Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 27 March 2014
Sec. Neuroendocrine Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Programming HPA-axis by early life experience: Mechanisms of stress susceptibility and adaptation View all 14 articles

Social isolation disrupts hippocampal neurogenesis in young non-human primates

\r\nSimone M. Cinini&#x;Simone M. Cinini1†Gabriela F. Barnabe&#x;Gabriela F. Barnabe1†Nicole Galvo-CoelhoNicole Galvão-Coelho2Magda A. de MedeirosMagda A. de Medeiros3Patrícia Perez-MendesPatrícia Perez-Mendes1Maria B. C. SousaMaria B. C. Sousa2Luciene CovolanLuciene Covolan1Luiz E. Mello*Luiz E. Mello1*
  • 1Departamento de Fisiologia, Universidade Federal de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
  • 2Departamento de Fisiologia, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, Brazil
  • 3Departamento de Ciências Fisiológicas, Universidade Federal Rural do Rio de Janeiro, Seropédica, Rio de Janeiro Brazil

Social relationships are crucial for the development and maintenance of normal behavior in non-human primates. Animals that are raised in isolation develop abnormal patterns of behavior that persist even when they are later reunited with their parents. In rodents, social isolation is a stressful event and is associated with a decrease in hippocampal neurogenesis but considerably less is known about the effects of social isolation in non-human primates during the transition from adolescence to adulthood. To investigate how social isolation affects young marmosets, these were isolated from other members of the colony for 1 or 3 weeks and evaluated for alterations in their behavior and hippocampal cell proliferation. We found that anxiety-related behaviors like scent-marking and locomotor activity increased after social isolation when compared to baseline levels. In agreement, grooming—an indicative of attenuation of tension—was reduced among isolated marmosets. These results were consistent with increased cortisol levels after 1 and 3 weeks of isolation. After social isolation (1 or 3 weeks), reduced proliferation of neural cells in the subgranular zone of dentate granule cell layer was identified and a smaller proportion of BrdU-positive cells underwent neuronal fate (doublecortin labeling). Our data is consistent with the notion that social deprivation during the transition from adolescence to adulthood leads to stress and produces anxiety-like behaviors that in turn might affect neurogenesis and contribute to the deleterious consequences of prolonged stressful conditions.

Introduction

In the adult hippocampus, progenitor cells in the subgranular zone of the dentate gyrus give rise to new neurons that migrate into the granule cell layer, differentiate into granular neurons, and are capable of functional integration into the hippocampal circuitry (Gould and Gross, 2002; Van Praag et al., 2002; Kee et al., 2007). The functional role of hippocampal neurogenesis has not been fully understood until now, but despite the divergent results from different laboratories and models, most data points toward its involvement with specific aspects of learning, conditioning, and spatial information (for review see Balu and Lucki, 2009).

Reduction in hippocampal neurogenesis is associated with stress (Gould et al., 1998) mainly by means of increased excitatory transmission (Gould et al., 1997; Abraham et al., 1998), pro-inflammatory cytokines (Koo and Duman, 2008), diminished neurotrophins expression (Duman and Monteggia, 2006; Jacobsen and Mork, 2006), and glucocorticoid signaling (Wong and Herbert, 2005, 2006). Social isolation is a form of stress, which affects some hippocampal-related functions such as learning and memory and may lead to affective disorders. In marmosets there is a strong exponential negative correlation between the number of dentate proliferating cells and aging where 2–3 years-old animals are considered young adults, from 4 to 7 years they are middle-aged and above 8 years old they are considered old (Bunk et al., 2011). In the present study we used social isolation of young animals as the stressful event (Laudenslager et al., 1995; Stranahan et al., 2006) in order to characterize behavioral consequences of social isolation during the transition phase from adolescence to adulthood, when the animals are at the peak of dentate neurogenesis, so any disturbance might bear a greater relevance in the onset of future mood disorders.

The long-term effects of social isolation among rodent pups include decreased hippocampal neurogenesis, which can culminate in a reduced ability to cope with stressful events in adulthood (Laudenslager et al., 1995; Mirescu et al., 2004; Karten et al., 2005; Stranahan et al., 2006; Rizzi et al., 2007). As compared to rodents, social interactions in primates are considerably more important for the appropriate neuropsychological development (Rosenblum and Andrews, 1994). Marmosets partially deprived of parental care during infancy develop abnormal patterns of behavior that persist even when they are later reunited with their parents (Dettling et al., 2002a,b). In spite of the well-characterized behavioral consequences of social isolation during infancy in these animals, little is known about the neurobiological effects of social isolation during its transition to adulthood. In the present study we investigate the consequences of social isolation in the behavior and hippocampal neurogenesis in these non-human primates.

Methods

Animal Care

All experimental procedures were approved by the Research Ethics Board of the Federal University of São Paulo and by the Ethics Committee of the Department of Physiology of Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte. The collected behavioral data was obtained in the animal facilities from both Universities, while the data for brain analysis were obtained from animals raised in Federal University of São Paulo animal facility. The cortisol and behavioral analysis were held at Federal University of Rio Grande do Norte. We are fully aware that under ideal experimental conditions all animals should have been subjected to exactly the same conditions. Here, in order to maximize the availability of animals, to expand the multiple uses of animals and to minimize the unnecessary deaths of non-human primates we opted to use two separate groups of animals under similar laboratory conditions. Indeed, the similarity in the behavioral repertoire of both groups before and after isolation clearly indicates a similar underlying physiological response to social isolation.

Marmosets (Callithrix sp) bred in both animal facilities (n = 16) were housed in cages, on a standard light/dark cycle (12/12 h). The animals were fed twice a day, around 8:00 am with a protein mixture (powdered milk, corn syrup, eggs, bread, soy bean protein, and bone flour supplemented with vitamins A, D, and E) and around 2:00 pm with a portion of regional tropical fruits. Water was provided ad libitum. At baseline, animals were kept with other members of their families (mother, father, and more than one sibling).

In the São Paulo group, 8–10 months old marmosets (n = 9) had regular weight (3 males, 198 ± 38 g; 6 females, 223 ± 27 g) in the beginning of the experiments. Animals were subdivided in 3 different groups, each containing 2 females and 1 male. Two groups were socially isolated for 1 or 3 weeks, respectively named here as 1W and 3W groups. Isolated marmosets were kept on individual metallic cages (0.75 × 0.75 × 0.8 m) and had no physical or visual contact with other members of the colony. The third group of animals (control, CTR) remained with their families in metallic cages (1.5 × 1.5 × 0.8 m) for 3 weeks. At the end of the third experimental week all these animals were perfused for immunolabeling of hippocampal proliferating cells.

The Rio Grande do Norte group consisted of 7 animals, with same age and similar weight as the Sao Paulo group (4 males, 200 ± 35 g; 3 females, 220 ± 18 g). Similarly to the São Paulo group, the baseline behavioral repertoire of these sub-adults marmosets was taken for each animal while they were with their families, housed in brick wall cages (2 × 2 × 2 m). Thereafter, animals were subdivided into the social isolated groups 1W and 3W. During isolation marmosets were kept on individual cages (2 × 2 × 1 m) without physical or visual contact with other members of the colony. Finally, after a period of social isolation, marmosets were returned to their family cages and their behavioral repertoire was also assessed to evaluate their familiar reunion.

Behavioral Observation and Cortisol Measurement

The behavioral repertoire assessment and the feces collection for cortisol levels measurements were made once a day, 2 days prior to the social isolation in order to establish its baseline levels. Feces samples were taken in the first 2 days of social isolation (early phase) and again in the last 2 days (late phase) totalizing four samples per subject during the isolation period. Behavioral observations were also made during the first and last 2 days of the isolation period. The animals were then moved back to their original family cage and again, feces collection and behavioral observations were made during the two following days, considered the reunion phase. The choice of assessment of cortisol levels from collected feces enabled us to avoid the manipulation of animals and the stress related to daily blood sample collection, which would in turn have influenced the animal's behavior.

The fecal collection was performed between 06:30 and 09:00 a.m. to avoid circadian influence on cortisol (Raminelli et al., 2003). All animals were observed until the defecation occurred. After that, a collector entered the cage and collected the feces from the cage bedding on the floor. Only completely identified feces were collected. Material of defecation was collected in small identified glass tubes with snap caps. After collection, the samples were frozen immediately.

Fecal samples were allowed to reach room temperature for at least 10 min before homogenization. Separation of the steroids by extraction was performed on 0.1 g of well-mixed feces by the addition of 2.5 mL ethanol and 2.5 mL distilled water. The samples were vortexed for 5 min and centrifuged for 10 min at 3000 rpm, and then the aqueous phase was decanted into glass tubes and then frozen at −20°C until solvolysis procedure.

Solvolysis was performed as previously described (Ziegler et al., 1995, 1996). To separate the steroid conjugates, frozen samples were left at room temperature for 10 min before the 500 samples were vortexed for 10 s and were added 100 mL solution of sodium chloride (NaCl) saturated, 50 mL sulfuric acid (H2SO4), and 5 mL of organic solvent ethyl acetate (C4H8O2). The solutions were vortexed for 1 min and kept under agitation in a water bath (40°C) overnight. Next morning, 4 mL of ethyl acetate was added to the mixture; this was followed by 5 min vortex and centrifuged for 3 min (1000 rpm). After supernatant extraction, 2.5 mL of distilled water was added and the samples were vortexed 5 min, followed by 3 min centrifugation at 1000 rpm. The supernatants were allowed to dry on a water bath at 40°C until its complete evaporation. Finally, the fractions of free steroids were ressuspended in 500 μL of ethanol and assayed for cortisol using a modification of the enzyme immunoassay method developed by (Munro and Stabenfeldt, 1984; Ziegler et al., 1995, 1996) with the standards used for this assay prepared in ethanol. Parallelism using serial dilution of marmoset fecal pool and standards did not differ (P > 0.05), and the accuracy was 109.6 ± 4.4%. For this, 50 μL of each sample was placed on a water bath 40°C until its complete evaporation. Immediately following was added 300 μL of solution containing the enzyme conjugate (cortisol:HRP, diluted at 1: 75,000) to the respective hormone (dilution 1:16,000 in monophosphate buffer:sodium diphosphate). The solution was kept under agitation for 5 min. Then, 100 μL of each sample was placed in multiwell plates prepared with the cortisol antibody (R4866, 1:12,000), which was developed and characterized by Munro and Stabenfeldt (1984). The samples were incubated for 2 h in a humid chamber. After that, 100 μL/well a solution containing 25 mL of 10% citrate buffer, 250 μ L of ABTS substrate [2,2′-Azino-bis (3- ethylbenzothiazoline-6-sulfonic acid) diammonium salt, Sigma] and 80 μL of H2O2 was added to each well. Thus, the multiwell plate was incubated for 1 h in humidity chamber. A solution containing 5.048 mL of hydrofluoric acid; 1.2 mL of sodium hydroxide (5N) diluted in 500 mL of distilled water was prepared. From this solution, 100 μL of was added to each well to stop the reaction. The optical density reading of the plaque was assessed with a spectrophotometer (Asys/Hitech Expert plus, Eugendorf, Austria) using 410 nm filter.

The whole behavioral repertoire analysis was performed during 2 h daily using the focal continuous observation. This method has been previously demonstrated to be effective to monitor the bimodal pattern in captive common marmosets (Barbosa and Mota, 2009). For statistical analysis we selected anxiety-related behaviors (Barros and Tomaz, 2002; Dettling et al., 2002a), like scent-marking (frequency of rubbing the anogenital on a surface of cage), scratching, piloerection, and locomotor activity (the cage was divided into 60 quadrants of 38 cm each and the frequency of movement between two quadrants was recorded as one movement), as well as behaviors that are indicative of attenuation of tension like auto-grooming: time spent licking, picking at or parting his or her own fur with the fingers (Devries et al., 2007; Wittig et al., 2008), attempts to establish contact, and social-grooming.

BrdU and Doublecortin Staining

On the last day at end of 1 (1W group) or 3 weeks (3W group) of social isolation or 3 weeks after the beginning of the experiments (CTR group), animals (n = 3 per group) were given a single injection of 5-bromo-2′-deoxyuridine (BrdU; 75 mg/Kg i.p. dissolved in a solution containing 0.9% NaCl and 0.007 M NaOH). Twenty-four hours later with the animals still in isolation, they were deeply anesthetized with thiopental (50 mg/Kg, i.p) and transcardially perfused with 0.9% saline, followed by a 4% paraformaldehyde fixative solution. The brains were then removed from the skull and post-fixed overnight in the same fixative solution before cryoprotection in 30% sucrose solution. Thirty-two μm thick coronal sections throughout the entire antero-posterior axis of the hippocampus were cut on a cryostat. Sections from each animal were sequentially collected in 24-well plates filled with anti-freezing solution and stored at −20°C.

The immunohistochemistry procedure was a priori defined and it was performed on every 12th section of the entire hippocampus (the distance between each two sections was about 360 μm). With this sampling, we were able to run countings in 1 out of 12 representative sections along the rostro-caudal extension of the dentate gyrus. The tissue was first incubated with 3% H2O2 in PB (10 min), washed in PB, followed by 2M HCl at 40°C (30 min), and washed in PBS (3 × 10 min). Sections were then incubated for 1 h in a blocking buffer solution containing 2% normal goat serum (Sigma-Aldrich) and 0.1% TritonX-100 in PBS. Thereafter, they were incubated with monoclonal primary antibody to BrdU (rat anti-BrdU, 1:200, Axyll/Accurate Chemical) in the blocking solution overnightat 4°C. In the next morning, sections were washed in PBS and incubated with biotinylated secondary antibody (goat anti-rat IgG, 1:200, Vector) during 2 h in PB. After 3 washes in PBS (3 × 10 min) were incubated for 1 h in avidin-biotin-peroxidase complex (Vectastain Elite ABC,Vector) and the reaction product was developed using 0.05% 3,3′-diaminobenzidine-tetrahydrochloride(DAB, Sigma-Aldrich). Stained sections were mounted onto gelatin-coated glass slides, air dried, dehydrated, and coverslipped with Entellan (Merk).

Co-localization of BrdU and doublecortin (DCX) was examined in the sections adjacent to those used for BrdU labeling. Sections were incubated in HCl 2M at 40°C for 30 min, followed by incubation with a mixture of the monoclonal primary antibodies to BrdU (rat,1:200; Axyll/Accurate Chemical) and DCX (guinea pig, 1:100; Chemicon) diluted in blocking buffer overnight, at room temperature. After several rinses in PBS, sections were incubated with Alexa Fluor® 488 goat anti-rat IgG (1:300, Molecular Probes) and Alexa Fluor® 546 donkey anti-guinea pig IgG (1:300, Molecular Probes) for 1 h at room temperature. Rinsed tissue sections were then mounted onto gelatin-subbed slides and coverslipped with Fluoromount G (Electron Microscopy Sciences).

Cell Counts

Cell counts were conducted on a blinded fashion at 3 different rostro-caudal levels throughout the hippocampal extent of 9 animals. These corresponded to plates A 6.5–A 5.5 (rostral level), A 5.0–A 4.0 (intermediate level) and A3.5–A 1.5 (caudal level) of the marmoset brain atlas by Stephan et al., 1980. Both right and the left hippocampi were assessed, rendering a total of 55, 58, and 30 coronal sections counted in the groups of animals isolated for 1 week, 3 weeks and controls, respectively. Since the number of proliferating cells labeled with BrdU or BrdU/DCX in the dentate gyrus was very low and the spatial distribution was inhomogeneous, the total number of labeled cells was counted in the selected sections, as above described, for each animal. For this, we took care to assure that each analyzed section was similar among all groups.

To quantify BrdU-positive cells, we have used a light microscope (400 × magnification; Olympus® BX 50 microscope, Japan) and a digital image analysis system (Neurozoom software, NY, USA). Small BrdU-labeled nuclei in the hilar border of the dentate gyrus (presumed to be glial precursors) and fusiform immunostained cells (endothelial-like) were excluded from the analysis. In order to normalize data, the number of BrdU-positive cells in the three different groups of animals is expressed as the mean number of labeled cells per section.

The number of cells co-localizing BrdU and DCX was determined with a Zeiss (Oberkochen) LSM 510 confocal laser microscope. Sections were scanned using a 40 × oil-immersion objective and dual-channel excitation with argon (488 nm) and helium-neon (543 nm). Four coronal sections per subject corresponded to the ones described above were randomly assessed for counting. Co-localization analysis included visual inspection of size and shape of cell throughout a z-stack and orthogonal planes. The percentage of co-localization was achieved by dividing the number of double-labeled cells (DCX/BrdU-positive) by the total number of BrdU-positive cells in each hippocampal section.

Statistical Analysis

The comparison of cortisol levels between baseline, early social isolation, late social isolation, and after family's reunion was assessed by means of repeated measure analysis of variance followed by post-hoc Fisher PLSD. The variation of behaviors between phases was assessed with non-parametric tests (Friedman, Wilcoxon, Mann-Whitney). Pearson's correlation coefficient (r) was obtained to investigate the relationship between anxiety-like behaviors with the cortisol levels along different phases of social isolation. Differences in the total number of BrdU-positive cells between groups, across hippocampal plates or the full structure, were assessed with Two-Way and One-Way ANOVA (applying Newman-Keuls post-hoc test), respectively. Percentages of BrdU and DCX co-localization were analyzed by Kruskal-Wallis and Mann-Whitney tests. After the percentage was determined for the total BrdU counting, we performed One-Way analysis of variance followed by Newman-Keuls post-hoc test. Statistical significance was set at P ≤ 0.05. Results in the text are expressed as mean ± standard error (SE).

Results

Different Durations of Social Isolation Resulted in Behavioral Changes Among Young Marmosets

Animals that have been isolated for 1 week decreased auto-grooming at both early and late phases of isolation, when compared to baseline values (X2 = 9.52, P = 0.02; Basal × Early phase: z = 2.02; P = 0.04; Basal × Late phase: z = 2.02; P = 0.04) (Figure 1A). The 1W group also had higher frequency of scent-marking during both phases of isolation than in the reunion phase (X2 = 7.8, P = 0.05; Reunion × Early phase: z = 2.02; p = 0.04; Reunion × Late phase: z = 2.02; P = 0.04) (Figure 1C).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Behavioral changes produced by isolation and cortisol measurement from fecal samples. Scatter plot graphs from behavioral observation data show (A) duration of auto-grooming (expressed in seconds); (B) frequencies of locomotor activity; and (C) frequencies of scent-marking during baseline phase (Base), two initial days of isolation (Early), two finals days of isolation (Late) and reunion for both 1 week and 3 weeks social isolation. In (D), scatter plots represent normalized cortisol levels for marmosets isolated during one and 3 weeks; Horizontal lines indicate median values; brackets designate significant alterations between phases (P < 0.05). # Indicate tendency of variation between respective phase and baseline phase (0.05 < P < 0.06).

Animals isolated for 3 weeks had higher frequencies of locomotor activity at later phase of isolation than during other phases (X2 = 13.8, P = 0.03; Basal × Late phase: z = 2.52; P = 0.01; Early × Late phase: z = 2.36; P = 0.01; Reunion × Late phase: z = 2.36; P = 0.01) (Figure 1B) and higher frequencies of scent-marking at late phase of isolation than after reunion (X2 = 8.19, P = 0.04; Reunion × Late phase: z = 2.02; P = 0.04) (Figure 1C). Altogether these data indicate that marmosets had a higher expression of anxiety-related behaviors and a reduction of those related to attenuation of tension during social isolation, but we were not able to distinguish the effects between 1 and 3 week groups. We did not observe any other behavioral alteration regarding the other analyzed parameters, such as piloerection, attempts to contact and social-grooming. It is important to emphasize that all anxiety-related behaviors returned to their basal levels after family reunion, as well as the social grooming, which is an indicative of good adaptation. This result is confirmed by the cortisol levels measurement in the reunion phase, as described below.

Social Isolation Induced Stress in Marmosets

In order to infer about stress levels during social isolation, each session of behavioral monitoring was followed by cortisol levels measurement, for all animals under study (Figure 1D). The early phase of social deprivation caused significant increase in cortisol in both 1W (P = 0.03) and 3W groups (P < 0.01) when compared to their respective baseline levels. However, thereafter cortisol levels gradually decreased, reaching values between baseline and early isolation phases (P = 0.057). After family reunion, the cortisol levels of previously isolated marmosets became similar to their respective baseline values (P > 0.05).

Positive correlations were observed between anxiety-related behaviors and cortisol levels indicating that animals had indeed experienced stress. The cortisol level was significantly correlated with the frequencies of scent-marking during the early phase of social deprivation (r = 0.53, P < 0.05) and with increased locomotion (r = 0.10, P < 0.05) in the later phase of social deprivation.

Hippocampal Cell Proliferation and Neurogenesis are Impaired in Isolated Marmosets

Cell counts of the entire hippocampus showed that the number of dentate gyrus progenitor cells incorporating BrdU was significantly decreased in animals that were socially isolated for 1 (3.71 ± 0.65 cells/section) or 3 (3.20 ± 0.87 cells/section) weeks, as compared to controls (6.04 ± 0.04 cells/section; P = 0.04, F = 5.82) (Figure 2A). In order to verify if this reduction was homogeneous throughout the rostro-caudal extent of the hippocampus, we further evaluated the distribution of BrdU-labeled cells in rostral, intermediate and caudal levels of hippocampus, as described in Methods. A One-Way ANOVA indicated that major BrdU-labeled cell reduction occurred in rostral levels, where 1W and 3W groups were statistically different from non-isolated animals (P < 0.01, F = 14.98). It also revealed that cell proliferation in the intermediate or caudal portions of dentate gyrus was similar between 1W, 3W, or CTR marmosets (Figure 2B). No interaction was found between groups and hippocampal levels (Two-Way ANOVA; P = 0.26, F = 1.44).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Effects of social isolation on hippocampal cell proliferation. (A) Representative images of BrdU-positive cell labeling in dentate gyrus subgranular area in rostral region for each group in a wider view (upper panel) and in detail of the indicated area as showed in the lower panel; arrow heads indicate cells included in the counting; scale bar for upper panel = 200 μm; scale bar for lower panel = 150 μm. (B) graphical representation of the total number of BrdU-positive cells per section at three different rostral-caudal levels of the hippocampus for animals that were socially isolated for 1 week, 3 weeks, and non-isolated age matched controls (CTR). (C) Orthogonal Z-section of confocal microscope image of BrdU-DCX double-labeled cell in hippocampal dentate gyrus used to estimate the number of new cells undergoing a neuronal fate per section, showed in graph (D). Figure (E) represents DCX-immunorreactivity density in dentate gyrus for each marmoset under control or 1 weeks or 3 weeks isolation periods. BrdU labels in green and DCX labels in red; scale bar: 50 μm. Data represented as mean ± standard error. * Indicates statistical significance between CTR and the respective group.

To assess whether newborn cells underwent neuronal fate, sections were double-stained for BrdU and DCX (a marker of immature neurons) (Figure 2D). Among the BrdU-labeled cells 43.7% co-localized to DCX in non-isolated controls, 35.5% in 1W group, and 19.2% in 3W group. These proportions pointed toward a decrease in the neurogenesis rate after 3 weeks isolation (P = 0.05, compared to Control and 1W groups). Taken together, these results suggest that marmosets that were socially isolated not only had a reduced number of dentate gyrus proliferating cells, but also fewer of these cells underwent a neuronal phenotype. Thus, once the number of newborn neurons can be estimated by applying the proportion of double-labeled cells over the total BrdU counting (Figure 2C), the neurogenesis impairment among the isolated primates can be clearly appreciated (P = 0.0004, F = 39.17). The late effects of neurogenesis impairment after 3 weeks of social isolation resulted in an overall reduced DCX immunoreactivity in the dentate gyrus, as illustrated in Figure 2E, which includes a representative image for each subject in all three groups.

Discussion

In summary, our results led us toward a possible cascade of events from social isolation to hippocampal substrates of anxiety-related behaviors during early adulthood of non-human primates. Here we found that socially isolated young marmosets had an increase in cortisol levels during the first days of isolation, concomitant with changes in affective states and subsequently a steady decrease in hippocampal neurogenesis.

It is well accepted that social isolation among rodent pups affects brain function, leading the organism to adapt to challenges in the environment. However, the long-term resulting stress can produce maladaptive behaviors and ultimately disease (Marsden et al., 2011; Pereda-Perez et al., 2013). Compared to rodents, social interactions in primates are even more complex. Some years ago it has been proposed that callitrichids when exposed to disturbing conditions do not overreact to it. This response was considered to be due to their familiar organization, which is very complex and stable over time, therefore such familiar relationship could have a buffering effect when these animals are exposed to stressful situations (Rylands, 1993). This however, is in opposition to recent findings (Barbosa and Mota, 2009; Yamaguchi et al., 2010). In spite of their complex organization and stability, in captive conditions families of callitrichids are easily stressed even by their caregivers, showing higher stress levels during week daily care manipulations than during weekends (Barbosa and Mota, 2009). Thus, we believe that individual isolation of juvenile marmosets for 1 or 3 weeks could adequately model anxiety in non-human primates. Age is critical factor as it has been demonstrated that adult marmosets are insensitive to psychosocial stress paradigms that include a recovery period, as measured by neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus (Marlatt et al., 2011).

The potential impact of the reduced neurogenesis reported here is that it is occurring at a critical period, which is essential to make predictions regarding possible future mental disturbances. The transition between childhood and adulthood is characterized by high impulsivity, high plasticity and the development of complex behavioral repertoires, ultimately leading to stable behavioral patterns that guarantee successful survival (Spear, 2000).The transition from adolescence to adulthood varies in different primate species, yet it is well defined and displays many of the characteristic traits described in humans (Pereira and Altmann, 1985; Steinberg et al., 1989). Recent data based on computational estimates suggested that adult-generated neurons could be additionally incorporated to the population of existing (mature) granule cells leading to gradual increases in the total number of neurons (Aimone et al., 2009). Thus, stressing factors, applied during this critical period, as used in our current study may not only affect neuron birth but also the structural organization of the granule cell layer. We do not have data to test this hypothesis thus future studies should address these specific questions using hippocampal-dependent tasks. Our current results demonstrate that rostral hippocampus levels are the most affected in terms of cell proliferation. Similarly it has recently been reported that depressed adult female cynomolgus monkeys also have hippocampal atrophy in rostral but not caudal portions (Willard et al., 2009). It is noteworthy that previous studies consider that posterior hippocampus in humans (or primates) activation may reflect contextual fear encoding, whereas the engagement of rostral regions during later phases of acquisition may reflect the emotional expression of that fear (Bannerman et al., 2003; Fanselow and Dong, 2010).

The greater manifestation of anxiety-related behaviors right after social deprivation was concomitant with increased cortisol levels, as previously demonstrated by others (Smith et al., 1998; Marlatt et al., 2011). Similarly to our results, these authors reported increases in cortisol levels 48 h after isolation with recovery to basal levels when animals had returned to their family. In the current study, past some days of isolation, cortisol slightly decreased until remaining constant between 1 and 3 weeks after familial separation. On the other hand, unlike cortisol levels, anxiety-related behaviors continuously augmented during late phase of isolation. At first, animals that were socially isolated for 1 week showed signs of increased distress with high number of scent marking and high levels of cortisol, similarly to previous findings (Laudenslager et al., 1990, 1995; Norcross and Newman, 1999; Rukstalis and French, 2005). As the social isolation persists, some authors have reported a drop in physiological indicators of stress, indicative of habituation (Honess and Marin, 2006). Our current results on the other hand indicate that even when individual isolation took longer, animals still had a high number of scent marking and locomotor activities, having these anxiety-related behaviors returned to basal levels only after familiar reunion. At the final days in the 1 week isolation group, cortisol level became stable, but not anxiety-like behaviors, indicating here dissociation between hormones and behaviors. Similar findings were described in others studies with different species (Norcross and Newman, 1999; Hennessy et al., 2008) that in turn show the importance of measuring both indicators for more accurate results.

Direct modulation of dentate gyrus cell population by corticoids has been considered an important mechanism through which stressors reduce hippocampal neurogenesis (Mirescu and Gould, 2006). Indeed, already in the early phase of isolation animals had significantly increased cortisol levels, a hormone involved in mediating the effects of stress on neurogenesis. The hippocampus contains glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptors, besides being an important structure in the modulation of stressful responses (for a review see McEwen, 1999). Accordingly, a recent study has demonstrated that early life stress in primate infants leads in adolescence to mild reductions in the expression of mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptor genes in the hippocampus (Arabadzisz et al., 2010). As concluded by these last authors, it is unlikely that these reductions are only acute mediators of the long-term effects of early life stress. Indeed, increases in the corticosterone levels during social isolation in marmosets did alter the number of proliferating cells in the dentate gyrus (Marlatt et al., 2011).

Decreased hippocampal neurogenesis plays a role in depression and anxiety-related behaviors (Wong and Licinio, 2004; Cryan and Holmes, 2005). Nevertheless, it does not establish a direct link between both since inhibition of neurogenesis in mice did not seem to trigger a depressive or anxious behavioral phenotype, but the full capacity of giving rise to new neurons may be crucial for antidepressants to take effect (Santarelli et al., 2003; Balu and Lucki, 2009). Accordingly, as shown here, inhibition of neurogenesis was exacerbated as a time-dependent outcome of social isolation-induced stress, even when cortisol levels were steady (from 1 until 3 weeks of separation). Some reports have indicated that decreased neurogenesis can keep on even after re-establishment of standard cortisol levels (Malberg and Duman, 2003; Mirescu et al., 2004). Thus, the hypothesis that high cortisol levels were not necessary to maintain neurogenesis suppressed is supported by the present results. From this work we can suggest that separation-induced stress affects proliferation of progenitor cells, as well as neuronal fate of newborn cells in the dentate gyrus.

Our current interpretations are limited by the number of animals and current experimental conditions. In rodent brain, profound differences have been reported between males and females in response to early life stress, particularly on neurogenesis (Oomen et al., 2009; Negrigo et al., 2011; Korosi et al., 2012; Lima et al., 2014). In the current study, we showed that gender does not interfere in the dentate gyrus neurogenesis rate (Marlatt et al., 2011), and thus we have chosen to pool male and female animals. Unfortunately, here we could not provide a direct correlation between the cortisol measurements and the dentate proliferating rate given these results were obtained from different sets of animals. Yet, behavioral results did not differ between the two sets of animals and due to the current experimental settings we were able to avoid the death of seven animals.

Conclusion

In non-human primates, social relationships comprise an important aspect of the juvenile's environment and are crucial for the development of normal behavior. Though these behavioral abnormalities have been somewhat characterized, the mechanisms underlying these events are still elusive. Our data is consistent with the notion that social deprivation leads to stress (indicated by higher cortisol levels), producing anxiety-like behaviors. We showed that some of the consequences of the stressful condition, such as reduction of neurogenesis, have a slower response profile. Our data might be relevant for the understanding of the pathophysiological conditions that ensue after episodes of social separation in humans.

Authors' Contributions

Simone M. Cinini was responsible for animal care at UNIFESP facility, participated in the study design, performed immunohistochemistry, BrdU cell counts, and drafted the manuscript. Gabriela F. Barnabe helped in study design, performed immunofluorescence, carried out confocal microscopy, statistical analysis, and drafted the manuscript. Nicole Galvão-Coelho conducted all behavioral and cortisol measurement experiments and its analysis. Magda A. de Medeiros conceived of the study and helped with animal care. Patrícia Perez-Mendes helped with animal care and participated in the study design. Maria B. C. Sousa designed the behavioral and cortisol experiments, also contributed with data interpretation. Luciene Covolan participated in experimental design, conducted cell count acquisition and interpretation, drafted the manuscript. Luiz E. Mello conceived of the study, and participated in its design and coordination and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

FAPESP, CNPq and Instituto do Milênio de Bioengenharia Tecidual; Simone M. Cinini was a CAPES fellow, Gabriela F. Barnabe was a FAPESP fellow. We also thank Ivone de Paulo for technical support and Beatriz Longo for providing reagents, both from Department of Physiology, Universidade Federal de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil. We are grateful to Carolina and Elizabeth for operating the confocal microscope.

Abbreviations

1W, socially isolated for one week; 3W, socially isolated for three weeks; BrdU, 5-bromo-2′-deoxyuridine; PBS, phosphate buffered saline; DAB, 3,3′-diaminobenzidine-tetrahydrochloride; DCX, doublecortin.

References

Abraham, I., Juhasz, G., Kekesi, K. A., and Kovacs, K. J. (1998). Corticosterone peak is responsible for stress-induced elevation of glutamate in the hippocampus. Stress 2, 171–181. doi: 10.3109/10253899809167281

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Aimone, J. B., Wiles, J., and Gage, F. H. (2009). Computational influence of adult neurogenesis on memory encoding. Neuron 61, 187–202. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Arabadzisz, D., Diaz-Heijtz, R., Knuesel, I., Weber, E., Pilloud, S., Dettling, A. C., et al. (2010). Primate early life stress leads to long-term mild hippocampal decreases in corticosteroid receptor expression. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 1106–1109. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Balu, D. T., and Lucki, I. (2009). Adult hippocampal neurogenesis: regulation, functional implications, and contribution to disease pathology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 232–252. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bannerman, D. M., Grubb, M., Deacon, R. M., Yee, B. K., Feldon, J., and Rawlins, J. N. (2003). Ventral hippocampal lesions affect anxiety but not spatial learning. Behav. Brain Res. 139, 197–213. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00268-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barbosa, M. N., and Mota, M. T. (2009). Behavioral and hormonal response of common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus, to two environmental conditions. Primates 50, 253–260. doi: 10.1007/s10329-009-0137-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barros, M., and Tomaz, C. (2002). Non-human primate models for investigating fear and anxiety. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 187–201. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00064-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bunk, E. C., Stelzer, S., Hermann, S., Schafers, M., Schlatt, S., and Schwamborn, J. C. (2011). Cellular organization of adult neurogenesis in the Common Marmoset. Aging Cell 10, 28–38. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-9726.2010.00639.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cryan, J. F., and Holmes, A. (2005). The ascent of mouse: advances in modelling human depression and anxiety. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 4, 775–790. doi: 10.1038/nrd1825

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dettling, A. C., Feldon, J., and Pryce, C. R. (2002a). Early deprivation and behavioral and physiological responses to social separation/novelty in the marmoset. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 73, 259–269. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(02)00785-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dettling, A. C., Feldon, J., and Pryce, C. R. (2002b). Repeated parental deprivation in the infant common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus, primates) and analysis of its effects on early development. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 1037–1046. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01460-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Devries, A. C., Craft, T. K., Glasper, E. R., Neigh, G. N., and Alexander, J. K. (2007). 2006 Curt P. Richter award winner: social influences on stress responses and health. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 587–603. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.04.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Duman, R. S., and Monteggia, L. M. (2006). A neurotrophic model for stress-related mood disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 59, 1116–1127. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.02.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fanselow, M. S., and Dong, H. W. (2010). Are the dorsal and ventral hippocampus functionally distinct structures? Neuron 65, 7–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gould, E., and Gross, C. G. (2002). Neurogenesis in adult mammals: some progress and problems. J. Neurosci. 22, 619–623.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gould, E., McEwen, B. S., Tanapat, P., Galea, L. A., and Fuchs, E. (1997). Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the adult tree shrew is regulated by psychosocial stress and NMDA receptor activation. J. Neurosci. 17, 2492–2498.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gould, E., Tanapat, P., McEwen, B. S., Flugge, G., and Fuchs, E. (1998). Proliferation of granule cell precursors in the dentate gyrus of adult monkeys is diminished by stress. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 3168–3171. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.6.3168

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hennessy, M. B., Zate, R., and Maken, D. S. (2008). Social buffering of the cortisol response of adult female guinea pigs. Physiol. Behav. 93, 883–888. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.12.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Honess, P. E., and Marin, C. M. (2006). Behavioural and physiological aspects of stress and aggression in nonhuman primates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 390–412. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jacobsen, J. P., and Mork, A. (2006). Chronic corticosterone decreases brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) mRNA and protein in the hippocampus, but not in the frontal cortex, of the rat. Brain Res. 1110, 221–225. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.077

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Karten, Y. J., Olariu, A., and Cameron, H. A. (2005). Stress in early life inhibits neurogenesis in adulthood. Trends Neurosci. 28, 171–172. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.01.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kee, N., Teixeira, C. M., Wang, A. H., and Frankland, P. W. (2007). Preferential incorporation of adult-generated granule cells into spatial memory networks in the dentate gyrus. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 355–362. doi: 10.1038/nn1847

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Koo, J. W., and Duman, R. S. (2008). IL-1beta is an essential mediator of the antineurogenic and anhedonic effects of stress. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 751–756. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0708092105

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Korosi, A., Naninck, E. F., Oomen, C. A., Schouten, M., Krugers, H., Fitzsimons, C., et al. (2012). Early-life stress mediated modulation of adult neurogenesis and behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 227, 400–409. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.07.037

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laudenslager, M. L., Boccia, M. L., Berger, C. L., Gennaro-Ruggles, M. M., McFerran, B., and Reite, M. L. (1995). Total cortisol, free cortisol, and growth hormone associated with brief social separation experiences in young macaques. Dev. Psychobiol. 28, 199–211. doi: 10.1002/dev.420280402

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laudenslager, M. L., Held, P. E., Boccia, M. L., Reite, M. L., and Cohen, J. J. (1990). Behavioral and immunological consequences of brief mother-infant separation: a species comparison. Dev. Psychobiol. 23, 247–264. doi: 10.1002/dev.420230305

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lima, M., Malheiros, J., Negrigo, A., Tescarollo, F., Medeiros, M., Suchecki, D., et al. (2014). Sex-related long-term behavioral and hippocampal cellular alterations after nociceptive stimulation throughout postnatal development in rats. Neuropharmacology 77, 268–276. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.10.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Malberg, J. E., and Duman, R. S. (2003). Cell proliferation in adult hippocampus is decreased by inescapable stress: reversal by fluoxetine treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 1562–1571. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300234

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marlatt, M. W., Philippens, I., Manders, E., Czeh, B., Joels, M., Krugers, H., et al. (2011). Distinct structural plasticity in the hippocampus and amygdala of the middle-aged common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Exp. Neurol. 230, 291–301. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2011.05.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marsden, C. A., King, M. V., and Fone, K. C. (2011). Influence of social isolation in the rat on serotonergic function and memory–relevance to models of schizophrenia and the role of 5-HT(6) receptors. Neuropharmacology 61, 400–407. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.03.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McEwen, B. S. (1999). Stress and hippocampal plasticity. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 105–122. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.22.1.105

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mirescu, C., and Gould, E. (2006). Stress and adult neurogenesis. Hippocampus 16, 233–238. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20155

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mirescu, C., Peters, J. D., and Gould, E. (2004). Early life experience alters response of adult neurogenesis to stress. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 841–846. doi: 10.1038/nn1290

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Munro, C., and Stabenfeldt, G. (1984). Development of a microtitre plate enzyme immunoassay for the determination of progesterone. J. Endocrinol. 101, 41–49. doi: 10.1677/joe.0.1010041

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Negrigo, A., Medeiros, M., Guinsburg, R., and Covolan, L. (2011). Long-term gender behavioral vulnerability after nociceptive neonatal formalin stimulation in rats. Neurosci. Lett. 490, 196–199. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.12.050

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Norcross, J. L., and Newman, J. D. (1999). Effects of separation and novelty on distress vocalizations and cortisol in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Am. J. Primatol. 47, 209–222. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1999)47:3<209::AID-AJP3>3.0.CO;2-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Oomen, C. A., Girardi, C. E., Cahyadi, R., Verbeek, E. C., Krugers, H., Joels, M., et al. (2009). Opposite effects of early maternal deprivation on neurogenesis in male versus female rats. PLoS ONE 4:e3675. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003675

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pereda-Perez, I., Popovic, N., Otalora, B. B., Popovic, M., Madrid, J. A., Rol, M. A., et al. (2013). Long-term social isolation in the adulthood results in CA1 shrinkage and cognitive impairment. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 106, 31–39. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.07.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pereira, M., Altmann, J. (1985). Nonhuman Primate Models for Human Growth and Development. New York, NY: Blank Liss.

Raminelli, J., Sousa, M. B. C., Cunha, M. S., and Barbosa, M. F. V. (2003). Morning and afternoon patterns of fecal cortisol excretion among reproductive and non-reproductive male and female common marmosets, callithrix jacchus. Biol. Rhythm Res. 32, 159–167. doi: 10.1076/brhm.32.2.159.1357

CrossRef Full Text

Rizzi, S., Bianchi, P., Guidi, S., Ciani, E., and Bartesaghi, R. (2007). Neonatal isolation impairs neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus of the guinea pig. Hippocampus 17, 78–91. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20247

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rosenblum, L. A., and Andrews, M. W. (1994). Influences of environmental demand on maternal behavior and infant development. Acta Paediatr. Suppl. 397, 57–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1994.tb13266.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rukstalis, M., and French, J. A. (2005). Vocal buffering of the stress response: exposure to conspecific vocalizations moderates urinary cortisol excretion in isolated marmosets. Horm. Behav. 47, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.09.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rylands, A. (1993). Marmosets and Tamarins: Systematics, Behaviour, and Ecology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Santarelli, L., Saxe, M., Gross, C., Surget, A., Battaglia, F., Dulawa, S., et al. (2003). Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science 301, 805–809. doi: 10.1126/science.1083328

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, T. E., McGreer-Whitworth, B., and French, J. A. (1998). Close proximity of the heterosexual partner reduces the physiological and behavioral consequences of novel-cage housing in black tufted-ear marmosets (Callithrix kuhli). Horm. Behav. 34, 211–222. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1469

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 417–463. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00014-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Steinberg, L., Adams, G., Montemayor R., and Gullotta T. (1989). Biology of Adolescent Behavior and Development. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.

Stephan, H., Baron, G., and Schwerdtfeger, W. K. (1980). The Brain of the Common Marmoset (Callithrix Jacchus) A Stereotaxic Atlas. Berlin, NY: Springer-Verlag. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-67491-4

CrossRef Full Text

Stranahan, A. M., Khalil, D., and Gould, E. (2006). Social isolation delays the positive effects of running on adult neurogenesis. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 526–533. doi: 10.1038/nn1668

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Praag, H., Schinder, A. F., Christie, B. R., Toni, N., Palmer, T. D., and Gage, F. H. (2002). Functional neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus. Nature 415, 1030–1034. doi: 10.1038/4151030a

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Willard, S. L., Friedman, D. P., Henkel, C. K., and Shively, C. A. (2009). Anterior hippocampal volume is reduced in behaviorally depressed female cynomolgus macaques. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1469–1475. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.04.022

CrossRef Full Text

Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Lehmann, J., Whitten, P. L., Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (2008). Focused grooming networks and stress alleviation in wild female baboons. Horm. Behav. 54, 170–177. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.02.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wong, E. Y., and Herbert, J. (2005). Roles of mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptors in the regulation of progenitor proliferation in the adult hippocampus. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 785–792. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04277.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wong, E. Y., and Herbert, J. (2006). Raised circulating corticosterone inhibits neuronal differentiation of progenitor cells in the adult hippocampus. Neuroscience 137, 83–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.073

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wong, M. L., and Licinio, J. (2004). From monoamines to genomic targets: a paradigm shift for drug discovery in depression. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 3, 136–151. doi: 10.1038/nrd1303

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yamaguchi, C., Izumi, A., and Nakamura, K. (2010). Time course of vocal modulation during isolation in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Am. J. Primatol. 72, 681–688. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20824

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ziegler, T. E., Scheffler, G., and Snowdon, C. T. (1995). The relationship of cortisol levels to social environment and reproductive functioning in female cotton-top tamarins, Saguinus oedipus. Horm. Behav. 29, 407–424. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1995.1028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ziegler, T. E., Scheffler, G., Wittwer, D. J., Schultz-Darken, N., Snowdon, C. T., and Abbott, D. H. (1996). Metabolism of reproductive steroids during the ovarian cycle in two species of callitrichids, Saguinus oedipus and Callithrix jacchus, and estimation of the ovulatory period from fecal steroids. Biol. Reprod. 54, 91–99. doi: 10.1095/biolreprod54.1.91

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: social isolation, young marmosets, hippocampal neurogenesis, anxiety, isolation stress

Citation: Cinini SM, Barnabe GF, Galvão-Coelho N, de Medeiros MA, Perez-Mendes P, Sousa MBC, Covolan L and Mello LE (2014) Social isolation disrupts hippocampal neurogenesis in young non-human primates. Front. Neurosci. 8:45. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00045

Received: 01 December 2013; Paper pending published: 05 January 2014;
Accepted: 19 February 2014; Published online: 27 March 2014.

Edited by:

Nikolaos P. Daskalakis, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, USA

Reviewed by:

Lan Ma, Fudan University, China
Christopher Robert Pryce, University of Zurich, Switzerland

Copyright © 2014 Cinini, Barnabe, Galvão-Coelho, de Medeiros, Perez-Mendes, Sousa, Covolan and Mello. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Luiz E. Mello, Departamento de Fisiologia, Universidade Federal de São Paulo, Rua Botucatu, 862, 5° andar, 04023-062 São Paulo, Brazil e-mail: lemello@unifesp.br

These authors have contributed equally to this work.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.