Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 24 April 2018
Sec. Sensory Neuroscience
Volume 12 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00160

Commentary: An Adaptation-Induced Repulsion Illusion in Tactile Spatial Perception

  • Neuroscience Research Australia, University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia

A commentary on
An Adaptation-Induced Repulsion Illusion in Tactile Spatial Perception

by Li, L., Chan, A., Iqbal, S. M., and Goldreich, D. (2017). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:331. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00331

Locating a stimulus on the skin is a well-described perceptual task, but little is known about how this ability remains stable under ecological conditions. Specifically, the effect of recent stimulation has been the subject of very few studies in the tactile domain (Craig, 1993; Braun et al., 2000), but is well described in the visual system. Exposure to lines at one orientation causes perceptual repulsion of the orientation of a subsequently viewed line (Gibson and Radner, 1937), an effect also observed in touch (Silver, 1969). In the tactile domain, contention surrounds a different kind of aftereffect, spatial repulsion, whereby adaptation can elicit a position shift of the perceived location of a stimulus away from the adapted area. Maintaining stable spatial perception in touch in these situations is important for tasks requiring fine motor control (Medina and Coslett, 2016; Goodman and Bensmaia, 2018), like the case of an artist switching between tools of different grip sizes, or a factory worker sorting and grading produce.

Day and Singer (1964) found a spatial repulsion aftereffect following sustained pressure on the forearm. Two bars placed either side of the adapted region were perceived as further apart than a comparison stimulus on the other arm. If the region outside of the bars was instead adapted, the perceived distance decreased. However, Gilbert (1967) found that this spatial distortion arose from a methodological bias from using an asymmetrical range of comparison stimuli. Given these results it remained open to investigation whether spatial repulsion is observed in the tactile domain and if so, by what neural mechanism.

In the study of Li et al. (2017) these questions were addressed over three experiments using a force-controlled two-point stimulus. The first experiment used a two-alternative forced choice task in which participants were given the two-point stimulus on the reference arm (always given 30 mm) and subsequently on the comparison arm (counterbalanced order). Critically, a symmetrical range of comparison stimuli was used (6, 12, 18, 24, 30, 36, 42, 48, or 54 mm). Participants were relatively stable at this task across time. In the second experiment adaptation was measured using a two-alternative forced choice touch detection task. Exposure to stimulation consisted of an initial 40 s of vibration with subsequent 3 s bursts of top-up stimulation separated by short breaks for threshold testing. Adaptation from the stimulation resulted in increased touch detection thresholds.The severity of the adaptation decreased with distance from the center of the stimulation. In the third experiment, vibro-tactile stimulation was applied to the area within the two points on the reference arm in the critical condition. Exposure was the same as in the second experiment, with the two-point comparison task performed in the breaks between top-up stimulation. The perceived distance of the two-point stimulus spanning the adapted area increased by approximately 10%.

In summary, the two preliminary experiments showed that tactile spatial perception is stable with time and that vibro-tactile stimulation reduces sensitivity to a subsequently applied touch stimulus. The main experiment found that adaptation results in perceived spatial expansion. Li et al. (2017) propose that the spatial distortion arises from changes in receptive field size, given two assumptions about the neural processing of tactile information. First, the brain remains largely unaware of peripheral adaptation (Seriès et al., 2009). Second, perceived location comes from the pooled response from multiple neurons (Ghazanfar et al., 2000). This hypothesis states that adaptation causes the signaled location of a touch to shift away from the adapted region, as there is a shift in the distribution of neural activity corresponding to the stimulus.

In a different test of such a model, my own study tested the effects of adaptation following exposure to tactile motion on the dorsal side of the forearm. The study used a 15 cm/s motion stimulus which moved along a 19 cm path but only touched the skin for 4.5 cm at the proximal and distal ends of motion. Exposure to this stimulation resulted in an inwards position shift (i.e., repulsion from the stimulus) of a stimulus at the edge of the gap (Brooks et al., 2015). The size of the shift was approximately six percent of the gap size, comparable to the effect observed by Li et al. (2017). Together these observations are most consistent with changes to receptive field geometry, but whether these manifest sub-cortically or cortically is unknown. One test of this would be to apply local anaesthesia as the adapting stimulus, or use different adapting and test stimuli (e.g., selectively adapt a class of mechanoreceptors).

Li et al. have shown results for only one stimulus size of adaptation, but could the observed spatial expansion be size dependent? Calzolari et al. (2017) show that repetitive exposure to a small or large two-point extent stimulus on the hand causes a subsequently presented stimulus to feel larger or smaller respectively. Local changes in receptive field geometry are unlikely to explain the effect, as the adapting stimulus was applied at slightly different locations to reduce the possibility of local adaptation. The authors raise the possibility of reduced responses of distance specific neurons with tuning curves that overlap with the adaptor.

Rather than considering aftereffects to arise as a result of fatiguing neurons, they might represent the optimization of perception to the recent statistics of the environment. In vision there is increasing evidence that perception is optimized to statistical regularities of the environment (Seriès and Seitz, 2013). In touch, evidence in macaques (Clark et al., 1988; Wang et al., 1995) and in humans (Braun et al., 2000) suggests that the somatosensory cortex reorganizes in response to prolonged periods of tactile stimulation. Whether any such processes operate for shorter windows of sensory input in other brain regions remains an open question. One way in which the new results of Li et al. (2017) can be functionalized is under the hypothesis that the body is used as a tool to perceive external objects (Linkenauger et al., 2010; Van der Hoort and Ehrsson, 2016). Under this hypothesis, the rapid malleability of higher order body representations (Taylor-Clarke et al., 2004) can afford enhanced object perception. For instance, creating the visual illusion that the arm is larger improves tactile discrimination (Kennett et al., 2001) and increases perceived distances on the arm (Taylor-Clarke et al., 2004). Thus, the perceived elongation of a two-point stimulus in the Li et al. (2017) study affords higher resolution perception of objects touching the adapted region.

Unlike many previous studies, Li et al. (2017) show preliminary evidence that aftereffects following only seconds of stimulation may persist for up to 20 min. Extending the exposure to tactile stimulation can result in even longer aftereffects. Mechanical stimulation for minutes can lead to improvements in tactile discrimination for hours (Godde et al., 2000), and weeks of vibro-tactile stimulation elicit shifts in perceived touch location weeks after the cessation of stimulation (Craig, 1993). A possible application of these methods is to correct distorted body representations. For instance, stroke patients with damage to somatosensory areas of the brain mislocalize touch toward the middle of the hand (Rapp et al., 2002; White et al., 2010). This distortion likely arises from a reduced cortical representation of the hand, leading to more uncertainty (Rapp et al., 2002; White et al., 2010). If sustained tactile stimulation does improve perception then it might be viable as a treatment for these patients.

In summary, it is clear that sustained pressure leads to repulsive spatial aftereffects. Li et al. (2017) have shown that vibro-tactile stimulation elicits a spatial dilation of a two-point stimulus spanning the stimulated area. This result seems consistent with a probabilistic model in which changes in the population response of neurons, to which the brain is unaware, result in a spatial distortion. These new findings raise intriguing possibilities for further work to determine if the spatial distortion arises from sub-cortical or cortical adaptation, optimizes object perception, and can be harnessed for clinical use.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Thanks to Jen Nicholas for comments on a draft manuscript.

References

Braun, C., Schweizer, R., Elbert, T., Birbaumer, N., and Taub, E. (2000). Differential activation in somatosensory cortex for different discrimination tasks. J. Neurosci. 20, 446–450. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-01-00446.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, J., Seizova-Cajic, T., and Taylor, J. L. (2015). “Motion across a tactile scotoma shifts perceived position,” in Presentation at the Conference of the Australasian Society for Experimental Psychology (Sydney, NSW).

Calzolari, E., Azañón, E., Danvers, M., Vallar, G., and Longo, M. R. (2017). Adaptation aftereffects reveal that tactile distance is a basic somatosensory feature. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 4555–4560. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1614979114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, S. A., Allard, T., Jenkins, W. M., and Merzenich, M. M. (1988). Receptive fields in the body-surface map in adult cortex defined by temporally correlated inputs. Nature 332, 444–445. doi: 10.1038/332444a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, J. C. (1993). Anomalous sensations following prolonged tactile stimulation. Neuropsychologia 31, 277–291. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(93)90092-E

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Day, R. H., and Singer, G. (1964). A tactile spatial aftereffect. Aust. J. Psychol. 16, 33–37. doi: 10.1080/00049536408255500

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghazanfar, A. A., Stambaugh, C. R., and Nicolelis, M. A. (2000). Encoding of tactile stimulus location by somatosensory thalamocortical ensembles. J. Neurosci. 20, 3761–3775. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-10-03761.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibson, J. J., and Radner, M. (1937). Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of tilted lines. I. Quant. Stud. J. Exp. Psychol. 20, 453–467. doi: 10.1037/h0059826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbert, A. J. (1967). Tactile spatial aftereffect or adaptation level? J. Exp. Psychol. 73, 450–455. doi: 10.1037/h0024258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godde, B., Stauffenberg, B., Spengler, F., and Dinse, H. R. (2000). Tactile coactivation-induced changes in spatial discrimination performance. J. Neurosci. 20, 1597–1604. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-04-01597.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, J. M., and Bensmaia, S. J. (2018). “The Neural Basis of Haptic Perception,” in Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, Sensation, Perception, and Attention, Vol. 201, eds J. T. Wixted and J. Serences (New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons).

Google Scholar

Kennett, S., Taylor-Clarke, M., and Haggard, P. (2001). Noninformative vision improves the spatial resolution of touch in humans. Curr. Biol. 11, 1188–1191. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00327-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, L., Chan, A., Iqbal, S. M., and Goldreich, D. (2017). An adaptation-induced repulsion illusion in tactile spatial perception. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:331. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linkenauger, S. A., Ramenzoni, V., and Proffitt, D. R. (2010). Illusory shrinkage and growth: body-based rescaling affects the perception of size. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1318–1325. doi: 10.1177/0956797610380700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medina, J., and Coslett, H. B. (2016). What can errors tell us about body representations? Cogn. Neuropsychol. 33, 5–25. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1188065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapp, B., Hendel, S. K., and Medina, J. (2002). Remodeling of somatosensory hand representations following cerebral lesions in humans. NeuroReport 13, 207–211. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200202110-00007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seriès, P., and Seitz, A. (2013). Learning what to expect (in visual perception). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:668. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seriès, P., Stocker, A. A., and Simoncelli, E. P. (2009). Is the homunculus “aware” of sensory adaptation? Neural. Comput. 21, 3271–3304. doi: 10.1162/neco.2009.09-08-869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silver, R. J. (1969). Tilt After-Effects in Touch. Doctoral dissertation, Waltham, MA: Brandeis University. Database: ProQuest/UMI Dissertations Publishing.

Google Scholar

Taylor-Clarke, M., Jacobsen, P., and Haggard, P. (2004). Keeping the world a constant size: object constancy in human touch. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 219–220. doi: 10.1038/nn1199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Hoort, B., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2016). Illusions of having small or large invisible bodies influence visual perception of object size. Sci. Rep. 6:34530. doi: 10.1038/srep34530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Merzenich, M. M., Sameshima, K., and Jenkins, W. M. (1995). Remodelling of hand representation in adult cortex determined by timing of tactile stimulation. Nature 378, 71–75. doi: 10.1038/378071a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, R. C., Aimola Davies, A. M., and Kischka, U. (2010). Errors of somatosensory localisation in a patient with right-hemisphere stroke. Neurocase 16, 238–258. doi: 10.1080/13554790903456175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: somatosensory, tactile distance, tactile aftereffects, adaptation, object perception

Citation: Brooks J (2018) Commentary: An Adaptation-Induced Repulsion Illusion in Tactile Spatial Perception. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:160. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00160

Received: 20 October 2017; Accepted: 06 April 2018;
Published: 24 April 2018.

Edited by:

Hauke R. Heekeren, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany

Reviewed by:

Srikantan S. Nagarajan, University of California, San Francisco, United States
Matthew R. Longo, Birkbeck University of London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2018 Brooks. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jack Brooks, jackbrooks@uchicago.edu

Download