condition and participant were averaged for epochs of −200 to 900 ms with the baseline set from −150 to 0 ms. Mean amplitudes were calculated where there were discernible peaks in the average ERP waveform for each of the 17 participants; these included the epochs N170 (160–220 ms) and P200 (250–300 ms) of the right and left occipito-parietal cortex, which are consistent with those proposed in previous self-recognition experiments (Sui et al., 2006; Tanaka et al., 2006; Keyes et al., 2010).

The original ERPs for each subject were also transformed into reference-free Current Source Density (CSD) estimates (μV/cm2 units) using a spherical spline surface Laplacian Matlab-based CSD toolbox (Kayser and Tenke, 2006) to better show the topographical maps.

Results

Evaluation of Faces

Participants rated of the realism of the faces by responding to the statement: “The face looks like the real person”; and the similarity between the avatar face and the photograph by responding to: “The avatar face looks like the face in the photograph” (Figure 2). After the experiment, participants were asked to rate again all the faces. There were no significant differences between any of the real or virtual faces in the pre-post scoring for the realism question (Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, all p > 0.45, n = 17), nor for the similarity pre-post (Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, all p > 0.55, n = 17). The real image scores served as a consistency control: none of the real pictures scored less than 4 out of the maximum of 5. Furthermore, there were no significant differences amongst the scores for all the avatar faces, which indicates that all of them were perceived similarly realistically (Friedman test, p > 0.37, n = 17).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Boxplots of the scores for the different faces, from the pre and post questionnaires. The scores range from 1 (not alike at all) to 5 (totally looks alike). Participants rated the faces by (A) Realism: they compared the pictures to the real person that they knew; (B) Similarity: they compared the real picture to the picture of the avatar face independently of whether they knew not the person in the picture. The thick horizontal bars are the medians and the boxes are the interquartile ranges. The whiskers range from max (lower quartile − 1.5 * IQR, smallest value) to min (upper quartile + 1.5 * IQR, largest value). Values outside of this range are marked by *.

There were significant differences when comparing the realism scores between the avatar and the real faces (Wilcoxon signed rank tests, all p < 0.001). This indicates that even though all the participants did recognize themselves and their confederates in the avatar faces, the avatars were not perceived as overly realistic as the real faces (Figure 2A). To the similarity question all avatars (self, familiar and unfamiliar) were rated as equally similar to their real counterparts (Figure 2B).

EEG Analysis

In order to explore how the faces were processed in the brain we proceeded to study well-defined visual processing-related ERP components in occipito-parietal cortex, such as the N170 and the P200 components (Bentin and Deouell, 2000; Caharel et al., 2002; Sui et al., 2006; Tanaka et al., 2006; Keyes et al., 2010). Visual inspection of Figure 3 shows a clear negative voltage for all the faces during the N170 (both for avatar and real faces); and a decreased voltage during the P200 and the N250 for the self-faces, both in the real and in the avatar conditions, as a result of familiarity processing (Tanaka et al., 2006).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Grand average event related potentials (ERPs) for the 17 subjects of the parietal electrodes (PO7, P7) and (PO8, P8) elicited during the first 100 trials by (A) Real faces: self, familiar, and other. (B) Virtual faces: self, familiar, and unfamiliar. The current source density (CSD) topographical plots show how the difference between others-self for the P200 component is mainly located in the occipito-parietal cortex. A low pass filter (15 Hz, half-amplitude cut-off) was applied in these grand averaged graphs.

The amplitude of each component was extracted for each participant in the specified time-windows (N170: 160–220 ms; P200: 250–300 ms) and analyzed via a repeated measures ANOVA with three within-subject factors: Hemisphere (left (PO7, P7), right (PO8, P8)) × Familiarity (self, familiar, other) × Virtuality (real, avatar). Importantly, only the first 100 trials of each condition are taken into account for this section to avoid the effects of adaptation (see below for the analysis of fast adaptation). The Mauchly test was run to account for significant differences on the variances. In cases where the test was positive (<0.05) we applied and noted the corresponding correction for sphericity.

N170

No significant main effects were found for the N170; there was a non-significant trend for Familiarity (F(2,32) = 2.916, p = 0.069) and an interaction between Hemisphere × Virtuality (F(1,16) = 5.076, p = 0.039). In a post hoc analysis only of the virtual faces, a trend was found for Familiarity only in the left hemisphere (F(2,32) = 2.552, p < 0.094), however no significant effects were found on the real faces. No differences were found between the real and the virtual faces. Meaning that all faces, both virtual and real, were indeed classified as the same category in the N170 component. This is in agreement with the literature that has reported the N170 to be able to differentiate object categories, being so that observation of faces produces a more negative voltage than observation of other objects (Bentin et al., 1996; Zion-Golumbic and Bentin, 2007).

P200

The same repeated measures ANOVA was run on the P200 component. This analysis showed a significant main effect of Familiarity (F(2,32) = 7.253, p < 0.003; Figure 3). Post hoc pairwise comparisons pooling together the amplitude in all occipito-parietal electrode locations (PO7, P7, PO8, P8) for both hemispheres showed significant differences between self (2.681 ± SE 0.319 μv) vs. familiar (3.147 ± SE 0.298 μv; t = 2.712, p = 0.015, df = 16) and self vs. unfamiliar (3.277 ± SE 0.298 μv; t = 3.403, p = 0.004, df = 16) but not between familiar vs. unfamiliar (t = 0.9, p = 0.381, df = 16), indicating the existence of a self-oriented encoding process (Figure 3).

No interactions were found between Familiarity ×Virtuality, and in fact, both the avatar and the real faces did show similar Familiarity processing. More precisely when analyzing separately the avatar from the real faces we find that: the self-avatar (2.453 ± SE 0.293 μv) was processed with a significantly reduced voltage than the familiar-avatar (2.844 ± SE 0.265 μv) and the unfamiliar-avatar (2.953 ± SE 0.282 μv; t = 3.984, p < 0.033, df = 16). Similar effects were also found for the self-real (2.147 ± SE 0.318 μv) vs. the familiar-real (2.688 ± SE 0.306 μv) and vs. the unfamiliar-real (2.727 ± SE 0.343 μv; t = 2.407, p < 0.05, df = 16). In contrast, the pairwise comparison familiar vs. unfamiliar was not significant for any cases (t < 0.8, p > 0.3, df = 16), which can be explained if the neural mechanisms related to the P200 component were oriented mainly towards self-recognition and not so much towards the recognition of others’ faces (Figure 4). These effects seem to be stronger in the right hemisphere than the left hemisphere, although no interaction was found between Familiarity × Hemisphere visual inspection (Figure 3) seems to shows stronger effects on the right hemisphere.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Grand average ERPs of the 17 subjects in the parietal electrodes (PO7, P7) and (PO8, P8) elicited by the real and the computer generated faces for the first 100 trials. There is a significant difference in the P200, mainly in the parietal left electrodes. A low pass filter (15 Hz, half-amplitude cut-off) was applied in these grand averaged graphs.

Interestingly, regarding the nature of the face presented, we find a significant within subjects main effect for Virtuality (F(1,16) = 7.946, p = 0.012) as well as an interaction between Hemisphere × Virtuality (F(1,16) = 4.335, p = 0.05). Avatar faces (3.212 ± SE 0.249 μv) elicited larger amplitudes when compared to real faces (2.816 ± SE 0.360 μv) in the left hemisphere (t = 4.430, p < 0.001, df = 16). The same effect was not found in the right hemisphere locations (t = 0.722, p = 0.480, df = 16). This clear difference between virtual and real faces in the P200 component is depicted in Figure 4.

When observing the particular case of self-real vs. self-avatar we found that in the left hemisphere the processing was significantly different for both types of faces (t = 2.987, p = 0.009, df = 16, self-avatar = 2.56 ± SE 0.35 μv, self-real = 2.11 ± SE 0.35 μv), while the same did not occur in the right hemisphere (t = 0.763, p = 0.457, df = 16, self-avatar = 2.33 ± SE 0.28 μv, self-real = 2.18 ± SE 0.33 μv). These results suggest that while a self-recognition effect is mostly present in the right hemisphere where the self-avatar was classified in a similar way to the self-real, a Virtuality effect is found on the left hemisphere as if the avatar-faces were being dissociated from the real-faces.

Overall these results suggest that the amplitude of the P200 component is sensitive to the nature of the face, whether it is a computer generated avatar or a real face, as well as the familiarity level of the face, and more precisely the P200 seems to be oriented towards self-recognition in faces. In agreement with previous findings (Bentin et al., 1996; Zion-Golumbic and Bentin, 2007), this component might be related to the neural processes involved in distinguishing self from other faces. This particular effect of self-identification and familiarity seems to work for both the avatar and for the real faces, indicating that avatars that look alike the self are recognized as the self to a higher degree than the other avatars. Despite there is an evident visual difference between the computer generated avatars in this experiment and the real pictures, the fact that non-significant differences are found in ERPs traces is in agreement with previous findings showing large tolerance to distortions in familiar faces during self-recognition (Kaufmann and Schweinberger, 2008; Sandford and Burton, 2014).

Rapid Adaptation Effects

In the previous section, we found the P200 component to be sensitive to both Virtuality and Familiarity. In this section, we investigate further the modulation of these effects over time. In Figure 5 we can observe the evolution of the amplitude of the P200 component for the Virtuality parameter across the whole experiment. The amplitude of the P200 component was computed in bins of 30 trials for both avatar and real faces (pooling together all the conditions) in the left hemisphere, where the effect of Virtuality was stronger (see previous section). An important observation in this Figure 5 (top panel) is the clear tendency of the voltage amplitude of the P200 component to progressively reduce over time in the avatar condition, tending to merge with the real face condition. Figure 5 (lower panel) also shows the grand average ERP for the first 50 and last 50 trials of each condition showing a clear reduction of the P200 amplitude differences in the last trials for both types of real and avatar faces.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. The top panel shows the time evolution of the P200 amplitude in the left tempo-parietal cortex (P07, P7); presenting the cumulative voltage over blocks of 30 trials (the error bars show the standard error of the different participants ERPs). We observe how in the first trials the virtual and the real faces are processed as different objects; however, this effect is reduced after the overexposure. In the panel below we observe the grand averaged ERPs of the first and last 50 trials, and a clear reduction of the P200 component is also observed. The topographical plots show the CSD of the P200 component difference (avatar-real) in the scalp, we can see how the difference decreases over the last trials. A low pass filter (15 Hz, half-amplitude cut-off) was applied to these grand averaged graphs.

In order to analyze these effects, we conducted a repeated measures ANOVA with factors Time (bins, 1–6) × Virtuality (real, avatar) focused on occipito-parietal left-hemisphere electrodes (PO7, P7) where the effect was observed to be larger (Figure 4). This analysis showed a significant main effect on Virtuality (F(1,16) = 18.407, p = 0.001). Importantly and coherently with Figure 5 (top), a significant time interaction effect was found (Time × Virtuality (F(5,80) = 2.865, p = 0.02), indicating a decay of the P200 amplitude over time for the avatar faces. Further pairwise comparisons showed that while a significant Virtuality effect was present in the first 50 trials (t = 4.037, p = 0.001, df = 16), no significant effects were observed in the last 50 trials (t = 1.169, p = 0.26, df = 16).

These findings suggest that the avatar faces are initially processed as different from the real faces, as if they were not faces but another kind of object, which is analogous to what happens with exposure to faces vs. flowers or other objects (Bentin et al., 1996; Allison et al., 1999; Keyes et al., 2010). Nevertheless, this effect is reduced over time, and both real and avatar faces eventually converge to be classified as the same class of objects: faces.

A similar analysis was run to study Familiarity effects over time at the P200 component (combining both right and left hemisphere). For this purpose, bins of 30 trials were used in the repeated measures ANOVA with factors Time (bins, 1–6) × Familiarity (self, familiar, other). A significant main effect was found for Familiarity (F(2,32) = 5.308, p = 0.010); however, no interaction effects were observed for Time × Familiarity on the P200 component (F(10,160) < 1). These results suggest that the effect of familiarity did not significantly decay over time in the present design. The analysis was also run with the avatar faces alone and there was no interaction of Time. This indicated that the self-recognition mechanisms are still functioning after longer exposures and both the self-avatar and the self-real faces are processed as belonging to one familiarity level, which is different to the other faces.

Self-Identification Subjective Scores Match the Neurphysiological Responses

In the previous sections we have shown that the P200 amplitude was dependent on the Familiarity factor, and more precisely to the self-recognition in both real and avatar faces. In this section, we explore whether neurophysiological voltage could be related to higher cognitive processes such as the subjective self-identification score.

Since different participants may have different amplitudes in their components, we normalized the amplitude by calculating the voltage difference from the self-avatar to the self-real face for each participant (self-avatar—self-real) pooling together all the occipito-parietal electrodes in both hemispheres (PO7, PO8, P7, P8). A zero value coming out from that difference would indicate an equal voltage for the real picture and for the avatar, thus a greater self-identification, while more positive values would mean a lesser self-identification with the avatars, since in general a more reduced voltage in the P200 is associated with self-identification (Keyes et al., 2010).

The resulting voltage difference was then contrasted to the initial reported score of realism for the self-avatar face. In that question participants rated whether the avatar looked like themselves. A significant Spearman Correlation was found between the difference voltage (self-avatar—self-real) and the subjective score (r = −0.47, n = 17, p = 0.045; Figure 6).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Correlation between the virtual self-face identification scores in the realism question and the P200 voltage difference between self-avatar–self-real. The closer the voltage difference is to zero the greater the identification.

Discussion

Avatars have been used successfully as feasible substitutes of the body to study different aspects of neuroscience and psychology (Banakou et al., 2013; Banakou and Slater, 2014; González-Franco et al., 2014; Padrao et al., 2016). Some studies have found that behavioral responses may be modulated when such avatars look like the participants, indicating that there are some self-identification effects related to embodiment (Aymerich-Franch et al., 2012; Fox et al., 2012; Osimo et al., 2015). The current study presents novel neural evidence related to the process of self-identification in avatars. Differences in ERPs were prominent for the Familiarity levels of both real and avatar faces, more in particular a significant reduced voltage in the P200 component at the occipito-parietal locations was found both for the self-avatar and the self-real face conditions, showing that the look-alike avatars were self-recognized to a higher extent than other avatars. This P200 voltage response has been previously described in other self-recognition experiments (Caharel et al., 2002; Tanaka et al., 2006; Keyes et al., 2010). Despite the evident visual difference between the computer generated avatars in this experiment and the real faces (Figure 1), the fact that non-significant differences are found in the P200 traces for the self-avatar vs. the self-real is in agreement with previous findings: caricaturing familiar faces with distortions of up to 30% did not affect the neural processing of P200 or N250 (Kaufmann and Schweinberger, 2008), nor did the observation of low quality CCTV images (Bruce et al., 1999). Furthermore, in Kaufmann and Schweinberger (2008) the familiarity differences (familiar vs. unfamiliar) remained prominent both in veridical and caricature faces, which is also coherent with our results that find the P200 effects of self-recognition to be prominent both in real as well as in avatar faces. Our results therefore also contributed to confirm the tolerance in the processing of self-faces even when they are distorted (Sandford and Burton, 2014).

In our experiment a significant correlation was found between the self-identification scoring and the P200 component related with self-recognition mechanisms: the more the participants scored the self-avatar face to be like the real face, the smaller the difference in voltage between the self-avatar and self-real faces. Hence, the self-avatar face seems to have been processed at a low level (ERPs) in congruence with higher-cognitive functions (subjective questionnaire). The correlation between the subjective scoring and the self-identification mechanism of the P200 suggests that, either the conscious self-reporting is somehow influenced by the underlying visual processing of the faces, or the other way around. Anyhow given that both higher and lower cognitive functions are aligned, this correlation can be interpreted also as a validation of the P200 being indeed related to self-recognition. Thus providing yet more evidence to existing studies by other authors (Caharel et al., 2002; Tanaka et al., 2006; Kaufmann and Schweinberger, 2008; Keyes et al., 2010).

Besides, our results showed that all faces, both avatars and real faces, were indeed classified as the same object category in the N170 component where no significant voltage differences were found. The N170 component has been widely regarded as a neural trace of object categorization processing, being so that observation of faces produces a more negative voltage than observation of other objects such as flowers or cars (Bentin et al., 1996; Zion-Golumbic and Bentin, 2007). In our experiment both real and avatar faces did not exhibit strong differences in the N170 component. However, the amplitude of the P200 component was sensitive to the nature of the face for the earliest trials, i.e., whether the face was computer generated or real. Indeed, we found evidence in the left ocipito-parietal locations that the Virtuality of the faces affected the voltage of the P200 component only in the first trials of the experiment, but not in later trials where the effect disappears and both avatar and real faces are processed the same (Figure 5). This result seems to indicate that both computer generated and real faces elicited equivalent ERP components after a fast adaptation process. Visual expertise effect has been described before during observation of faces with different familiarity and it has been related the P200 amplitude to the activation of pre-existing and acquired face representations (Tanaka et al., 2006), similar effects have also been associated to memory related face observation studies (Ito and Urland, 2003; Schweinberger et al., 2004; Ito and Bartholow, 2009). Furthermore, studies with children of different ages also showed how expertise influences face processing (Pfütze et al., 2002; Taylor et al., 2004). We suggest that in the present study participants underwent a similar adaptation: since they were not previously exposed to the avatar faces they required a certain amount of visual exposure (repetition) to gain visual expertise on these new faces.

Overall, the present study contributes towards expanding the frontiers of self-recognition and face processing. More precisely, we provide further evidence of the tolerance of the brain in regards to self-recognition and external appearance distortions, as well as of the link between higher level functions and the internal low level processing of faces during self-recognition. Future research on this area could help explore the link between the neurophysiological signatures and the subjective rejection of faces in complex scenarios or contexts, such as the uncanny valley effect (Seyama and Nagayama, 2007), where very realistic computer generated faces produce a drastic emotional rejection by the observers (Cheetham et al., 2011).

We provide objective evidence that avatar faces are interpreted just as any other face and that look-alike avatars can be recognized as the self to a greater extent than other avatars, and thus be processed similarly to the real self. In general, this finding has important implications for future and existing research as it validates previous studies that have shown a change in the participant’s behavior when avatars bear external physical similarities to themselves (Streuber et al., 2009; Fox et al., 2012; Osimo et al., 2015). With ERPs not only can we study the effects of look-alike computer generated avatars, but also track on-line adaptive changes without requesting participants to continuously provide behavioral judgments. Future research in this field could explore whether the neurophysiological response generated over a random avatar face can be modulated by a virtual body ownership experience, i.e., if after being embodied in an avatar, its face is perceived as closer to the self-face than before. In this way the methodology presented may also provide a new objective measure to evaluate the appearance effects across time on people whose body has been substituted in VR. This could be a useful tool to explore “other-race-effects” of ERPs (Balas and Nelson, 2010) and racial bias modulation after exposed to other race avatars (Peck et al., 2013).

Significance Statement

This article presents novel findings in the field of self-recognition. It shows evidence of face-memory accessing for avatar faces that corresponds to self-recognition in computer generated look-alike avatars. Furthermore, the neural traces for self-recognition are correlated to the subjective feeling of the avatar looking like the participant. These findings predict that avatars that look like the participants will have stronger self-representation impact than arbitrarily chosen avatars.

Author Contributions

MG-F and KJB conceived and designed the work; MG-F and AIB did the implementation and run the experiments. MG-F and AR-F did the analysis, and interpretation of data. MG-F, MS and AR-F wrote the article.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This study was funded by the European Union Seventh Framework Programme (FP7) Integrated Project VERE (No. 257695). MGF’s research was supported by the FI-DGR pre-doctorate grant. The European Research Council (ERC) project TRAVERSE (#227985). The authors would like to thank Veronica Orvalho, Xenxo Alvarez Blanco, Pedro Mendes, Catarina Runa and Ivan Silva for the development of components of the avatar pipeline.

References

Allison, T., Puce, A., Spencer, D. D., and McCarthy, G. (1999). Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. I: potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex by face and non-face stimuli. Cereb. Cortex 9, 415–430. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.5.415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axelrod, V., Bar, M., and Rees, G. (2014). Exploring the unconscious using faces. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 35–45. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aymerich-Franch, L., Karutz, C., and Bailenson, J. N. (2012). “Effects of facial and voice similarity on presence in a public speaking virtual environment,” in Proceeding of the International Society for Presence Reseach Annual Conference (Philadepelphia, PA), 24–26.

Google Scholar

Balas, B., and Nelson, C. A. (2010). The role of face shape and pigmentation in other-race face perception: an electrophysiological study. Neuropsychologia 48, 498–506. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banakou, D., Groten, R., and Slater, M. (2013). Illusory ownership of a virtual child body causes overestimation of object sizes and implicit attitude changes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 110, 12846–12851. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1306779110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banakou, D., and Slater, M. (2014). Body ownership causes illusory self-attribution of speaking and influences subsequent real speaking. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 111, 17678–17683. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414936111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, E., and McCarthy, G. (1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 8, 551–565. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentin, S., and Deouell, L. Y. (2000). Structural encoding and identification in face processing: erp evidence for separate mechanisms. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 17, 35–55. doi: 10.1080/026432900380472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentin, S., Taylor, M. J., Rousselet, G. A., Itier, R. J., Caldara, R., Schyns, P. G., et al. (2007). Controlling interstimulus perceptual variance does not abolish N170 face sensitivity. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 801–802; author reply 802–803. doi: 10.1038/nn0707-801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blom, K. J., Bellido Rivas, A. I., Alvarez, X., Cetinaslan, O., Oliveira, B., Orvalho, V., et al. (2014). Achieving participant acceptance of their avatars. Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ. 23, 287–299. doi: 10.1162/pres_a_00194

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruce, V., Henderson, Z., Greenwood, K., Hancock, P. J. B., Burton, A. M., and Miller, P. (1999). Verification of face identities from images captured on video. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 5, 339–360. doi: 10.1037/1076-898x.5.4.339

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caharel, S., Poiroux, S., Bernard, C., Thibaut, F., Lalonde, R., and Rebai, M. (2002). ERPs associated with familiarity and degree of familiarity during face recognition. Int. J. Neurosci. 112, 1499–1512. doi: 10.1080/00207450290158368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldara, R., Thut, G., Servoir, P., Michel, C., Bovet, P., and Renault, B. (2003). Face versus non-face object perception and the “other-race” effect: a spatio-temporal event-related potential study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 515–528. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(02)00407-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheetham, M., Suter, P., and Jäncke, L. (2011). The human likeness dimension of the “uncanny valley hypothesis”: behavioral and functional MRI findings. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:126. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Devue, C., and Brédart, S. (2011). The neural correlates of visual self-recognition. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 40–51. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimer, M. (2000). Event-related brain potentials distinguish processing stages involved in face perception and recognition. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 694–705. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00285-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimer, M. (2011). “The face-sensitive N170 component of the event-related brain potential,” in The Oxford Handbook of Face Perception, eds G. Rhodes, A. Calder, M. Johnson, and J. V. Haxby (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 329–344.

Google Scholar

Eimer, M., and Holmes, A. (2007). Event-related brain potential correlates of emotional face processing. Neuropsychologia 45, 15–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, J., Bailenson, J., and Ricciardi, T. (2012). Physiological responses to virtual selves and virtual others. J. Cyber. Therapy Rehabil. 5, 69–72.

Gauthier, I., Curran, T., Curby, K. M., and Collins, D. (2003). Perceptual interference supports a non-modular account of face processing. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 428–432. doi: 10.1038/nn1029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

González-Franco, M., Peck, T. C., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., and Slater, M. (2014). A threat to a virtual hand elicits motor cortex activation. Exp. Brain Res. 232, 875–887. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3800-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, T. A., and Bartholow, B. D. (2009). The neural correlates of race. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 524–531. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ito, T. A., and Urland, G. R. (2003). Race and gender on the brain: electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of multiply categorizable individuals. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 616–626. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufmann, J. M., and Schweinberger, S. R. (2008). Distortions in the brain? ERP effects of caricaturing familiar and unfamiliar faces. Brain Res. 1228, 177–188. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.06.092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufmann, J. M., Schweinberger, S. R., and Burton, A. M. (2009). N250 ERP correlates of the acquisition of face representations across different images. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 625–641. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kayser, J., and Tenke, C. E. (2006). Principal components analysis of Laplacian waveforms as a generic method for identifying ERP generator patterns: I. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 348–368. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.08.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keyes, H., Brady, N., Reilly, R. B., and Foxe, J. J. (2010). My face or yours? Event-related potential correlates of self-face processing. Brain Cogn. 72, 244–254. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Likowski, K. U., Mühlberger, A., Gerdes, A. B. M., Wieser, M. J., Pauli, P., and Weyers, P. (2012). Facial mimicry and the mirror neuron system: simultaneous acquisition of facial electromyography and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:214. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Harris, A., and Kanwisher, N. (2002). Stages of processing in face perception: an MEG study. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 910–916. doi: 10.1038/nn909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osimo, S. A., Pizarro, R., Spanlang, B., and Slater, M. (2015). Conversations between self and self as Sigmund Freud–A virtual body ownership paradigm for self counselling. Sci. Rep. 5:13899. doi: 10.1038/srep13899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padrao, G., Gonzalez-Franco, M., Sanchez-Vives, M. V., Slater, M., and Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2016). Violating body movement semantics: neural signatures of self-generated and external-generated errors. Neuroimage 124, 147–156. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peck, T. C., Seinfeld, S., Aglioti, S. M., and Slater, M. (2013). Putting yourself in the skin of a black avatar reduces implicit racial bias. Conscious. Cogn. 22, 779–787. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfütze, E.-M., Sommer, W., and Schweinberger, S. R. (2002). Age-related slowing in face and name recognition: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Psychol. Aging 17, 140–160. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitcher, D., Garrido, L., Walsh, V., and Duchaine, B. C. (2008). Transcranial magnetic stimulation disrupts the perception and embodiment of facial expressions. J. Neurosci. 28, 8929–8933. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1450-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitcher, D., Walsh, V., and Duchaine, B. (2011). The role of the occipital face area in the cortical face perception network. Exp. Brain Res. 209, 481–493. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2579-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Platek, S. M., and Kemp, S. M. (2009). Is family special to the brain? An event-related fMRI study of familiar, familial and self-face recognition. Neuropsychologia 47, 849–858. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramasubbu, R., Masalovich, S., Gaxiola, I., Peltier, S., Holtzheimer, P. E., Heim, C., et al. (2011). Differential neural activity and connectivity for processing one’s own face: a preliminary report. Psychiatry Res. 194, 130–140. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossion, B., Caldara, R., Seghier, M., Schuller, A.-M., Lazeyras, F., and Mayer, E. (2003). A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary for normal face processing. Brain 126, 2381–2395. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossion, B., Kung, C.-C., and Tarr, M. J. (2004). Visual expertise with nonface objects leads to competition with the early perceptual processing of faces in the human occipitotemporal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 101, 14521–14526. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0405613101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandford, A., and Burton, A. M. (2014). Tolerance for distorted faces: challenges to a configural processing account of familiar face recognition. Cognition 132, 262–268. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schweinberger, S. R., Huddy, V., and Burton, A. M. (2004). N250r: a face-selective brain response to stimulus repetitions. Neuroreport 15, 1501–1505. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000131675.00319.42

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seyama, J., and Nagayama, R. S. (2007). The uncanny valley: effect of realism on the impression of artificial human faces. Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ. 16, 337–351. doi: 10.1162/pres.16.4.337

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spanlang, B., Normand, J.-M., Borland, D., Kilteni, K., Giannopoulos, E., Pomes, A., et al. (2014). How to build an embodiment lab: achieving body representation illusions in virtual reality. Front. Robot. AI 1:9. doi: 10.3389/frobt.2014.00009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Streuber, S., De La Rosa, S., Trutoiu, L. C., Bülthoff, H. H., and Mohler, B. (2009). “Does brief exposure to a self-avatar affect common human behaviors in immersive virtual environments?” in Eurographics 2009: Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the European Association for Computer Graphics (Munich, Germany), 33–36.

Google Scholar

Sui, J., Zhu, Y., and Han, S. (2006). Self-face recognition in attended and unattended conditions: an event-related brain potential study. Neuroreport 17, 423–427. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000203357.65190.61

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, J. W., Curran, T., Porterfield, A. L., and Collins, D. (2006). Activation of preexisting and acquired face representations: the N250 event-related potential as an index of face familiarity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1488–1497. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.9.1488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, J. W., and Pierce, L. J. (2009). The neural plasticity of other-race face recognition. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 122–131. doi: 10.3758/CABN.9.1.122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, M. J., Batty, M., and Itier, R. J. (2004). The faces of development: a review of early face processing over childhood. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1426–1442. doi: 10.1162/0898929042304732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tecchia, F., Carrozzino, M., Bacinelli, S., Rossi, F., Vercelli, D., Marino, G., et al. (2010). A flexible framework for wide–spectrum VR development. Presence Teleoperators Virtual Environ. 19, 302–312. doi: 10.1162/pres_a_00002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thierry, G., Martin, C. D., Downing, P., and Pegna, A. J. (2007). Controlling for interstimulus perceptual variance abolishes N170 face selectivity. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 505–511. doi: 10.1038/nn1864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., and Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron 30, 829–841. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00328-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiese, H. (2012). The role of age and ethnic group in face recognition memory: ERP evidence from a combined own-age and own-race bias study. Biol. Psychol. 89, 137–147. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zion-Golumbic, E., and Bentin, S. (2007). Dissociated neural mechanisms for face detection and configural encoding: evidence from N170 and induced gamma-band oscillation effects. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1741–1749. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: self-recognition, face-recognition, visual cortex, event related potentials, memory, avatars

Citation: Gonzalez-Franco M, Bellido AI, Blom KJ, Slater M and Rodriguez-Fornells A (2016) The Neurological Traces of Look-Alike Avatars. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:392. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00392

Received: 19 May 2016; Accepted: 21 July 2016;
Published: 03 August 2016.

Edited by:

Mikhail Lebedev, Duke University, USA

Reviewed by:

Lutz Jäncke, University of Zurich, Switzerland
Isabelle Hupont, UPMC Sorbonne University, France

Copyright © 2016 Gonzalez-Franco, Bellido, Blom, Slater and Rodriguez-Fornells. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mar Gonzalez-Franco, margon@microsoft.com

Download