Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 07 May 2018
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
Volume 12 - 2018 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00180

Understanding Activation Patterns in Shared Circuits: Toward a Value Driven Model

  • 1Brain and Creativity Institute, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 2Division of Occupational Science and Occupational Therapy, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 3Department of Economics, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States

Over the past decade many studies indicate that we utilize our own motor system to understand the actions of other people. This mirror neuron system (MNS) has been proposed to be involved in social cognition and motor learning. However, conflicting findings regarding the underlying mechanisms that drive these shared circuits make it difficult to decipher a common model of their function. Here we propose adapting a “value-driven” model to explain discrepancies in the human mirror system literature and to incorporate this model with existing models. We will use this model to explain discrepant activation patterns in multiple shared circuits in the human data, such that a unified model may explain reported activation patterns from previous studies as a function of value.

Introduction

Understanding other people’s actions and feelings is an essential component of successful social interactions. Recent neuroscience data indicate that the neural mechanisms involved in processing one’s own actions, sensations and emotions are also involved in perceiving and understanding the actions, sensations and emotions of others (Gallese and Goldman, 1998; Keysers et al., 2004, 2010; Keysers and Gazzola, 2009). These “shared circuits” are thought to constitute pre-reflective processes in social cognition, involving automatic and intuitive levels of representation (Coricelli, 2005). Such shared circuits include the mirror neuron system (MNS), the pain matrix and the somatosensory cortices.

The MNS (inferior frontal gyrus [IFG], ventral premotor cortex [vPMC] and posterior parietal cortex [PPC]) responds both when one executes an action and when one observes someone else make the same action (e.g., opening a bottle of champagne and watching someone else perform the action; Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). Data also indicate that some mirror neurons may also respond to the sounds of actions (Kohler, 2002). Because observing others activates one’s own motor systems, it is thought that the MNS is important for action and social understanding. That is, part of how we may understand other people’s actions and intentions is by simulating their actions onto our own motor representations (Keysers et al., 2004).

Since the discovery of the MNS, it has been suggested that other brain regions might also be active both for processing one’s own experiences as well for processing the experiences of other people. For example, there is evidence for a shared neural system for processing disgust, where human subjects use the same neural regions in the anterior insula for the physical experience of disgust as well as for perceiving another person experience disgust (facial expressions), has also been shown (Wicker et al., 2003). A similar social mirroring mechanism is thought to exist for emotion and pain processing as well as somatosensation. Previous research indicates that we process other people’s pain by activating the neural systems that processes pain in our own bodies. This “pain matrix” includes the insula, anterior and middle cingulate gyrus and somatosensory cortices (Singer et al., 2004; Avenanti et al., 2005; Jackson et al., 2006; Bufalari et al., 2007; Di Cesare et al., 2015), though it has been suggested that this network responds to a variety of salient stimuli (Lannetti and Mouraux, 2010). In addition, watching another person being touched (e.g., watching a snake slither up another person’s leg or being brushed up against by a cat) activates our own secondary somatosensory cortices (Keysers et al., 2004; Meyer, 2011). Keysers et al. (2004) reported that observing another person’s leg being stroked activated the secondary somatosensory cortex (SII) bilaterally, the same as when the person was touched themselves. Such “shared neural circuits” involved in processing one’s own experiences as well as the experiences of other people, might be an important neural basis of social cognition.

Discrepancy in Research Findings Regarding Activation of Shared Neural Circuits

The idea that we utilize our own sensorimotor representations to process and understand other people has been the focus of much recent research. However, there has been a great deal of discrepant findings in what drives activity in these shared neural circuits (e.g., MNS, somatosensory cortices, pain matrix, emotion-related brain regions). Some research groups have shown increased activity in shared circuits for observing liked individuals (Singer et al., 2006) or individuals more similar to the self (Xu et al., 2009) while other studies show the opposite (Fox et al., 2013; Losin et al., 2014). Some groups find more activity for observing actions for which one has expertise (Raichle et al., 2001; Cross et al., 2006, 2009; Beudel et al., 2011; Liew et al., 2011; Gardner et al., 2015), while other groups find that novelty drives activation patterns (Cross et al., 2012; Aziz-Zadeh, 2013; Grossmann et al., 2013; Liew et al., 2013; Tipper et al., 2015). These discrepant findings (discussed further in “Relation of the Value Model to Human Data” section) not only raise important questions about what drives activity in shared neural circuits, but also their function.

Previous Models

One model that has previously been put forward to better understand the function of the MNS and its activation patterns utilizes the notion of predictive modeling. In this model, Kilner et al. (2007) utilize largely established forward or generative models that are critical to motor control (Miall and Wolpert, 1996; Wolpert et al., 2003; Kilner et al., 2007; Neal and Kilner, 2012). They extend the same model for action observation. The predictive coding account posits that the MNS is involved in creating predications of other people’s actions (Gallese and Goldman, 1998), the context (Liepelt et al., 2009), or their physical body (Buccino et al., 2004). Following an empirical Bayes inference, the model states that our prior expectations of an action have an associated standard deviation. A predication error is generated by the comparison between the predicted action and the actual observed action. Thus, they theorize that predictive coding may provide a computational framework for inferring the causes of sensory information. In the case of action observation, causes may include goals, intentions and motor commands and sensory inputs may include observed kinematics. Thus, when we observe someone swing a tennis racket and hit a ball, we may use the same models that we use to perform the action ourselves to infer motor commands and kinematics from the observed actions of other people. A discrepancy in prediction error would result in greater MNS activation, which may reflect increased demands to learn, predict, or assimilate to novel actions (Cross et al., 2007).

Cross and her colleagues utilized this predictive model for the MNS to build a model that used predictability as a metric to explain fMRI results (Cross et al., 2012; Diersch et al., 2013). In their U-shaped model, observed actions that are extremely high or extremely low in predictability activated the MNS most strongly, while observed actions that are moderately predictable activated the MNS the least (Figure 1). However, when considering the neural efficiency theory, one would expect that increased familiarity with an action would result in a more efficient use of neural resources while perceiving that action. Cross and colleagues (Gardner et al., 2017) recently tested this theory and proposed adjusting the quadratic U-shaped predicative model to a cubic model to account for changes in neural efficiency upon increased familiarity with an action (Gardner et al., 2017). However, as we will delineate in “Relation of the Value Model to Human Data” section, the U-shaped model cannot explain why individuals with stroke show more activity for observation of actions made with the paretic rather than the non-paretic hand (the non-paretic hand is highly predictable while the paretic hand is moderately predictable, Garrison et al., 2013). Nor can it explain why observation of an action made by an amputee’s residual limb (highly unpredictable) and a typical hand action (highly predictable) should differ in their activation levels (Liew et al., 2013). While the cubic model has more potential to explain these results, the shape of the model remains largely unknown, and thus it is difficult to determine where many moderately familiar stimuli (e.g., paretic hand) would fit in the cubic model. For a stroke patient, a paretic hand is somewhere in the middle for familiarity, and in the cubic model, since the shape of it is vague, somewhere in the middle can either be high MNS activity or low MNS activity. Furthermore, while it is likely that familiarity and expertise modulate shared circuits, it seems that other factors, such as value, valance, etc., are needed to further explain these results. Indeed, there is a need to understand shared circuits as broader networks that are tied with emotion processing, reward systems and other circuits along with the sensorimotor networks within which they are commonly seen.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Familiarity Model: a representation of the predictability model proposed by Cross et al. (2012). The horizontal axis represents predictability of an action and the vertical axis represents activity in mirror neuron regions. In 2017, Cross and her colleagues updated this to a cubic model (Gardner et al., 2017).

Toward an Explanatory Model

Recent neurophysiological findings may shed light on an underlying mechanism that modulates the MNS. Single cell recordings in monkeys indicate that subjective value drives activity in mirror neurons in F5 (Caggiano et al., 2012). That is, mirror neurons respond to the observation of actions that are valuable to the monkey, such as picking up a banana as compared to picking up a pretzel or another food item they are not fond of (see Figure 2). Even observation of actions that from the offset are arbitrary to a monkey, once paired with reward, elicit a stronger response in mirror neurons than actions that are not paired with reward (Caggiano et al., 2012). The researchers posit that through connections with reward circuits in the basal ganglia, mirror neurons are especially attuned to observation of actions that are subjectively valuable to the observer (Caggiano et al., 2012). These findings are reminiscent of findings from a previous study by Platt and Glimcher (1999) showing that activity in the lateral intraparietal area (LIP), a brain region involved in transferring visual information to motor actions in the monkey as well as a region later shown to have mirror neurons (Shepherd et al., 2009), is correlated with expected value. Here, we define value as the subjective importance, worth or usefulness of something. Subjective value can be modulated by reward, learning, valence, motivation and social context.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mirror neuron activity is modulated by subjective value. As presented by Caggiano et al. (2012), the figure illustrates neural activity from single cell recordings of an exemplary mirror neuron in F5 of a monkey while it was presented with three different rewards: the most rewarding treat symbolized by a banana (blue coloring), a less relished reward indicated by the pretzel (green coloring) and an non-preferred food item represented by an “X” (red coloring). Taken from: Caggiano et al. (2012).

While the latter studies indicate that reward affects activity in the MNS, the direct neural pathways between monkey area F5 and prefrontal reward regions remain unknown. Previous studies indicate reward modulates neuronal activity during goal-directed actions in many parts of the brain, including mid-brain (ventral tegmental area [VTA], substantia nigra [SN]), basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex (Schultz, 2000; Schultz et al., 2000). This modulation is facilitated by the neurotransmitter dopamine, produced in the VTA, which is known to be involved with processing both natural and conditioned rewards. Interestingly, ventromedial regions of the VTA and substantia nigra compacta (SNc) may be involved in coding value, while dorsolateral regions of the SNc may be involved in coding motivational salience (Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2009). From the VTA, dopamine signals are most strongly projected to the ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens (NAc). The NAc in turn is thought to encode reward signals from the VTA. While striatal neurons are thought to be involved in reward learning, they do not seem to encode the specific reward. By contrast, orbital and medial prefrontal (OMPFC) neurons seem to process the specific nature of the reward (Schultz, 2000; Schultz et al., 2000). In the vmPFC, they may also be involved in the assessment and assignment of the personal value of the stimulus (Kim and Johnson, 2013).

While OMPFC neurons encode value, they are not modulated by the location in space a reward is given or the motor response associated with a reward. This is in contrast to other value processing brain regions, in which value modulates activity related to sensory or motor processes (Conen and Padoa-Schioppa, 2016). Thus indirect reward processing could modulate sensorimotor regions. For example, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) represents quantitative reward prediction errors (Amiez et al., 2006; Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2007; Seo and Lee, 2007) especially of actions (Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2007). Thus, its activity is closely tied to action selection and it may therefore be an important component for action observation as well. The anticipation of a large reward also triggers strong neuronal activation patterns in motor networks (Roesch and Olson, 2003, 2004, 2007; Wallis and Kennerley, 2010). The frontal eye fields, and the premotor cortex modulate attentional resources using the reward signal (Wallis and Kennerley, 2010) as do parietal regions (Platt and Glimcher, 1999). With regards to the premotor cortex, the study by Caggiano et al. (2012) on mirror neurons indicate this is true regardless of whether the reward is for the self or for another person. Again though, the direct pathways between mirror neuron regions and reward processing regions remains to be further explored, with current data showing indirect pathways to motor areas (Haber et al., 2000). As we delineate in Figure 3, such indirect pathways could allow not only reward signals to modulate the MNS, but also other factors such as salience, emotion and other cognitive processes. With regard to salience, while parts of the striatum code for this factor (Matsumoto and Hikosaka, 2009) and saliency is an important factor for processing one stimulus over another, it should be distinguished by motivational value, which is a separate factor. That is to say, all valuable stimuli may be salient (and thus chosen for further processing), but not all salient stimuli are valuable.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Integrating the proposed value-driven model with the Bayesian model of mirror neuron system (MNS) processing. Following processing in visual brain regions, information flows along established MNS pathways (Iacoboni, 2005) as well as along emotion processing/salience regions (anterior insula, anterior cingulate cortex [ACC], amygdala) and reward processing regions (substantia nigra reticulate [SNr], substantia nigra compacta [SNc], ventral tegmental area [VTA], ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens [NAc]). Here we integrate Bayesian models, which include prediction error signals (green arrows), and generative model processing (blue arrows, Kilner et al., 2007), with emotion and salience processing (pink arrows) and reward processing (red arrows). Indirect reward processing is depicted with dashed red lines. All of these processes modulate the MNS. The parietal MNS is thought to include the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) while the frontal MNS is thought to include the ventral premotor cortex (vPMC) and the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). STS = superior temporal sulcus. For brevity, we don’t include every region of salient, emotion and reward systems, only primary nodes most likely to be directly related to the MNS. We also note that components of the reward system also process saliency, as discussed in “Toward an Explanatory Model” section.

In our view, while the Kilner and Friston’s prediction model (also utilized by Cross et al., 2012) is an elegant one, and provides an excellent framework to understand the MNS, there is a need to understand the MNS circuit with broader networks that are tied with emotion processing, reward systems, saliency and other networks within which they are commonly seen (Caggiano et al., 2012). Here, we specifically propose a modification to the model such that preferred predictive models are those that are meaningful and valued. Indeed one study found increased activity in the caudate, as well as typical MNS regions, when participants had to predict another person’s future actions from more ambiguous prior observed actions, and the authors argued that ambiguous actions which need to be learned may be tied with more reward processing (Diersch et al., 2013). Pairing a value model with the prediction models may outcome as a tetrahedron model that combines the cubic model of Gardner et al. (2017) and adds to the dimension of value-driven modulation, though such a model is probably still underestimating the complexity added by other dimensions, such as valence and saliency. The need for Bayesian theories of brain processing to incorporate value, emotion and valence processing has previously been expressed by other researchers (Joffily and Coricelli, 2013).

Relation of the Value Model to Human Data

We extend this value-driven model from the monkey data to better explain discrepant activation patterns in multiple shared circuits in the human data, such that a unified model may explain reported activation patterns from previous studies as a function of value. Below we discuss findings from shared circuit research and explain their results from a value driven perspective. We focus here particularly on the MNS and pain matrix, as there is more data on these shared circuits than the others. Here we organize shared circuit studies along three categories: (1) observation of significant others; (2) observation of action familiarity; and (3) observations of actions to be learned.

Observation of Significant or Insignificant Others

Depending on the situation, observing either a loved one (Cheng et al., 2010) or an enemy (Fox et al., 2013) in pain can be equally meaningful and valuable for the observer and elicit more activity in the underlying shared circuits (Cheng et al., 2010; Fox et al., 2013). For example, if you are being pursued by an enemy, seeing them trip over and crouch with pain is highly relevant information to process, perhaps more so than observing a neutral or liked individual in pain (e.g., your child). In other circumstances, observing your child slam their finger in a door is the most relevant information to process. Thus, the significance or valence of a relationship cannot be the key-determining factor, but rather the meaning and value of the observed experience. Similarly, watching someone of the same race or a different race can be equally valuable information to an observer depending on the background, situation and circumstance of the people involved. Therefore, while some have previously argued that shared circuits are more active for those “like me” (Molnar-Szakacs and Uddin, 2013) we argue instead that activation patterns may be better explained by the value of the observed action and actor to the self, which depends on goals, relationships, personal history, context, environment and motivational state. Such a model may be able to explain the data better than the “like me” model which is built purely on motoric or physical similarity and familiarity. This could explain why in some cases individuals very different from the self can activate the MNS (Aziz-Zadeh et al., 2012; Fox et al., 2013), while in other cases more similar others activate the system (Calvo-Merino et al., 2005, 2006; Bangert et al., 2006). Furthermore, we note that the “like me” model has an implicit value consideration. Thus, the “like me” model may be simplified by thinking instead in terms of value.

In human studies, the value assigned to others is often determined quantitatively by comparing the activity elicited by viewing close family members in contrast to strangers. However, the complexity of human relationships makes interpreting quantitative data in regard to the value of a given individual difficult. In studies comparing self vs. non-self faces, researchers typically find differences in the bilateral and right IFG (Uddin et al., 2005; Heckendorf et al., 2016). However, when comparing personally familiar vs. unfamiliar faces (i.e., personally familiar faces vs. famous faces or one’s own child vs. a familiar but unrelated child), findings often indicate activity in the anterior paracingulate cortex, posterior superior temporal sulcus and precuneous suggesting personal knowledge retrieval (Gobbini and Haxby, 2007). The majority of studies do not report MNS brain region activation when comparing familiar and unfamiliar faces (Ida Gobbini et al., 2004; Leibenluft et al., 2004; for review, with a few exceptions, see Natu and O’Toole, 2011). This may be because they focus on face perception rather than action observation. However other studies do find IFG activation; Ishai et al. (2002) reported IFG activation during familiar (famous) vs. unfamiliar face processing and Taylor et al. (2009) reported similar results when individuals viewed their partner’s faces compared to a stranger’s.

Discrepant findings and tasks make the synthesis of the results difficult to interpret. In some instances, it appears that the IFG is sensitive not only to self-recognition (Kaplan et al., 2008) but also to the perceived closeness or value, such as observing a partner or an important political or social figure (Shah et al., 2001; Ishai et al., 2002; Taylor et al., 2009). However, these studies are not sufficient to detangle the “like me” from a “value” model since information about value was not collected from participants. Furthermore, “like me” characteristics can be correlated with value in some situations making these variables difficult to detangle. That is, similarity may be confounded by value (the more similar, the more value; (Mitchell et al., 2011; Tamir and Mitchell, 2013). Perhaps a way to disentangle the value model from the “like me” model is to consider a study where you compare situations in which you judge characteristics of similar vs. dissimilar others as well as features you value. For example, you may be similar with another person in that you are both smokers, but you consider smoking to be a negative value. By contrast, you may be similar with another person in that you both have athletic physiques, and you value this physique very much. Future studies such as this need to be explored.

Nevertheless, dissimilar to the “like me” model, the value driven model could explain reported findings of increased activity in MNS regions for both familiar (Hadjikhani et al., 2007; Avenanti et al., 2010; Liew et al., 2011) and unfamiliar individuals (Qin and Northoff, 2011; Aziz-Zadeh et al., 2012). Furthermore it could explain discrepant findings such as why some reports indicate increased activity in MNS regions for human agents than robotic or non-human agents (Tai et al., 2004; Costantini et al., 2005; Engel et al., 2008; Chaminade et al., 2010; Miura et al., 2010; Shimada, 2010) while others find more activity for robotic as compared to human agents (Cross et al., 2012, etc.). With regard to familiar individuals, in a study by Losin et al. (2012), individuals observed and imitated an unfamiliar hand action performed by actors of different races. The researchers found that neural activity during imitation was modulated by race of the actor that participants were imitating. Specifically, more activity was elicited in Caucasian and Asian participants when observing African American actors than any other race, including their own. The authors suggested that perceived social status (i.e., African Americans having the lowest perceived social status; Fiske et al., 1999) might be reflected in neural activity during imitation. In other words, activity was modulated by the perceived value of the actor and not by similarity to the participant (“like me” model). A follow up study by the same group tested this theory by recruiting African American subjects to participate in the same experiment. Indeed, African American and European Americans activated MNS regions more when observing African American actors than when observing European Americans ones (Losin et al., 2014; see also Avenanti et al., 2010). The authors posit that social status, rather than racial similarity, is responsible for this racial modulation during observation. This theory of perceived social status supports a “value” driven model of the MNS such that perceived value of an individual is driving activity in this system. Finally, such a model would predict that socially relevant stimuli, like individuals facing toward the observer rather than away from the observer, should show modulation in the MNS (Kilner et al., 2007). We note that in these examples, it is possible that value can interact with saliency and both factors are important for increased processing in the MNS (see Figure 3).

Observation of Action Familiarity

Calvo-Merino et al. (2005) reported that ballet dancers show increased activation in the MNS when watching ballet as compared to martial arts than those with no ballet expertise. Indeed, a number of studies indicate increased MNS activity for observed actions that are more familiar to the viewer or that the viewer has expertise in performing (Buccino et al., 2004; Järveläinen et al., 2004; Calvo-Merino et al., 2005, 2006; Cross et al., 2006; Wright et al., 2010). For example, when participants think they are observing the actions of another human being as opposed to a robot or inanimate agent, there have been reports of increased MNS activity (Wheatley et al., 2007) and motor priming. As we previously mentioned, this has led some people to propose that action familiarity drives activation in the MNS (see Figure 1).

Increased shared circuit activation for familiar actions is also observed in other sensory domains such as the auditory domain (Ricciardi et al., 2009). In a study by Ricciardi et al. (2009), congenitally blind individuals activated a premotor–temporoparietal cortical network in response to the sounds of actions (e.g., hammering). These regions overlapped both with MNS areas found in sighted participants in response to seeing or hearing an action, and with the brain response elicited by motor pantomime of the same actions. Furthermore, the MNS showed significantly greater activity to motor familiarity (actions previously performed by an individual) than to unfamiliar action sounds in both sighted and blind individuals.

However, familiarity with the action does not always correspond to more MNS activity. Some studies indicate no significant differences in key MNS regions when participants viewed human vs. robotic hand actions (Gazzola et al., 2007), or hands vs. geometric objects making goal directed actions (Ramsey and Hamilton, 2010). In fact some studies find increased MNS activation when observing non-human (e.g., robots) compared to human actions (Cross et al., 2012; Saygin and Stadler, 2012). Furthermore, it has been found that observing actions made by a limb that you do not have (e.g., a residual limb in an amputee) activates the MNS more than a limb that you do have (e.g., a hand; Liew et al., 2011). Thus, action familiarity cannot explain these discrepant results. However, in a value model, this discrepancy could be explained by looking at the value to the viewer behind the observed actions. In the first study discussed here by Calvo-Merino et al. (2005), it is likely that a ballet dancer would likely find observing ballet more “valuable” or meaningful than observing martial arts. Furthermore, learning the kinematics of a novel effector (e.g., a residual limb), or a novel agent (a robot) performing a dance, may be more meaningful. Furthermore, deciphering the underlying mechanisms of action understanding becomes more difficult when familiarly with actions and actors interact. The interplay between familiar and unfamiliar actions performed by similar or non-similar raced actors can elicit multiple neural systems as complexity increases. Activity in MNS and mentalizing regions (i.e., temporal-parietal junction [TPJ]) has been observed to increases when European American and Chinese participants view similar race actors performing actions compared to non-similar actors as well as when viewing unfamiliar hand actions compared to familiar actions in the same individuals (Van Overwalle and Baetens, 2009; Liew et al., 2011). Depending on the complexity of the situation and level of individual engagement, it is thought that multiple networks contribute differently to the understanding of actions (Liew et al., 2011). A “like me”, “familiarity”, or even a saliency model cannot explain the dynamic activation patterns seen in these studies, where a value driven model may be better able. The MNS activates with increased familiarity and unfamiliarity depending on the task and individual engagement. The value of the observed action does not necessitate that the individual be familiar with the actor or action.

Indeed, familiarity may have at least two effects: reduction of uncertainty (e.g., in action execution), and facilitation of automatic responses (e.g., motor repertoire for ballet dancers). Reduction of uncertainty interacts with value in that individuals prefer familiar contexts to unfamiliar ones (ambiguity aversion, discounting unfamiliar contexts due to entropy, Calvo-Merino et al., 2006; Gazzola et al., 2007). Thus, in some cases, higher activation in the MNS for familiar observed actions can be explained because it is more valued and because it activates automatically. We explore how novel actions may show increased MNS activity in the “Observation of Actions That Need to be Learned or Relearned” section.

Observation of Actions That Need to Be Learned or Relearned

We extend “value” to also incorporate the need for motor or social learning. Clearly, if there is a need to learn an observed action, then there is more value in observing that action and we would expect increased activity in the mirror system. Thus, as mentioned earlier, individuals who have different bodies than ourselves (e.g., an individual with an amputated limb) are people whose bodies we need to learn more about. Hence, some results show that there is more MNS activity in typically developing individuals when observing individuals performing actions with amputated limbs (Liew et al., 2013). Furthermore, some studies have shown that observing actions with novel kinematics also yields increased activity in the MNS as compared to more familiar actions (Cross et al., 2012). Observed actions that are unfamiliar to individuals compared to familiar actions can preferentially engage the MNS when performed by nonconspecifics. These findings can be understood in terms of the value model. In some situations, understanding the actions and intentions of action regardless of who is performing it is more valuable when the goal of the action is important, such as when an action is being learned. Indeed, goal-directed actions recruit shared circuit networks more than ambiguous or non-goal directed actions (Iacoboni et al., 2005). Similarly, a stroke patient finds more value in observing an actor’s counterpart to the paretic hand rather than the non-paretic hand perform actions (Garrison et al., 2013). Again, it may be more valuable to understand the actions of a paretic hand over the non-paretic depending on the environment and situation.

Relevance to Clinical Populations

To conclude the review, we speculate that in some instances, clinical cases may arise from abnormal function between value processing and sensorimotor processing. Here we will specifically consider data on individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD; Dapretto et al., 2006; Williams, 2008; Kana et al., 2011). Research suggests that the MNS may be important for understanding ASD for several reasons. First, extensive research has identified impairments in imitation and motor ability in individuals with ASD (Rogers et al., 2003; Mostofsky et al., 2006; Williams et al., 2006; Williams, 2008; Vanvuchelen et al., 2011). A recent meta-analysis and a systematic review concluded that motor coordination deficits, including but not limited to imitation, are present in individuals with ASD (even in studies that controlled for age and IQ), and that this should be considered a cardinal feature of ASD (Williams et al., 2004; Fournier et al., 2010). Based on evidence that the MNS is strongly involved in imitation (Koski et al., 2002; Heiser et al., 2003; Iacoboni, 2005), it has been proposed that individuals with ASD who have imitation deficits (Williams et al., 2004) may also have a deficit in the MNS (Dapretto et al., 2006; Williams et al., 2006; Oberman and Ramachandran, 2007), potentially along with deficits in other networks. Several studies support this hypothesis, showing differential MNS functioning in individuals with ASD as compared to typically developing individuals (Dapretto et al., 2006; Williams, 2008; Kana et al., 2011). However, a few behavioral studies have indicated no imitation deficits, or “hyper imitation” deficits, in high functioning individuals with ASD, and no differential MNS functioning in individuals with ASD (Hamilton et al., 2007; Press et al., 2010; Spengler et al., 2010). These discrepant findings can arise from a number of factors. Some of the latter studies used adult populations (Press et al., 2010; Spengler et al., 2010), which is problematic because the one study that examined the effects of age on the AON found increased activity in this system as a function of age in individuals with ASD (Bastiaansen et al., 2011). Other studies that failed to find between-group differences had small sample sizes (Avikainen et al., 1999) or used many ASD subjects with autism impairment scores in the normal range (Raymaekers et al., 2009). Heterogeneity of symptomology in ASD may also be an important factor to consider.

However, if a MNS deficit in ASD is true for at least a subgroup of individuals with ASD, it may be that impairments arise not just from abnormalities in the MNS regions, but also between interactions between the MNS circuit and the reward circuits. Indeed, a few studies have shown that individuals with ASD have an impairment in processing social rewards as compared to monetary rewards (Lin et al., 2012), and social as compared to monetary reward learning is associated with decreased frontostriatal activations in ASD (Scott-Van Zeeland et al., 2010). Furthermore, mutations in genes within the mesolimbic dopamine pathway have been linked to ASD (Hettinger et al., 2008; Staal, 2015), as have mutations in the dopamine transporter (DAT; Bowton et al., 2014). Indeed, the social motivation theory of ASD (Dawson et al., 2005) suggests that children with ASD do not find social stimuli rewarding. In other words, children with ASD do not value or find the same social information rewarding to the same extent as typically developing children. Therefore, if individuals with ASD do not value the same stimuli, then according to our proposed model, they would elicit less activity in MNS regions. In fact, the value driven model fits with results suggesting that in children with ASD, MNS activity and sociality traits (as measured by subsets of established social assessments, e.g., Social Responsiveness Scale; Constantino et al., 2003) are positively related (Dapretto et al., 2006). Children who have higher scores in sociality find social reward stimuli more valuable, and therefore would have higher levels of MNS activation. Indeed, a recent study in a non-clinical group of participants indicated that individuals with more autistic traits assigned less value to viewing human bodies in natural motion compared to human bodies in robot-like motion or non-human control motion (Williams and Cross, 2018). Future studies comparing the subjective value of social and nonsocial stimuli would be useful to determine the relationship between ASD, the MNS, and value.

In general, more research on MNS development is needed. While some researchers suggest that the MNS is present at birth (Lepage and Théoret, 2007), there is only indirect evidence of this. In adults, several studies posit that during action observation and execution, MNS activity may be detected by desynchronization of the electroencephalogram (EEG) mu rhythm recorded over motor regions (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2004; Ferrari et al., 2012). Similar EEG results have been found with human infants (Southgate et al., 2010; Marshall and Meltzoff, 2011; Nyström et al., 2011) and monkey infants (Ferrari et al., 2012) during action observation and execution. Furthermore, data indicate that mu desynchronization during action observation is predicted by the infant’s own motor abilities (van Elk et al., 2008). Thus MNS activity is modulated by development (Woodward and Gerson, 2014). A previous study indicated that the mu rhythm in infants is sensitive to action goals (Southgate et al., 2010) and the researchers argue that this allows infants to predict the outcomes of other people’s actions (Southgate et al., 2009, 2010). However, the degree to which this activation pattern is related to social cognitive and reward functioning remains unknown. Future longitudinal studies may focus on integrating social behavioral data with neural data on the MNS during development. Indeed abnormalities in the development of social abilities has been correlated with less activity in the MNS (Dapretto et al., 2006), and a longitudinal study may better decipher if there is a directionality to the data. This would then have implications for clinical cases as well.

Finally, more work on structural cortical changes to MNS regions through development are needed. Specifically, the prefrontal cortical thickness progresses in an inverted U-shaped pattern from infancy to one’s mid-20s (Sowell et al., 2002). It is likely that these structural changes in the IFG reflect differing activation patterns in the MNS and can be linked with behavioral development. In addition, neural connectivity to other brain systems, such as the reward network, salience system and emotion processing systems are likely to change throughout development. This may be reflected in differences found between mu desynchronization in infants compared to adults (Marshall and Meltzoff, 2011). Linking the structural brain, EEG and behavioral data together will be important for better understanding the MNS and its connections with other neural systems (e.g., reward systems, saliency network, etc.) through development and potentially allow for a better understanding of clinical disorders.

Conclusion

In sum, previous theories have yet to satisfy current literature findings. Here we propose adapting the “value-driven” model of the MNS proposed for monkeys to humans. Specifically, the role of the dopamine reward circuit and emotion processing in modulating MNS activity needs to be an important part of any future model of the MNS, or other shared circuits. After reviewing previous findings through the lens of this new model’s framework, we find to better explain discrepant activation patterns in multiple shared circuits in the human data.

Further research should be conducted to test if indeed this model can explain reported activation patterns from previous studies as a function of value. In order to test this model, one would need to be able to determine “value” of a stimulus to an individual. So logically then, to account for previous discrepant results, one would need to show that the cohorts in the different studies “valued” things differently. With a value driven model, one would predict that this type of comparison would result in a positive correlation between activity in the MNS, reward system and the value an individual contributed to a given condition or stimuli. Future studies are needed to further explore this hypothesis.

Author Contributions

LA-Z conceived the review article and wrote the first draft of the manuscript with EK. GC edited and contributed to sections of the manuscript.

Funding

Research reported in this publication was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R01HD079432. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Kathleen Garrison, Sook-Lei Liew, Sharon Cermak, and S. Butterfill for thoughtful comments and discussion during the development of this manuscript.

References

Amiez, C., Joseph, J. P., and Procyk, E. (2006). Reward encoding in the monkey anterior cingulate cortex. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1040–1055. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avenanti, A., Bueti, D., Galati, G., and Aglioti, S. M. (2005). Transcranial magnetic stimulation highlights the sensorimotor side of empathy for pain. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 955–960. doi: 10.1038/nn1481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avenanti, A., Sirigu, A., and Aglioti, S. M. (2010). Report racial bias reduces empathic sensorimotor resonance with other-race pain. Curr. Biol. 20, 1018–1022. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avikainen, S., Kulomäki, T., and Hari, R. (1999). Normal movement reading in Asperger subjects. Neuroreport 10, 3467–3470. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199911260-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aziz-Zadeh, L. (2013). “Embodied semantics for language related to actions: a review of fMRI and neuropsychological research,” in Language and Action in Cognitive Neuroscience, eds Y. Coello and A. Bartolo (East Sussex: Psychology Press), 273–283.

Google Scholar

Aziz-Zadeh, L., Sheng, T., Liew, S. L., and Damasio, H. (2012). Understanding otherness: the neural bases of action comprehension and pain empathy in a congenital amputee. Cereb. Cortex 22, 811–819. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bangert, M., Peschel, T., Schlaug, G., Rotte, M., Drescher, D., Hinrichs, H., et al. (2006). Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists: evidence from fMRI conjunction. Neuroimage 30, 917–926. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastiaansen, J. A., Thioux, M., Nanetti, L., van der Gaag, C., Ketelaars, C., Minderaa, R., et al. (2011). Age-related increase in inferior frontal gyrus activity and social functioning in autism spectrum disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 832–838. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beudel, M., Zijlstra, S., Mulder, T., Zijdewind, I., and de Jong, B. M. (2011). Secondary sensory area SII is crucially involved in the preparation of familiar movements compared to movements never made before. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 564–579. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowton, E., Saunders, C., Reddy, I. A., Campbell, N. G., Hamilton, P. J., Henry, L. K., et al. (2014). SLC6A3 coding variant Ala559Val found in two autism probands alters dopamine transporter function and trafficking. Transl. Psychiatry 4:e464. doi: 10.1038/tp.2014.90

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Lui, F., Canessa, N., Patteri, I., Lagravinese, G., Benuzzi, F., et al. (2004). Neural circuits involved in the recognition of actions performed by nonconspecifics: an fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 114–126. doi: 10.1162/089892904322755601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bufalari, I., Aprile, T., Avenanti, A., Di Russo, F., Aglioti, S. M., Psicologia, D., et al. (2007). Empathy for pain and touch in the human somatosensory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2553–2561. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caggiano, V., Fogassi, L., Rizzolatti, G., Casile, A., Giese, M. A., and Thier, P. (2012). Mirror neurons encode the subjective value of an observed action. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 109, 11848–11853. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1205553109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., and Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1243–1249. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., Haggard, P., Grezes, J., et al. (2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Curr. Biol. 16, 1905–1910. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaminade, T., Zecca, M., Blakemore, S. J., Takanishi, A., Frith, C. D., Micera, S., et al. (2010). Brain response to a humanoid robot in areas implicated in the perception of human emotional gestures. PLoS One 5:e11577. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, Y., Chen, C., Lin, C.-P., Chou, K.-H., and Decety, J. (2010). Love hurts: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 51, 923–929. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conen, K. E., and Padoa-Schioppa, C. (2016). The dynamic nature of value-based decisions. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 866–867. doi: 10.1038/nn.4329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Constantino, J. N., Davis, S. A., Todd, R. D., Schindler, M. K., Gross, M. M., Brophy, S. L., et al. (2003). Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 33, 427–433. doi: 10.1023/A:1025014929212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coricelli, G. (2005). Two-levels of mental states attribution: from automaticity to voluntariness. Neuropsychologia 43, 294–300. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costantini, M., Galati, G., Ferretti, A., Caulo, M., Tartaro, A., Romani, G. L., et al. (2005). Neural systems underlying observation of humanly impossible movements: an fMRI study. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1761–1767. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F. D. C., and Grafton, S. T. (2006). Building a motor simulation de novo: observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage 31, 1257–1267. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F. D. C., Kraemer, D. J. M., Kelley, W. M., and Grafton, S. T. (2009). Dissociable substrates for body motion and physical experience in the human action observation network. Eur. J. Neurosci. 30, 1383–1392. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06941.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross, E. S., Liepelt, R., Hamilton, H. F., Parkinson, J., Ramsey, R., Stadler, W., et al. (2012). Robotic movement preferentially engages the action observation network. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 2238–2254. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross, E. S., Schmitt, P. J., and Grafton, S. T. (2007). Neural substrates of contextual interference during motor learning support a model of active preparation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1854–1871. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.11.1854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 28–30. doi: 10.1038/nn1611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., and McPartland, J. (2005). Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Dev. Neuropsychol. 27, 403–424. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2703_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Cesare, G., Di Dio, C., Marchi, M., and Rizzolatti, G. (2015). Expressing our internal states and understanding those of others. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 112, 10331–10335. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1512133112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diersch, N., Mueller, K., Cross, E. S., Stadler, W., Rieger, M., and Schütz-Bosbach, S. (2013). Action prediction in younger versus older adults: neural correlates of motor Familiarity. PLoS One 8:e64195. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, A., Burke, M., Fiehler, K., Bien, S., and Rosler, F. (2008). How moving objects become animated: the human mirror neuron system assimilates non-biological movement patterns. Soc. Neurosci. 3, 368–387. doi: 10.1080/17470910701612793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, P. F., Vanderwert, R. E., Paukner, A., Bower, S., Suomi, S. J., and Fox, N. A. (2012). Distinct EEG amplitude suppression to facial gestures as evidence for a mirror mechanism in newborn monkeys. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 1165–1172. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T., Xu, J., Cuddy, A. C., and Glick, P. (1999). (Dis)respecting versus (Dis)liking: status and interdependence predict ambivalent stereotypes of competence and warmth. J. Soc. Issues 55, 473–489. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fournier, K. A., Hass, C. J., Naik, S. K., Lodha, N., and Cauraugh, J. H. (2010). Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: a synthesis and meta-analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 1227–1240. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0981-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, G. R., Sobhani, M., and Aziz-Zadeh, L. (2013). Witnessing hateful people in pain modulates brain activity in regions associated with physical pain and reward. Front. Psychol. 4:772. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallese, V., and Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the mind-reading. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 493–501. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01262-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, T., Aglinskas, A., and Cross, E. S. (2017). Using guitar learning to probe the Action Observation Network’s response to visuomotor familiarity. Neuroimage 156, 174–189. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, T., Goulden, N., and Cross, E. S. (2015). Dynamic modulation of the action observation network by movement familiarity. J. Neurosci. 35, 1561–1572. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2942-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrison, K. A., Aziz-Zadeh, L., Wong, S. W., Liew, S. L., and Winstein, C. J. (2013). Modulating the motor system by action observation after stroke. Stroke 44, 2247–2253. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.113.001105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gazzola, V., Rizzolatti, G., Wicker, B., and Keysers, C. (2007). The anthropomorphic brain: the mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions. Neuroimage 35, 1674–1684. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gobbini, M. I., and Haxby, J. V. (2007). Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces. Neuropsychologia 45, 32–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossmann, T., Cross, E. S., Ticini, L. F., Daum, M. M., and Luca, F. (2013). Action observation in the infant brain: the role of body form and motion Action observation in the infant brain: the role of body form and motion. Soc. Neurosci. 8, 22–30. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2012.696077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haber, S. N., Fudge, J. L., and McFarland, N. R. (2000). Striatonigrostriatal pathways in primates form an ascending spiral from the shell to the dorsolateral striatum. J. Neurosci. 20, 2369–2382. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-06-02369.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. H., Snyder, J., and Tager-Flusberg, H. (2007). Abnormal activation of the social brain during face perception in autism. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 441–449. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, A. F. D. C., Brindley, R. M., and Frith, U. (2007). Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: how valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? Neuropsychologia 45, 1859–1868. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heckendorf, E., Huffmeijer, R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2016). Neural processing of familiar and unfamiliar children’s faces: effects of experienced love withdrawal, but no effects of neutral and threatening priming. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:231. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heiser, M., Iacoboni, M., Maeda, F., Marcus, J., and Mazziotta, J. C. (2003). The essential role of Broca’s area in imitation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17, 1123–1128. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02530.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hettinger, J. A., Liu, X., Schwartz, C. E., Michaelis, R. C., and Holden, J. J. A. (2008). A DRD1 haplotype is associated with risk for autism spectrum disorders in male-only affected sib-pair families. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 147, 628–636. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iacoboni, M. (2005). Neural mechanisms of imitation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 632–637. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., and Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biol. 3:e79. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ida Gobbini, M., Leibenluft, E., Santiago, N., and Haxby, J. V. (2004). Social and emotional attachment in the neural representation of faces. Neuroimage 22, 1628–1635. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishai, A., Haxby, J. V., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Visual imagery of famous faces: effects of memory and attention revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage 17, 1729–1741. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, P. L., Rainville, P., and Decety, J. (2006). To what extent do we share the pain of others? Insight from the neural bases of pain empathy. Pain 125, 5–9. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Järveläinen, J., Schürmann, M., and Hari, R. (2004). Activation of the human primary motor cortex during observation of tool use. Neuroimage 23, 187–192. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joffily, M., and Coricelli, G. (2013). Emotional valence and the free-energy principle. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9:e1003094. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kana, R. K., Wadsworth, H. M., and Travers, B. G. (2011). A systems level analysis of the mirror neuron hypothesis and imitation impairments in autism spectrum disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 894–902. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, J. T., Aziz-Zadeh, L., Uddin, L. Q., and Iacoboni, M. (2008). The self across the senses: an fMRI study of self-face and self-voice recognition. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 3, 218–223. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keysers, C., and Gazzola, V. (2009). Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions, emotions, and sensations. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 666–671. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keysers, C., Kaas, J. H., and Gazzola, V. (2010). Somatosensation in social perception. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 417–428. doi: 10.1038/nrn2833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keysers, C., Wicker, B., Gazzola, V., Anton, J.-L., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (2004). A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch. Neuron 42, 335–346. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00156-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilner, J. M., Friston, K. J., and Frith, C. D. (2007). Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system. Cogn. Process. 8, 159–166. doi: 10.1007/s10339-007-0170-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, K., and Johnson, M. K. (2013). Activity in ventromedial prefrontal cortex during self-related processing: positive subjective value or personal significance? Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 494–500. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohler, E. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons. Science 297, 846–848. doi: 10.1126/science.1070311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koski, L., Wohlschläger, A., Bekkering, H., Woods, R. P., Dubeau, M. C., Mazziotta, J. C., et al. (2002). Modulation of motor and premotor activity during imitation of target-directed actions. Cereb. Cortex 12, 847–855. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.8.847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lannetti, G. D., and Mouraux, A. (2010). From the neuromatrix to the pain matrix (and back). Exp. Brain Res. 205, 1–12. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2340-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leibenluft, E., Gobbini, M. I., Harrison, T., and Haxby, J. V. (2004). Mothers’ neural activation in response to pictures of their children and other children. Biol. Psychiatry 56, 225–232. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lepage, J. F., and Théoret, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system: grasping others’ actions from birth? Dev. Sci. 10, 513–523. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00631.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liepelt, R., Ullsperger, M., Obst, K., Spengler, S., von Cramon, D. Y., and Brass, M. (2009). Contextual movement constraints of others modulate motor preparation in the observer. Neuropsychologia 47, 268–275. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liew, S. L., Han, S., and Aziz-Zadeh, L. (2011). Familiarity modulates mirror neuron and mentalizing regions during intention understanding. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1986–1997. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liew, S. L., Sheng, T., and Aziz-Zadeh, L. (2013). Experience with an amputee modulates one’s own sensorimotor response during action observation. Neuroimage 69, 138–145. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, A., Rangel, A., and Adolphs, R. (2012). Impaired learning of social compared to monetary rewards in autism. Front. Neurosci. 6:143. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Losin, E. A. R., Cross, K. A., Iacoboni, M., and Dapretto, M. (2014). Neural processing of race during imitation: self-similarity versus social status. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 1723–1739. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Losin, E. A. R., Iacoboni, M., Martin, A., Cross, K. A., and Dapretto, M. (2012). Race modulates neural activity during imitation. Neuroimage 59, 3594–3603. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, P. J., and Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). Neural mirroring systems: exploring the EEG μ rhythm in human infancy. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 1, 110–123. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2010.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, M., and Hikosaka, O. (2007). Lateral habenula as a source of negative reward signals in dopamine neurons. Nature 447, 1111–1115. doi: 10.1038/nature05860

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, M., and Hikosaka, O. (2009). Two types of dopamine neuron distinctly convey positive and negative motivational signals. Nature 459, 837–841. doi: 10.1038/nature08028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, K. (2011). Primary sensory cortices, top-down projections and conscious experience. Prog. Neurobiol. 94, 408–417. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miall, R. C., and Wolpert, D. M. (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw. 9, 1265–1279. doi: 10.1016/s0893-6080(96)00035-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, J. P., Schirmer, J., Ames, D. L., and Gilbert, D. T. (2011). Medial prefrontal cortex predicts intertemporal choice. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 857–866. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miura, N., Sugiura, M., Takahashi, M., Sassa, Y., Miyamoto, A., Sato, S., et al. (2010). Effect of motion smoothness on brain activity while observing a dance: an fMRI study using a humanoid robot. Soc. Neurosci. 5, 40–58. doi: 10.1080/17470910903083256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molnar-Szakacs, I., and Uddin, L. Q. (2013). Self-processing and the default mode network: interactions with the mirror neuron system. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:571. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mostofsky, S. H., Dubey, P., Jerath, V. K., Jansiewicz, E. M., Goldberg, M. C., and Denckla, M. B. (2006). Developmental dyspraxia is not limited to imitation in children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 12, 314–326. doi: 10.1017/s1355617706060437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Johnson, B. W., and McNair, N. A. (2004). Mu rhythm modulation during observation of an object-directed grasp. Cogn. Brain Res. 19, 195–201. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Natu, V., and O’Toole, A. J. (2011). The neural processing of familiar and unfamiliar faces: a review and synopsis. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 726–747. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02053.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neal, A., and Kilner, J. M. (2012). Europe PMC funders group what is simulated in the action observation network when we observe actions? Eur. J. Neurosci. 32, 1765–1770. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07435.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyström, P., Ljunghammar, T., Rosander, K., and von Hofsten, C. (2011). Using mu rhythm desynchronization to measure mirror neuron activity in infants. Dev. Sci. 14, 327–335. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00979.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oberman, L. M., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The simulating social mind: the role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychol. Bull. 133, 310–327. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.2.310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Platt, M. L., and Glimcher, P. W. (1999). Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal cortex. Nature 400, 233–238. doi: 10.1038/22268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Press, C., Richardson, D., and Bird, G. (2010). Intact imitation of emotional facial actions in autism spectrum conditions. Neuropsychologia 48, 3291–3297. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qin, P., and Northoff, G. (2011). How is our self related to midline regions and the default-mode network? Neuroimage 57, 1221–1233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 98, 676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramsey, R., and Hamilton, A. F. D. C. (2010). Triangles have goals too: understanding action representation in left aIPS. Neuropsychologia 48, 2773–2776. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raymaekers, R., Wiersema, J. R., and Roeyers, H. (2009). EEG study of the mirror neuron system in children with high functioning autism. Brain Res. 1304, 113–121. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ricciardi, E., Bonino, D., Sani, L., Vecchi, T., Guazzelli, M., Haxby, J. V., et al. (2009). Do we really need vision? How blind people “See” the actions of others. J. Neurosci. 29, 9719–9724. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0274-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roesch, M. R., and Olson, C. R. (2003). Impact of expected reward on neuronal activity in prefrontal cortex, frontal and supplementary eye fields and premotor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 1766–1789. doi: 10.1152/jn.00019.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roesch, M. R., and Olson, C. R. (2004). Neuronal activity related to reward value and motivation in primate frontal cortex. Science 304, 307–310. doi: 10.1126/science.1093223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roesch, M. R., and Olson, C. R. (2007). Neuronal activity related to anticipated reward in frontal cortex: does it represent value or reflect motivation? Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1121, 431–446. doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., and Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 44, 763–781. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saygin, A. P., and Stadler, W. (2012). The role of appearance and motion in action prediction. Psychol. Res. 76, 388–394. doi: 10.1007/s00426-012-0426-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, W. (2000). Multiple reward signals in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 199–207. doi: 10.1038/35044563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, W., Tremblay, L., and Hollerman, J. R. (2000). Reward processing in primate orbitofrontal cortex and basal ganglia. Cereb. Cortex 10, 272–283. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott-Van Zeeland, A. A., Dapretto, M., Ghahremani, D. G., Poldrack, R. A., and Bookheimer, S. Y. (2010). Reward processing in autism. Autism Res. 3, 53–67. doi: 10.1002/aur.122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seo, H., and Lee, D. (2007). Temporal filtering of reward signals in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex during a mixed-strategy game. J. Neurosci. 27, 8366–8377. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2369-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shah, N. J., Marshall, J. C., Zafiris, O., Schwab, A., Zilles, K., Markowitsch, H. J., et al. (2001). The neural correlates of person familiarity: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study with clinical implications. Brain 124, 804–815. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.4.804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepherd, S. V., Klein, J. T., Deaner, R. O., and Platt, M. L. (2009). Mirroring of attention by neurons in macaque parietal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 9489–9494. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900419106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimada, S. (2010). Deactivation in the sensorimotor area during observation of a human agent performing robotic actions. Brain Cogn. 72, 394–399. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162. doi: 10.1126/science.1093535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2006). Europe PMC Funders Group Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature 439, 466–469. doi: 10.1038/nature04271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Southgate, V., Johnson, M. R., El Karoui, I., and Csibra, G. (2010). Motor system activation reveals infants’ on-line prediction of others’ goals. Psychol. Sci. 21, 355–359. doi: 10.1177/0956797610362058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Southgate, V., Johnson, M. H., Osborne, T., and Csibra, G. (2009). Predictive motor activation during action observation in human infants. Biol. Lett. 5, 769–772. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowell, E. R., Trauner, D. A., Gamst, A., and Jernigan, T. L. (2002). Development of cortical and subcortical brain structures in childhood and adolescence: a structural MRI study. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 44, 4–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2002.tb00253.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spengler, S., Bird, G., and Brass, M. (2010). Hyperimitation of actions is related to reduced understanding of others’ minds in autism spectrum conditions. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 1148–1155. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Staal, W. G. (2015). Autism, DRD3 and repetitive and stereotyped behavior, an overview of the current knowledge. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 25, 1421–1426. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tai, Y. F., Scherfler, C., Brooks, D. J., Sawamoto, N., and Castiello, U. (2004). The human premotor cortex is “mirror” only for biological actions. Curr. Biol. 14, 117–120. doi: 10.1016/s0960-9822(04)00006-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamir, D. I., and Mitchell, J. P. (2013). Anchoring and adjustment during social inferences. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142, 151–162. doi: 10.1037/a0028232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, M. J. M., Arsalidou, M., Bayless, S. S. J., Morris, D., Evans, J. W., and Barbeau, E. J. (2009). Neural correlates of personally familiar faces: parents, partner and own faces. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 2008–2020. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tipper, C. M., Signorini, G., and Grafton, S. T. (2015). Body language in the brain: constructing meaning from expressive movement. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:450. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uddin, L. Q., Kaplan, J. T., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Zaidel, E., and Iacoboni, M. (2005). Self-face recognition activates a frontoparietal “mirror” network in the right hemisphere: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 25, 926–935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Elk, M., van Schie, H. T., Hunnius, S., Vesper, C., and Bekkering, H. (2008). You’ll never crawl alone: neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy. Neuroimage 43, 808–814. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Overwalle, F., and Baetens, K. (2009). Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis. Neuroimage 548, 564–584. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanvuchelen, M., Roeyers, H., and De Weerdt, W. (2011). Imitation assessment and its utility to the diagnosis of autism: evidence from consecutive clinical preschool referrals for suspected autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 484–496. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1074-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallis, J. D., and Kennerley, S. W. (2010). Heterogeneous reward signals in prefrontal cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 20, 191–198. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheatley, T., Milleville, S. C., and Martin, A. (2007). Distinct roles for the social network and mirror system. Psychol. Sci. 18, 469–474. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01923.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40, 655–664. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. H. G. (2008). Self-other relations in social development and autism: multiple roles for mirror neurons and other brain bases. Autism Res. 1, 73–90. doi: 10.1002/aur.15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, E. H., and Cross, E. S. (2018). Decreased reward value of biological motion among individuals with autistic traits. Cognition 171, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. H., Waiter, G. D., Gilchrist, A., Perrett, D. I., Murray, A. D., and Whiten, A. (2006). Neural mechanisms of imitation and “mirror neuron” functioning in autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia 44, 610–621. doi: 10.1016/j.neunet.2006.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., and Singh, T. (2004). A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 285–299. doi: 10.1023/b:jadd.0000029551.56735.3a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., and Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 358, 593–602. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodward, A. L., and Gerson, S. A. (2014). Mirroring and the development of action understanding. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 369:20130181. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, M. J., Bishop, D. T., Jackson, R. C., and Abernethy, B. (2010). Functional MRI reveals expert-novice differences during sport-related anticipation. Neuroreport 21, 94–98. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328333dff2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, X., Zuo, X., Wang, X., and Han, S. (2009). Do you feel my pain? Racial group membership modulates empathic neural responses. J. Neurosci. 29, 8525–8529. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2418-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mirror neuron system, motor learning, social cognition, shared neural networks, value-based decision making

Citation: Aziz-Zadeh L, Kilroy E and Corcelli G (2018) Understanding Activation Patterns in Shared Circuits: Toward a Value Driven Model. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:180. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00180

Received: 13 December 2017; Accepted: 17 April 2018;
Published: 07 May 2018.

Edited by:

Lucina Q. Uddin, University of Miami, United States

Reviewed by:

Lei Xu, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, China
Tetsuo Yamamori, RIKEN Brain Science Institute (BSI), Japan

Copyright © 2018 Aziz-Zadeh, Kilroy and Corcelli. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lisa Aziz-Zadeh, lazizzad@usc.edu

Download