Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 11 April 2017
Sec. Sensory Neuroscience
Volume 11 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00181

The Lateral Occipito-temporal Cortex Is Involved in the Mental Manipulation of Body Part Imagery

Mitsuru Kikuchi1 Tetsuya Takahashi1 Tetsu Hirosawa2 Yumi Oboshi3 Etsuji Yoshikawa4 Yoshio Minabe1,2 Yasuomi Ouchi3*
  • 1Research Center for Child Mental Development, Graduate School of Medical Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Neurobiology, Graduate School of Medical Science, Kanazawa University, Kanazawa, Japan
  • 3Department of Biofunctional Imaging, Medical Photonics Research Center, Hamamatsu University School of Medicine, Hamamatsu, Japan
  • 4Central Research Laboratory, Hamamatsu Photonics K.K., Hamamatsu, Japan

The lateral occipito-temporal cortex (LOTC), including the extrastriate body area, is known to be involved in the perception of body parts. Although still controversial, recent studies have demonstrated the role of the LOTC in higher-level body-related cognition in humans. This study consisted of two experiments (E1 and E2). The first (E1) was an exploratory experiment to find the neural correlate of the mental manipulation of body part imagery, in which brain cerebral glucose metabolic rates and the performance of mental rotation of the hand were measured in 100 subjects who exhibited a range of symptoms of cognitive decline. In E1, we found that the level of glucose metabolism in the right LOTC was significantly correlated with performance in a task involving mental manipulation of the hand. Next, in E2, we performed a randomized, double-blind, controlled intervention study (clinical trial number: UMIN 000018310) in younger healthy adults to test whether right occipital (corresponding to the right LOTC) anodal stimulation using transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) could enhance the mental manipulation of the hand. In E2, we demonstrated a significant effect of tDCS on the accuracy rate in a task involving mental manipulation of the hand. Although further study is necessary to answer the question of whether these results are specific for the mental manipulation of body parts but not non-body parts, E1 demonstrated a possible role of the LOTC in carrying out the body mental manipulation task in patients with dementia, and E2 suggested the possible effect of tDCS on this task in healthy subjects.

Introduction

Previous neuroimaging studies have identified two brain regions that are more sensitive to visually perceived body parts than to non-human objects in the visual cortex. These regions are known as the extrastriate body area (EBA), which is located in the lateral occipito-temporal cortex (LOTC) (Downing et al., 2001), and the fusiform body area (FBA), which is found ventrally in the fusiform gyrus (Peelen and Downing, 2005; Schwarzlose et al., 2005). Since their identification, the functions of the EBA and FBA in higher-level body-related cognition have been elucidated through correlational evidence in human brain imaging studies (Peelen and Downing, 2007; Taylor et al., 2007; Moro et al., 2008; Ferri et al., 2012b; Kitada et al., 2014). Furthermore, as causal evidence, transcranial magnetic stimulation studies (Peelen and Downing, 2007; Urgesi et al., 2007a,b; Pitcher et al., 2012) and a brain lesion study (Moro et al., 2008) also indicated that the EBA and the FBA are responsible for the representation of body part identification. In addition to the EBA’s role in recognition, the EBA has also been reported to be active when individuals perform movements, prepare self-actions, or perceive the body movements of others (Astafiev et al., 2004; David et al., 2007; Peelen and Downing, 2007; Urgesi et al., 2007a; Ishizu et al., 2009; Orlov et al., 2010; Kuhn et al., 2011; van Nuenen et al., 2012). These findings suggest that the EBA not only receives sensory inputs regarding others’ body information but also represents the human body in a dynamic manner, including kinesthetic feedback for one’s own actions. If the EBA receives kinesthetic information about self-actions, it is easy to speculate that the EBA would also be activated during the manipulation of body imagery.

The mental rotation task is a well-established paradigm to study the cognitive process of mentally rotating objects. The brain network that governs mental rotation has been studied using a variety of stimuli (e.g., hands) (Bonda et al., 1995; Parsons et al., 1995; Kosslyn et al., 1998; Vingerhoets et al., 2002; Wraga et al., 2003; de Lange et al., 2005; Seurinck et al., 2005; de Lange et al., 2006; Creem-Regehr et al., 2007; Corradi-Dell’Acqua et al., 2009; Ferri et al., 2012a; Papeo et al., 2012). Using activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis, a recent study (Tomasino and Gremese, 2015) showed activation by the mental rotation task itself in the bilateral inferior and superior parietal lobule, the precentral gyrus, the inferior frontal gyrus, the middle frontal gyrus, the supplementary motor area, the insula, the inferior and middle occipital gyrus and the cerebellum. They also demonstrated that the mental rotation of bodily parts activates the cerebellum, the middle and inferior occipital and calcarine gyrus, the superior parietal lobule, the bilateral postcentral gyrus, the left postcentral gyrus, the left inferior parietal lobe and the right supramarginal gyrus, the left precentral gyrus and the bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, the left superior frontal gyrus, the right middle frontal gyrus and the medial posterior frontal gyrus in addition to the right insula. Therefore, not only motor areas but also visual areas, including the EBA and FBA, seem to be involved in the mental rotation task.

With the aid of decompensated brains (i.e., elderly subjects mainly consisting of patients with dementia; n = 100), the first part of the present study (E1) intended to examine the brain region that is critical in cognitive decline (i.e., decompensation) in the mental rotation task (Figure 1). From the results of E1, we discovered that lower glucose metabolic ratios in the EBA and the FBA were correlated with lower performance on the mental manipulation of the hand imagery task.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Task paradigms. (A) Choice reaction time (CRT) task. The participants were instructed to select the right or left picture that included a circle (i.e., a target) by pressing a button with their right or left hand as soon as possible after a circle appeared on the screen. (B) During the recognition phase of the simple visual working memory task (WM task), the participants were instructed to select the picture that depicted the same hand as that presented during the acquisition phase (non-flipped images). (C) During the recognition phase of the visual working memory with mental rotation task (RWM task), the participants were instructed to select the picture that depicted the same hand, which was flipped from the palm side (acquisition) to the back side (recognition). Each condition consisted of 10 trials with different hand shape pictures and lasted for a duration of 3 min.

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a non-invasive stimulation method that can induce prolonged excitability changes in the cortex, as shown in several studies in humans (Nitsche and Paulus, 2000, 2011; Nitsche et al., 2003, 2007, 2008; Antal et al., 2004a,b; Labruna et al., 2016). In tDCS, weak currents are applied to the cortex via two electrodes placed on the scalp. In the visual cortex, tDCS modulated the amplitude of visual-evoked potentials in a polarity-dependent way: anodal stimulation increased the amplitude of the visual-evoked potentials, but cathodal stimulation decreased it (Antal et al., 2004a). Based on the results of E1, we hypothesized that active stimulation of the LOTC using tDCS would enhance performance on the mental manipulation of the hand imagery task. In the second experiment (E2), we used the tDCS technique to perform a randomized, double-blind, controlled study to test this hypothesis.

Materials and Methods

Experiment 1: Explorative Brain Imaging Study

Participants

We examined 100 elderly subjects, including 22 healthy elderly subjects (9 men and 13 women; mean age 68.0 ± 7.28 years), 52 patients diagnosed with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (30 men and 22 women; mean age 67.4 ± 8.63 years), 14 patients diagnosed with fronto-temporal dementia (FTD) (6 men and 8 women; mean age 64.3 ± 6.73 years), and 12 patients diagnosed with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (6 men and 6 women; mean age 59.8 ± 6.69 years). Therefore, our subjects consisted of 22 healthy and 78 cognitively impaired (from MCI to dementia) elderly subjects. All patients with dementia and MCI were diagnosed on the basis of an extensive clinical history and physical examinations. The diagnoses of AD, FTD, and MCI were based on the criteria of the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke-Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS/ADRDA) (McKhann et al., 1984), the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV (DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), the Lund and Manchester criteria (Neary et al., 1998), and the Petersen MCI criteria (Petersen, 2004).

All participants underwent brain MRI and [18F] fluoro-deoxyglucose (FDG)-PET scans. The present study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Hamamatsu Medical Center (Heisei 8-1), and written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to enrollment. The methods were carried out in accordance with approved guidelines.

Working Memory Tasks

Three visual tasks were presented on a liquid crystal screen in front of the subjects. As shown in Figure 1, one choice reaction time task (CRT task) and two types of working memory tasks were employed (Kikuchi et al., 2011). In the CRT task, the participants were instructed to respond to a circle (i.e., a target) that appeared on the screen by pressing a button with the hand that was located on the same side as the circle (either their right or their left hand) as soon as possible. One working memory task consisted of a simple visual working memory task (WM task) that involved pictures of various hand shapes (palm side only) and the other working memory task consisted of a visual working memory task with mental rotation (RWM task), which used pictures of various hand shapes (palm and back side).

Inverse Efficiency (IE) Scores for the CRT, WM and RWM Tasks

To control for speed–accuracy trade-offs in the cognitive outcome data, we calculated a value [inverse efficiency (IE) score (Townsend and Ashby, 1983; Ludwig et al., 2011)] by dividing the median response time (RT) by the accuracy rate in each task condition. Because the RT was measured in ms and was divided by a unitless number, the IE score was expressed in ms as well. For instance, an average RT of 1000 ms and a 10% error rate would yield an IE value of 1111 ms (1000/(1–0.1)). A lower IE score indicates better performance.

MRI Scanning

All participants underwent 3-dimensional MRI immediately before the PET measurements. During this process, a static magnet (0.3 T MRP7000AD; Hitachi, Tokyo, Japan) was used in the 3-dimensional mode (Ouchi et al., 2001).

PET Scanning and Image Data Acquisition

The patients underwent a series of PET measurements after completing the battery of neuropsychological tests and the MRI examination. A high-resolution brain PET scanner was used (SHR12000; Hamamatsu Photonics K.K., Hamamatsu, Japan) (Ouchi et al., 1999). After a subject’s head was fixed with a thermoplastic face mask and a 10-min transmission scan was acquired, a static 15-min PET scan was performed 45 min after an injection of a 1.2 MBq/kg dose of [18F] FDG.

Image Data Processing

To evaluate glucose metabolism, a semiquantitative ratio index of [18F]FDG was calculated to obtain the standardized uptake value ratio (SUVR) (Ouchi et al., 2009).

Voxel-Wise Statistical Analysis

SPM8 was used for the voxel-wise analysis (voxel-size; 2 mm × 2 mm × 2 mm resolution). All [18F]FDG-SUVR parametric images were first normalized to the MNI space and smoothed with an 8 mm isotropic Gaussian kernel. Voxel-based correlations were computed between [18F]FDG-SUVR parametric images and IE scores or accuracy rates in the three conditions using a multiple regression model with the statistical threshold set at p = 0.05 (corrected with FWE) for the peak height. These analyses were applied for the data from the healthy elderly subjects (n = 22) and all participants, including the cognitively impaired subjects (n = 100).

Experiment 2: tDCS Study

Study Design and Setting

Based on the results from the first part of the present study, we conducted a second randomized, double-blind, controlled study. The study was registered with the University Hospital Medical Information Network (UMIN) Clinical Trials Registry (number UMIN000018310). Forty healthy men (age range, 20–43 years; all right-handed as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory) performed the same tasks as described in the current PET study while receiving anodal tDCS to either the right occipital cortex covering the LOTC (i.e., the region highlighted in the mental rotation task) or the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) (i.e., the region used for the control condition) in combination with cathodal tDCS of the right DLPFC or the right occipital cortex (the former condition was referred to as occipital anodal/frontal cathodal stimulation and the latter condition was referred to as frontal anodal/occipital cathodal stimulation). The control condition involved sham tDCS. The participants were not taking any medications, had no history of neurological or psychiatric disease, and had normal physical and neurological examinations. All subjects were naive to tDCS. Written informed consent was obtained prior to participation in the study. The Ethics Committee of Kanazawa University Hospital approved the methods and procedures and the methods were carried out in accordance with approved guidelines. The demographic data for all participants are presented in Supplementary Table S1.

Before the test, each subject underwent a practice session to ensure that they understood and were well-trained for the tasks used in the present study (i.e., the CRT, WM, and RWM tasks shown in Figure 1). A direct current was induced by two saline-soaked surface sponge electrodes (35 cm2) and delivered by a battery-driven, constant current stimulator (DC-STIMULATOR Plus, neuroConn GmbH, Germany). The participants were randomly assigned to receive occipital anodal/frontal cathodal (n = 20; active group), frontal anodal/occipital cathodal (n = 10; control group 1), or sham tDCS (n = 10; control group 2). During stimulation in the occipital anodal/frontal cathodal condition, as shown in Figure 2A, the anode electrode was placed over PO8 (international EEG 10/10 system) and the cathode electrode was placed over F4 (international EEG 10/10 system). During stimulation in the frontal anodal/occipital cathodal condition, the polarity was reversed. During active stimulation, the participants received a constant current with an intensity of 2 mA. tDCS was initiated 10 min before the task began and was delivered throughout the duration of the three tasks, which lasted approximately 10 min in total (Figure 2B). During sham stimulation, the electrodes were placed at the same positions used during active stimulation, but the stimulator was turned on for only 30 s. Therefore, the participants may have experienced a tDCS-induced itching sensation at the beginning of the session but received no active current for the remaining stimulation period.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. (A) Stimulation targets (i.e., LOTC and DLPFC] marked on the reconstructed scalp and brain surface of one subject in the active condition. (B) Schematic representation of the experimental design. Each participant began to perform the CRT, WM, and RWM tasks after receiving active or control stimulation for 10 min. The stimulation continued throughout the duration of the three tasks and for an additional 10 min. CRT, choice reaction time. WM, working memory. RWM, working memory with hand mental rotation. LOTC, lateral occipito-temporal cortex. DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

Data Analysis of the tDCS Study

To determine whether anodal stimulation to the right occipital region affected the mental manipulation of body part imagery, we evaluated the performance of the subjects (IE score and accuracy) during the CRT, WM, and RWM tasks. The performances of the subjects in the active group (i.e., occipital anodal/frontal cathodal stimulation) were compared with those of the subjects in the control group (i.e., a mixture of frontal anodal/occipital cathodal stimulation and sham stimulation, n = 20). Based on the results of E1 in the present study, for the RWM task, we hypothesized that the IE score will be lower and the accuracy rate will be higher during stimulation in the occipital anodal/frontal cathodal condition compared with the control condition.

For the IE score of the RWM task, an unpaired t-test (one-sided) was used to compare the two groups (i.e., active vs. control stimulation). For the accuracy rate of the RWM task, a Wilcoxon rank-sum test (one-sided) was used to compare the two groups (i.e., active vs. control stimulation). To avoid the risk of low statistical power caused by the smaller sample size, we added complementary analyses between sub-conditions (i.e., we divided the control condition into two conditions) to test the difference between the active (occipital anodal/frontal cathodal stimulation; n = 20) and the reversed (frontal anodal/occipital cathodal stimulation; n = 10) or the sham conditions (n = 10).

As complementary analyses, a two-way ANOVA was performed (task × tDCS condition) for the IE score of the three tasks. The within-subjects factor was the task effect (CRT vs. WM vs. RWM tasks) and the between-subjects factor was the tDCS effect (active vs. control condition or active vs. 2 control conditions).

The significance level was set at 0.05.

Results

Experiment 1: Explorative Brain Imaging Study

As shown in Supplementary Figure S1 and Table 1, the healthy elderly tended to show higher accuracy, shorter RTs, and lower IE scores compared with the cognitively impaired patients in all tasks. The diversified data of the cognitive and brain metabolic profiles in the 22 healthy and 78 cognitively impaired subjects allowed us to conduct correlation analyses.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Demographic characteristics of all subjects in the PET study.

Regarding the correlation between [18F]FDG-SUVR and the accuracy rates, with the conservative statistical threshold set at p = 0.05 (corrected with FWE), no significant correlation was found for the three tasks (i.e., the CRT task, the WM task and the RWM task) in healthy subjects (n = 22) or in all participants, including the cognitively impaired subjects (n = 100). At the risk of type I error, if we employed a statistical threshold set at p = 0.001 (uncorrected) for all subjects (n = 100), a decrease in the [18F]FDG-SUVR in the right frontal areas was associated with a lower accuracy rate in the WM task (Figure 3A) and a decrease in the [18F]FDG-SUVR in the bilateral occipital and parietal areas was associated with a lower accuracy rate on the RWM task (Figure 4A).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. SPM analyses with multiple regression models in which the accuracy rate (A) or the inverse efficiency (IE) scores (B–D) for the WM conditions were used as independent variables. (A) A decrease in the [18F]FDG-SUVR in the right frontal cortices was associated with poorer performance (i.e., lower accuracy rate) in the WM task. However, this significant association disappeared if we employed a conservative statistical threshold set at p = 0.05 (corrected with FWE). (B,D) A decrease in the [18F]FDG-SUVR in the frontal cortices was associated with poorer performance (i.e., higher IE) in the WM task. (C) This significant correlation was still observed in the right frontal cortex if we employed a conservative statistical threshold set at p = 0.05 (corrected with FWE). The yellow color bar indicates the T-value. L, left hemisphere. R, right hemisphere. A, anterior. P, posterior.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. SPM analyses with multiple regression models in which the accuracy rate (A) or the IE scores (B–D) for the RWM conditions were used as independent variables. (A) A decrease in the [18F]FDG-SUVR in the occipital and parietal cortices was associated with poorer performance (i.e., lower accuracy rate) in the RWM task. However, this significant association disappeared if we employed a conservative statistical threshold set at p = 0.05 (corrected with FWE). (B,D) A decrease in the [18F]FDG-SUVR in the occipital and parietal cortices was associated with poorer performance (i.e., higher IE) in the RWM task. (C) This significant correlation was still observed in the bilateral occipito-temporal cortices if we employed a conservative statistical threshold set at p = 0.05 (corrected with FWE). The yellow color bar indicates the T-value. L, left hemisphere. R, right hemisphere. A, anterior. P, posterior.

Regarding the correlation between [18F]FDG-SUVR and IE scores in the healthy elderly subjects (n = 22), with the conservative statistical threshold set at p = 0.05 (corrected with FWE), no significant correlation was found for the three tasks. In all participants, including the cognitively impaired subjects (n = 100), with the conservative statistical threshold set at p = 0.05 (corrected with FWE), no significant correlation was found for the CRT task. For the WM task, decreases in the [18F]FDG-SUVR in the right middle frontal gyrus (z score = 4.91, cluster size = 100 voxels) and the left superior frontal gyrus (z score = 4.46, cluster size = 4 voxels) were associated with lower performance (Figures 3B–D and Table 2). For the RWM task, which was the focus of our study, a decrease in the [18F]FDG-SUVR in the bilateral EBA, the right FBA (i.e., decreased glucose metabolism in the right middle/inferior occipital gyri and the middle temporal gyrus), and in the left middle occipital gyrus was significantly correlated with poorer task performance (Figures 4B–D and Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Brain regions in which a decrease in the [18F]FDG-SUVR was significantly associated with poorer task performance.

Experiment 2: tDCS Study

Regarding the IE score, an unpaired t-test (one-sided) did not reveal significantly higher performance (i.e., a lower IE score) in the right occipital anodal stimulation group (i.e., active condition, n = 20) compared to the control group (n = 20) (t = 0.684, p-value > 0.05), which did not support our hypothesis. However, regarding the accuracy rate, as shown in Figure 5A, a Wilcoxon rank-sum test (one-sided) revealed a significantly higher performance (i.e., higher accuracy rate) in the RWM task in the right occipital anodal stimulation group (i.e., active condition, n = 20) than in the control group (W = 135.5, p-value = 0.020), which supported our hypothesis.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. (A) Accuracy scores in the three tasks (i.e., CRT, WM, and RWM) with the application of active (red circles) and control (blue circles) transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). The RWM task scores during right occipital anodal stimulation (i.e., the active condition, n = 20) were significantly higher than those during the control stimulation (n = 20). (B) If the control condition was divided into two conditions, i.e., the reversed condition (frontal anodal/occipital cathodal stimulation; n = 10) (blue circles) and sham condition (n = 10) (green circles), the RWM task scores during the active condition (n = 20) were significantly higher than those during the reversed condition (n = 10) but not than those during the sham condition (n = 10). The error bar indicates 1 standard error. CRT, choice reaction time. WM, working memory. RWM, working memory with hand mental rotation.

Next, we divided the control condition into two conditions, i.e., the reversed (frontal anodal/occipital cathodal stimulation; n = 10) condition and the sham condition (n = 10). Regarding the IE score, an unpaired t-test (one-sided) did not reveal a significantly higher performance (i.e., lower IE score) in the active condition group (n = 20) than in the reversed condition group (n = 10) (t = –0.388, p-value > 0.05) or the sham condition group (n = 10) (t = 1.643, p-value > 0.05). Regarding the accuracy rate, a Wilcoxon rank-sum test (one-sided) revealed a significantly higher performance (i.e., higher accuracy rate) in the RWM task in the active condition group (n = 20) than in the reversed condition group (n = 10) (W = 62.0, p-value = 0.025), but not in the sham condition group (n = 10) (W = 73.5, p-value > 0.05) (Figure 5B).

As complementary analyses for the IE score of the three repeated tasks, a two-way ANOVA (3 tasks × 2 tDCS conditions) revealed neither a significant main effect (tDCS condition; F = 0.173, p-value > 0.05) nor an interaction between the two factors (task × tDCS condition; F = 0.501, p-value > 0.05). When we divided the control condition into two conditions, i.e., the reversed (n = 10) condition and the sham condition (n = 10), a two-way ANOVA (3 tasks × 3 tDCS conditions) revealed neither a significant main effect (tDCS condition; F = 2.487, p-value > 0.05) nor an interaction between the two factors (task × tDCS condition; F = 1.691, p-value > 0.05).

Discussion

In the present study, the results of E1 demonstrated that the right LOTC and the FBA are involved in the mental rotation of body part imagery, which supported the previous knowledge regarding LOTC function in the mental rotation task (Tomasino and Gremese, 2015), in the perception of body parts (Downing et al., 2001; Peelen and Downing, 2007; Moro et al., 2008; Bracci et al., 2012; Pitcher et al., 2012), and in their movements and motor representations (Astafiev et al., 2004; Peelen and Downing, 2007; Urgesi et al., 2007a; Ishizu et al., 2009; van Nuenen et al., 2012; Tomasino and Gremese, 2015; Zimmermann et al., 2016). In E2, we tested the hypothesis that active stimulation of the LOTC using tDCS would enhance performance on the mental manipulation in the hand imagery task. The results suggested a possible effect of tDCS on this task. Further study is necessary to clarify whether these results in E1 and E2 were specific for the mental manipulation of body parts but not non-body parts.

Mental rotation is the ability to rotate an object or a body part in one’s mind, i.e., the ability to make perceptual judgments regarding an object’s new spatial configuration in one’s mind. Many neuroimaging studies on the functional neuroanatomy of mental rotation have identified three core brain regions that are considered to serve mental rotation processing: (i) the superior parietal region (Alivisatos and Petrides, 1997; Iwaki et al., 1999; Harris et al., 2000; Richter et al., 2000; Jordan et al., 2001); (ii) the motor and/or premotor regions (Kosslyn et al., 1998, 2001; Vingerhoets et al., 2002); and (iii) the extrastriate visual region (e.g., V5) (Howard et al., 1995; Cohen et al., 1996; Barnes et al., 2000). The neural correlates of the mental rotation of hands in the mind have also been a focus of study (Bonda et al., 1995; Parsons et al., 1995; Kosslyn et al., 1998; Vingerhoets et al., 2002; Wraga et al., 2003; de Lange et al., 2005; Seurinck et al., 2005; de Lange et al., 2006; Creem-Regehr et al., 2007; Corradi-Dell’Acqua et al., 2009; Ferri et al., 2012a; Papeo et al., 2012). One fMRI study reported that the increase in cerebral blood flow (CBF) in the LOTC was greater during the mental rotation of hands than during the mental rotation of objects (Vingerhoets et al., 2002). Coincidentally, the region where the CBF was significantly increased in this study was similar to the area identified using our lesion mapping method, as shown in Figure 4C. Our results contribute further evidence that deterioration in the LOTC is associated with lower performance in the mental rotation of body parts. However, using activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis, a recent study reported that the right middle occipital gyrus was activated during the mental rotation of non-bodily stimuli compared to that of bodily related stimuli (Tomasino and Gremese, 2015). This activation was not observed in the reverse contrast (i.e., non-bodily related < bodily related stimuli) (Tomasino and Gremese, 2015). The discrepancy between this meta-analysis and the present study may be explained by two reasons. One possibility is that the LOTC is activated during mental rotation of any targets (not specific for body parts), which was also supported by this previous meta-analysis (Tomasino and Gremese, 2015). The other possibility is that the discrepancy is due to the difference in the imaging methodology. The present study revealed the brain region where functional deterioration induced a disability in mental rotation, but the previous activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revealed the brain region where the mental rotation task induced cerebral activation. Intriguingly, using transcranial magnetic stimulation with a cortical inhibitory paradigm, recent studies have demonstrated that inhibition of the EBA results in reduced performances in action planning in healthy subjects (Zimmermann et al., 2016) and in patients with Parkinson’s disease (van Nuenen et al., 2012), which is consistent with our results from E1 (i.e., functional deterioration in the LOTC induced the disability in mental rotation).

The exact mechanism by which mental rotation occurs in the human brain is still unclear, but two distinct possible strategies to execute the mental rotation task have been reported (Kosslyn et al., 1998): one is an internal strategy in which one anticipates what he/she would see if one were to physically manipulate the object (or the body part); the other is an external strategy in which one visualizes the consequences of someone else or an external force moving the object (or the body part). The internal strategy may be used in the case of imagery of the hands because the participants were ready to move their hands in their minds. In these cases, the imagery may be focused on the mental motor transformation of the viewer rather than of the viewed object. The external strategy may be used in the case of imagery of objects rotated by an external device such as an electric motor system because this type of stimulus does not prime one to move one’s own hands and therefore does not involve motor processes. Subsequent neuroimaging studies on mental rotation have also supported these two distinct strategies (Kosslyn et al., 2001; Vingerhoets et al., 2002; Zacks, 2008). Although we did not know which strategy our participants preferred when rotating their hands in their mind in this study, both strategies can explain our results. One explanation is that internal hand imagery (possibly governed by the EBA) was necessary for the subjective view (i.e., internal strategy) required to achieve mental rotation of the hands. The other explanation is that objective hand imagery (also possibly governed by the EBA) was required for an external object (i.e., external strategy) to be rotated.

Transcranial direct current stimulation induces a modulatory effect on brain activity (Nitsche and Paulus, 2001), although the participants barely noticed the stimulation. Therefore, tDCS allows for a reliable sham condition (Gandiga et al., 2006). This factor is essential for a double-blind, placebo-controlled study. In the present study (E2), we tested the hypothesized effect of tDCS on the mental manipulation (i.e., mental rotation) of body part imagery. The results showed a significant effect of tDCS on the accuracy ratio of mental manipulation of body part imagery, but not on the IE score. With regard to motor learning, previous studies have demonstrated that anodal stimulation of the primary motor cortex (Nitsche et al., 2003; Antal et al., 2004b), premotor cortex (Wade and Hammond, 2015) and middle temporal V5 (Antal et al., 2004b) resulted in increased performance. In addition, a recent study of human gesture processing showed that anodal stimulation of the left inferior parietal lobe induced better performance (Weiss et al., 2013). Although further studies are necessary to confirm whether anodal stimulation of these brain areas affects the mental manipulation of body part imagery, the right LOTC may be a possible candidate that plays a pivotal role not only in the mental recognition of body parts, as previously reported, but also in the mental manipulation of body part imagery, as shown in the present study.

There were some important limitations of our study. First, we did not employ a mental rotation task with non-body parts (i.e., objects) as a control rotation task in E1 and E2. Therefore, we could not conclude that these results were specific for a mental rotation task with body parts. Second, in E2, the anode electrode was placed over PO8 (international EEG 10/10 system) to activate the LOTC. However, the spatial resolution of tDCS is too low to precisely stimulate functional subdivisions of a cortical area (Woods et al., 2016) and inconsistent behavioral outcomes of tDCS might be caused by individual anatomical differences (Kim et al., 2014). Furthermore, the effect of both cathode and anode stimuli on the right frontal area in the active and in the reversed conditions, respectively, may be a confounding factor in cognitive performance (Seidler et al., 2017). Therefore, we could not exclude the possibility that the effects were driven by cathodal stimulation of the DLPFC rather than anodal stimulation of the LOTC. Further study using simulation methods to estimate brain regional current flow (e.g., the finite element method) is necessary to conclude the facilitative role of the LOTC in the mental rotation task. Third, in E2, we employed healthy subjects but not elderly subjects with dementia. Therefore, we could not test the possibility that tDCS is effective for individuals with cognitive decline in the mental rotation task. Fourth, regarding the IE score in E2, we failed to demonstrated any significant effect of tDCS (i.e., in unpaired t-tests and in two-way ANOVA analyses), which may be attributed to the small sample size. Furthermore, these visual tasks might have been too easy for the healthy subjects to accomplish because the difficulty of these tasks targeted elderly subjects in E1 who showed some degree of cognitive decline. Therefore, further improving their IE scores in any of the tasks via anodal stimulation to the LOTC may not have been possible (i.e., a ceiling effect) in the healthy younger subjects. Regarding the accuracy ratio, further improvement may not have been possible in the CRT and WM tasks for the same reason (i.e., a ceiling effect), which also implies that the putative effect of LOTC stimulation in the RWM task might not be specific for the manipulation of body parts. Therefore, further study with a larger sample size and with various visual tasks in which the degree of difficulty is adjusted for the participants is necessary to determine the specific role of the LOTC in the mental rotation task. Despite these limitations, the present results supported the previous knowledge regarding LOTC function in the mental rotation task (Tomasino and Gremese, 2015), in the perception of body parts (Downing et al., 2001; Peelen and Downing, 2007; Moro et al., 2008; Bracci et al., 2012; Pitcher et al., 2012) and in their movements and motor representations (Astafiev et al., 2004; Peelen and Downing, 2007; Urgesi et al., 2007a; Ishizu et al., 2009; van Nuenen et al., 2012; Zimmermann et al., 2016). In addition, our data suggested the facilitative effect of tDCS (anodal stimuli for the right posterior brain area) on performance in the mental rotation task.

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Hamamatsu University School of Medicine and Hamamatsu Medical Center and by the Ethics Committee of Kanazawa University Hospital in Japan. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to enrollment. The methods were carried out in accordance with approved guidelines. In case of patients with dementia, written informed consent was obtained from participants and their family prior to enrollment. The methods were carried out in accordance with approved guidelines.

Author Contributions

All authors listed, have made substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Funding

The study was supported by the Center of Innovation Program of the Japan Science and Technology Agency, JST.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Toshihiko Kanno, Yasuo Tanizaki (Hamamatsu Medical Center), Masami Futatsubashi (Hamamatsu Photonics KK), and Yutaka Naito (Japan Environment Research Corporation) for their support.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00181/full#supplementary-material

References

Alivisatos, B., and Petrides, M. (1997). Functional activation of the human brain during mental rotation. Neuropsychologia 35, 111–118. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00083-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (ed.) (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: Author.

Google Scholar

Antal, A., Kincses, T. Z., Nitsche, M. A., Bartfai, O., and Paulus, W. (2004a). Excitability changes induced in the human primary visual cortex by transcranial direct current stimulation: direct electrophysiological evidence. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 45, 702–707. doi: 10.1167/iovs.03-0688

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antal, A., Nitsche, M. A., Kincses, T. Z., Kruse, W., Hoffmann, K. P., and Paulus, W. (2004b). Facilitation of visuo-motor learning by transcranial direct current stimulation of the motor and extrastriate visual areas in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 2888–2892. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03367.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Astafiev, S. V., Stanley, C. M., Shulman, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2004). Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex responds to the performance of motor actions. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 542–548. doi: 10.1038/nn1241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barnes, J., Howard, R. J., Senior, C., Brammer, M., Bullmore, E. T., Simmons, A., et al. (2000). Cortical activity during rotational and linear transformations. Neuropsychologia 38, 1148–1156. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00025-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonda, E., Petrides, M., Frey, S., and Evans, A. (1995). Neural correlates of mental transformations of the body-in-space. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 11180–11184. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.24.11180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bracci, S., Cavina-Pratesi, C., Ietswaart, M., Caramazza, A., and Peelen, M. V. (2012). Closely overlapping responses to tools and hands in left lateral occipitotemporal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 107, 1443–1456. doi: 10.1152/jn.00619.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, M. S., Kosslyn, S. M., Breiter, H. C., DiGirolamo, G. J., Thompson, W. L., Anderson, A. K., et al. (1996). Changes in cortical activity during mental rotation. A mapping study using functional MRI. Brain 119(Pt 1), 89–100.

Google Scholar

Corradi-Dell’Acqua, C., Tomasino, B., and Fink, G. R. (2009). What is the position of an arm relative to the body? Neural correlates of body schema and body structural description. J. Neurosci. 29, 4162–4171. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4861-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creem-Regehr, S. H., Neil, J. A., and Yeh, H. J. (2007). Neural correlates of two imagined egocentric transformations. Neuroimage 35, 916–927. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

David, N., Cohen, M. X., Newen, A., Bewernick, B. H., Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R., et al. (2007). The extrastriate cortex distinguishes between the consequences of one’s own and others’ behavior. Neuroimage 36, 1004–1014. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Lange, F. P., Hagoort, P., and Toni, I. (2005). Neural topography and content of movement representations. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 97–112. doi: 10.1162/0898929052880039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Lange, F. P., Helmich, R. C., and Toni, I. (2006). Posture influences motor imagery: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 33, 609–617. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Downing, P. E., Jiang, Y., Shuman, M., and Kanwisher, N. (2001). A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human body. Science 293, 2470–2473. doi: 10.1126/science.1063414

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferri, F., Frassinetti, F., Ardizzi, M., Costantini, M., and Gallese, V. (2012a). A sensorimotor network for the bodily self. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 1584–1595. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferri, S., Kolster, H., Jastorff, J., and Orban, G. A. (2012b). The overlap of the EBA and the MT/V5 cluster. Neuroimage 66C, 412–425. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandiga, P. C., Hummel, F. C., and Cohen, L. G. (2006). Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): a tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 845–850. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, I. M., Egan, G. F., Sonkkila, C., Tochon-Danguy, H. J., Paxinos, G., and Watson, J. D. (2000). Selective right parietal lobe activation during mental rotation: a parametric PET study. Brain 123(Pt 1), 65–73. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.1.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, R. J., Bullmore, E., Brammer, M., Williams, S. C., Mellers, J., Woodruff, P., et al. (1995). Activation of area V5 by visual perception of motion demonstrated with echoplanar MR imaging. Magn. Reson. Imaging 13, 907–909. doi: 10.1016/0730-725X(95)00042-F

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishizu, T., Noguchi, A., Ito, Y., Ayabe, T., and Kojima, S. (2009). Motor activity and imagery modulate the body-selective region in the occipital-temporal area: a near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neurosci. Lett. 465, 85–89. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.08.079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iwaki, S., Ueno, S., Imada, T., and Tonoike, M. (1999). Dynamic cortical activation in mental image processing revealed by biomagnetic measurement. Neuroreport 10, 1793–1797. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199906030-00031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jordan, K., Heinze, H. J., Lutz, K., Kanowski, M., and Jancke, L. (2001). Cortical activations during the mental rotation of different visual objects. Neuroimage 13, 143–152. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kikuchi, M., Hirosawa, T., Yokokura, M., Yagi, S., Mori, N., Yoshikawa, E., et al. (2011). Effects of brain amyloid deposition and reduced glucose metabolism on the default mode of brain function in normal aging. J. Neurosci. 31, 11193–11199. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2535-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. H., Kim, D. W., Chang, W. H., Kim, Y. H., Kim, K., and Im, C. H. (2014). Inconsistent outcomes of transcranial direct current stimulation may originate from anatomical differences among individuals: electric field simulation using individual MRI data. Neurosci. Lett. 564, 6–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2014.01.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kitada, R., Yoshihara, K., Sasaki, A. T., Hashiguchi, M., Kochiyama, T., and Sadato, N. (2014). The brain network underlying the recognition of hand gestures in the blind: the supramodal role of the extrastriate body area. J. Neurosci. 34, 10096–10108. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0500-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosslyn, S. M., DiGirolamo, G. J., Thompson, W. L., and Alpert, N. M. (1998). Mental rotation of objects versus hands: neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. Psychophysiology 35, 151–161. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3520151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosslyn, S. M., Ganis, G., and Thompson, W. L. (2001). Neural foundations of imagery. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 635–642. doi: 10.1038/35090055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhn, S., Keizer, A., Rombouts, S. A., and Hommel, B. (2011). The functional and neural mechanism of action preparation: roles of EBA and FFA in voluntary action control. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 214–220. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Labruna, L., Jamil, A., Fresnoza, S., Batsikadze, G., Kuo, M. F., Vanderschelden, B., et al. (2016). Efficacy of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation is related to sensitivity to transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Stimul. 9, 8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludwig, V. U., Adachi, I., and Matsuzawa, T. (2011). Visuoauditory mappings between high luminance and high pitch are shared by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 20661–20665. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112605108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., and Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of department of health and human services task force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 34, 939–944. doi: 10.1212/WNL.34.7.939

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moro, V., Urgesi, C., Pernigo, S., Lanteri, P., Pazzaglia, M., and Aglioti, S. M. (2008). The neural basis of body form and body action agnosia. Neuron 60, 235–246. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neary, D., Snowden, J. S., Gustafson, L., Passant, U., Stuss, D., Black, S., et al. (1998). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 51, 1546–1554. doi: 10.1212/WNL.51.6.1546

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., Wassermann, E. M., Priori, A., Lang, N., Antal, A., et al. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 1, 206–223. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Doemkes, S., Karakose, T., Antal, A., Liebetanz, D., Lang, N., et al. (2007). Shaping the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation of the human motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 3109–3117. doi: 10.1152/jn.01312.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2000). Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Physiol. 527(Pt 3), 633–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2001). Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology 57, 1899–1901. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation–update 2011. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 29, 463–492. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2011-0618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Schauenburg, A., Lang, N., Liebetanz, D., Exner, C., Paulus, W., et al. (2003). Facilitation of implicit motor learning by weak transcranial direct current stimulation of the primary motor cortex in the human. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 619–626. doi: 10.1162/089892903321662994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orlov, T., Makin, T. R., and Zohary, E. (2010). Topographic representation of the human body in the occipitotemporal cortex. Neuron 68, 586–600. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouchi, Y., Okada, H., Yoshikawa, E., Futatsubashi, M., and Nobezawa, S. (2001). Absolute changes in regional cerebral blood flow in association with upright posture in humans: an orthostatic PET study. J. Nucl. Med. 42, 707–712.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ouchi, Y., Yoshikawa, E., Futatsubashi, M., Yagi, S., Ueki, T., and Nakamura, K. (2009). Altered brain serotonin transporter and associated glucose metabolism in Alzheimer disease. J. Nucl. Med. 50, 1260–1266. doi: 10.2967/jnumed.109.063008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouchi, Y., Yoshikawa, E., Okada, H., Futatsubashi, M., Sekine, Y., Iyo, M., et al. (1999). Alterations in binding site density of dopamine transporter in the striatum, orbitofrontal cortex, and amygdala in early Parkinson’s disease: compartment analysis for beta-CFT binding with positron emission tomography. Ann. Neurol. 45, 601–610. doi: 10.1002/1531-8249(199905)45:5<601::AID-ANA8>3.0.CO;2-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papeo, L., Rumiati, R. I., Cecchetto, C., and Tomasino, B. (2012). On-line changing of thinking about words: the effect of cognitive context on neural responses to verb reading. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 2348–2362. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parsons, L. M., Fox, P. T., Downs, J. H., Glass, T., Hirsch, T. B., Martin, C. C., et al. (1995). Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET. Nature 375, 54–58. doi: 10.1038/375054a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peelen, M. V., and Downing, P. E. (2005). Selectivity for the human body in the fusiform gyrus. J. Neurophysiol. 93, 603–608. doi: 10.1152/jn.00513.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peelen, M. V., and Downing, P. E. (2007). The neural basis of visual body perception. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 636–648. doi: 10.1038/nrn2195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. Intern. Med. 256, 183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitcher, D., Goldhaber, T., Duchaine, B., Walsh, V., and Kanwisher, N. (2012). Two critical and functionally distinct stages of face and body perception. J. Neurosci. 32, 15877–15885. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2624-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter, W., Somorjai, R., Summers, R., Jarmasz, M., Menon, R. S., Gati, J. S., et al. (2000). Motor area activity during mental rotation studied by time-resolved single-trial fMRI. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 310–320. doi: 10.1162/089892900562129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarzlose, R. F., Baker, C. I., and Kanwisher, N. (2005). Separate face and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus. J. Neurosci. 25, 11055–11059. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2621-05.2005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidler, R. D., Gluskin, B. S., and Greeley, B. (2017). Right prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation enhances multi-day savings in sensorimotor adaptation. J. Neurophysiol. 117, 429–435. doi: 10.1152/jn.00563.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seurinck, R., Vingerhoets, G., Vandemaele, P., Deblaere, K., and Achten, E. (2005). Trial pacing in mental rotation tasks. Neuroimage 25, 1187–1196. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. C., Wiggett, A. J., and Downing, P. E. (2007). Functional MRI analysis of body and body part representations in the extrastriate and fusiform body areas. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 1626–1633. doi: 10.1152/jn.00012.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasino, B., and Gremese, M. (2015). Effects of stimulus type and strategy on mental rotation network: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:693. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00693

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Townsend, J. T., and Ashby, F. G. (1983). Stochastic Modeling of Elementary Psychological Processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Urgesi, C., Calvo-Merino, B., Haggard, P., and Aglioti, S. M. (2007a). Transcranial magnetic stimulation reveals two cortical pathways for visual body processing. J. Neurosci. 27, 8023–8030. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0789-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urgesi, C., Candidi, M., Ionta, S., and Aglioti, S. M. (2007b). Representation of body identity and body actions in extrastriate body area and ventral premotor cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 30–31. doi: 10.1038/nn1815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Nuenen, B. F., Helmich, R. C., Buenen, N., van de Warrenburg, B. P., Bloem, B. R., and Toni, I. (2012). Compensatory activity in the extrastriate body area of Parkinson’s disease patients. J. Neurosci. 32, 9546–9553. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0335-12.2012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vingerhoets, G., de Lange, F. P., Vandemaele, P., Deblaere, K., and Achten, E. (2002). Motor imagery in mental rotation: an fMRI study. NeuroImage 17, 1623–1633. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1290

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wade, S., and Hammond, G. (2015). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation over premotor cortex facilitates observational learning of a motor sequence. Eur. J. Neurosci. 41, 1597–1602. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, P. H., Achilles, E. I., Moos, K., Hesse, M. D., Sparing, R., and Fink, G. R. (2013). Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of left parietal cortex facilitates gesture processing in healthy subjects. J. Neurosci. 33, 19205–19211. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4714-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woods, A. J., Antal, A., Bikson, M., Boggio, P. S., Brunoni, A. R., Celnik, P., et al. (2016). A technical guide to tDCS, and related non-invasive brain stimulation tools. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 1031–1048. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wraga, M., Thompson, W. L., Alpert, N. M., and Kosslyn, S. M. (2003). Implicit transfer of motor strategies in mental rotation. Brain Cogn. 52, 135–143. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00033-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zacks, J. M. (2008). Neuroimaging studies of mental rotation: a meta-analysis and review. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1–19. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmermann, M., Verhagen, L., de Lange, F. P., and Toni, I. (2016). The extrastriate body area computes desired goal states during action planning. eNeuro 3, 1–13. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0020-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: extrastriate body area, hand imagery, lateral occipito-temporal cortex, mental rotation, transcranial direct current stimulation

Citation: Kikuchi M, Takahashi T, Hirosawa T, Oboshi Y, Yoshikawa E, Minabe Y and Ouchi Y (2017) The Lateral Occipito-temporal Cortex Is Involved in the Mental Manipulation of Body Part Imagery. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:181. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00181

Received: 25 September 2016; Accepted: 28 March 2017;
Published: 11 April 2017.

Edited by:

Mikhail Lebedev, Duke University, USA

Reviewed by:

Carlo Sestieri, University of Chieti-Pescara, Italy
Marius Zimmermann, Stockholm University, Sweden

Copyright © 2017 Kikuchi, Takahashi, Hirosawa, Oboshi, Yoshikawa, Minabe and Ouchi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yasuomi Ouchi, ouchi@hama-med.ac.jp

Download