Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 09 May 2017
Sec. Clinical Diabetes

Decreasing Sedentary Behavior: Effects on Academic Performance, Meta-Cognition, and Sleep

  • Department of Psychology, Clemson University, Clemson, SC, USA

There is growing interest in using activity workstations as a method of increasing light physical activity in normally sedentary environments. The current study (N = 117) compared the effects of studying in college students while slowly pedaling a stationary bike with a desktop with studying at traditional desks across 10 weeks in an academic semester. The students were assigned to study either on the stationary bike or at a traditional desk located in the campus library for a minimum of 2 h a week. During the 10 weeks, the students studied for tests or worked on other required academic activities while working at their assigned desk. In addition, the participants completed a pre survey, weekly surveys, and a post survey. We found that although students studying at the traditional desks reported more ease of studying and more effective studying than those using the stationary bikes, the two groups performed equally well on tests in an introductory psychology course. Moreover, the students using the traditional desks reported a decrease in sleep quality later in the semester while those using the activity workstation reported stable levels of sleep quality. The current results indicate that activity workstations could be implemented in university settings to encourage light physical activity without negatively affecting academic performance while providing possible long-term health and well-being benefits. Furthermore, the results suggests that activity workstations could be a means of combating sedentary behavior in environments where individuals are expected to sit either while waiting (e.g., doctor's waiting rooms, airports) or when completing a necessary task (e.g., the workplace, educational settings).

Introduction

Many studies have shown the positive effects of short-term regular moderate-to-vigorous physical activity on health, well-being, and performance (e.g., Warburton et al., 2006). Brief bouts of exercise; however, do not entirely counteract the potential negative effects of extended sedentary activity (Owen et al., 2009), indicating that too much sedentary behavior could be a separate health risk factor. Furthermore, many work and social settings encourage sedentary behavior resulting in inactive behavior increasingly replacing light-intensity physical activity during waking hours (Mansoubi et al., 2014). Previous research also suggests that trying to reverse this trend by replacing sedentary behavior with light-intensity activity could have positive metabolic benefits (Healy et al., 2008) and improve health (Torbeyns et al., 2014). Health researchers have also shown that light- or moderate-intensity activity (such as walking) is related to decreases in anxiety (Fox, 1999) and improved quality of life (Oka et al., 2000). As such, it is important to consider factors that may contribute to decreasing the amount of sedentary activity.

Physical activity decreases during adolescence (Stone et al., 1998) and with age (Mullineaux et al., 2001), resulting in many adults leading largely sedentary lives. Previous studies have found that persons in many types of work environments such as those working at desks or on computers (Hill et al., 2003) as well as many factory settings (Ishizaki et al., 2004) have higher levels of sedentary behavior. Moreover, sedentary behavior is related to a decrease in positive emotions (Hogan et al., 2015) while increased sitting time is related to increases in depression and anxiety (Rebar et al., 2014). In addition, sedentary behavior negatively affects metabolic health and decreases overall cognitive and brain health (Voss et al., 2014) whereas light physical activity such as pedaling a bicycle at a normal walking pace during two laboratory-based cognitive tasks for 35 min increases positive affect, motivation, and morale (Pilcher and Baker, 2016). As such, it is important to consider how to best incorporate activity throughout the day (Tudor-Locke and Schuna, 2012), particularly in sedentary settings where physical activity is limited (Smith et al., 2014). College campuses are one such setting where individuals do not participate in regular physical activity (Fountaine et al., 2011; Pengpid et al., 2015), resulting in many college students leading largely sedentary lives due to their time in class and time studying.

Only recently have researchers started to examine how work or educational settings can help encourage light physical activity in the place of normally sedentary activity and the resultant effects on performance, health, and well-being. One way to reduce sedentary behavior is to adapt normally indoor sedentary environments by providing activity workstations that encourage light physical activity while completing necessary or desired tasks. Recent research suggests that using activity workstations can increase energy expenditure (Tudor-Locke et al., 2014) and could have health benefits (Carson et al., 2014); however, there is limited research on the effects of implementing activity workstations into normally sedentary settings.

Little research has addressed the effects of light physical activity on daily functioning such as performance and meta-cognition (Rhodes et al., 2012) and the available literature provides conflicting results. Meta-cognition has been referred to as a self-awareness of mental activity and is important to consider with regards to the perceived effectiveness of studying. Some studies suggest activity workstations result in decreased performance on specific motor skills tasks (Straker et al., 2009), while other studies conclude that cognitive performance does not decrease while working at activity workstations (Cox et al., 2011; Carr et al., 2014; Pilcher and Baker, 2016). Research; however, suggests that the educational environment has an effect on college-level performance (Marchard et al., 2014). As such, additional research examining common types of cognitive tasks as well as the meta-cognitive effects while using activity workstations is needed to help determine if activity workstations can be implemented without adverse effects in many educational as well as work settings.

The primary purpose of the current experiment was to examine the effects of studying when riding a stationary bike with a desktop (FitDesk) in comparison to studying at a traditional desk on academic performance, meta-cognitive factors, and self-reported sleep. The meta-cognitive factors included student studying preferences and the benefits of active study habits. We hypothesize that using the FitDesk will not decrease academic performance. Due to the paucity of research, we could not generate testable hypotheses on the effects of using the FitDesk vs. a traditional desk on the meta-cognitive and sleep measures used in the current study.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were students in an introductory psychology class. One hundred and seventeen from a total of 249 students in the class completed the study (75 females, age 18.39 ± 0.94; 42 males, age 18.33 ± 0.72). Fifty-nine participants were block random assigned to the FitDesk condition (37 females, age 18.41 ± 0.96; 22 males, age 18.27 ± 0.63). The remaining 58 participants were assigned to the traditional desk condition (38 females, age 18.37 ± 0.94; 20 males, age 18.40 ± 0.82). The university's institutional review board approved the study. Inclusion criteria for this study included reporting being in good mental and physical health, and the physical ability to slowly pedal a bicycle for up to 2 h.

Procedures

Students in an introductory psychology class were offered credit for required research participation as well as extra credit points to complete the study. The study was explained to the students in the class meeting following their first exam. Interested students signed the informed consent form which also gave experimenters permission to access their introductory psychology class grades. Researchers contacted the volunteers and asked them to complete a preliminary survey (see below). Participants were then assigned to the FitDesk study group or the traditional desk study group.

The FitDesks and traditional desks were located in the campus library providing access anytime the library was open. The FitDesk (Revo Innovations LLC, Antioch, TN) is a silent, stationary bike with a desktop. Users could work on a laptop, tablet, or other study materials while pedaling the bike. Participants were encouraged to peddle at a slow pace (similar in exertion to a normal walking pace) when using the FitDesk. All participants used their smart phones to scan QR codes posted in the room to indicate when they started and stopped studying. The time of logging in and logging out was automatically recorded. In addition, when logging out the participants indicated which FitDesk or traditional desk they used and briefly summarized what they worked on. The participants first used the FitDesks or traditional desks in the library for a 2-h acclimation period where they could do any type of activity (e.g., study for any class, on-line shopping). The participants were instructed to complete their acclimation period within 1 week after signing the consent form. After the acclimation week, participants were instructed to study only for their introductory psychology class for 2 h each week at their assigned desk type (FitDesk or traditional desk) for the 10 week study. Regular email reminders were sent informing the students of their number of minutes studying that week during the last 2 days of each week, to encourage all students to complete the full 2 h of study time for that week.

The participants completed weekly surveys (see below) that were initiated each Monday of the study and were completed by midnight on Tuesday. During the study, the participants completed their required in-class multiple-choice exams. At the end of the experiment, participants completed a final survey (see below). All surveys were administered electronically.

Academic Performance

Participants gave permission for the researchers to access their introductory psychology class grades on five exams, scores on a pre- and post-test (range of scores from 0 to 18), and their final grade in the course. The pre-test and exam 1 occurred before the onset of the study. Exams 2, 3, and 4 occurred during the study and were used to indicate academic performance during the study. Exam 5 and the post-test occurred after the conclusion of the study.

Subjective Measures

The preliminary survey contained one question on study habits, one question on class motivation, three questions on exercise, one question on physical health, and one question on sleep habits (see variable list in Table 1). These survey items were used to ensure that the two desk conditions were not different at the onset of the study. All questions on the preliminary survey were assessed using the same 5-point scale: almost never (1), less than half the time, about half the time, more than half the time, and almost always (5).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Academic Performance, survey items, and descriptive statistics.

The weekly survey (see variable list in Table 1) contained Borg's Rating of Perceived Exertion Scale (RPE; Borg, 1982, 1990). The RPE provides a subjective measure of exercise intensity and is related to physiological measures of physical exertion with validity coefficients ranging from 0.57 to 0.72 (Chen et al., 2002). The participants rated their level of perceived physical exertion while using their assigned desk type during the week on a scale from 0 (nothing at all) to 10 (very, very strong). The participants then filled out a single item question on motivation (5-point scale from very low to very high). A previous study using the FitDesk used this same single item motivation question and found that light physical activity when completing a complex cognitive task increased motivation (Pilcher and Baker, 2016). The five-item Morale Scale followed the motivation question and evaluated energy, drive, enthusiasm, eagerness, and morale (5-point scale from very low to very high) while studying during the week at their assigned desk type. The Morale Scale has been shown to have a Cronbach's alpha of 0.93 (Britt et al., 2013). A previous study using the FitDesk also found that light physical activity when completing a complex cognitive task increased morale using the Morale Scale (Pilcher and Baker, 2016). The weekly survey also contained six questions adapted from the Engagement Scale (e.g., sense of personal responsibility in studying, commitment to studying, completely absorbed in studying) to rate their studying at their assigned desk type using a 5-point Likert scale from strongly disagree to strongly agree (Britt et al., 2010). The Engagement Scale has been shown to be a predictor of performance in academic settings (Britt et al., 2010) and was used in a previous study with the FitDesk (Pilcher and Baker, 2016). The final portion of the weekly survey contained three questions about sleep habits (average time going to bed, average time waking up, and quality of sleep from very poor to very good).

The final survey (see variable list in Table 1) allowed participants to assess concepts that could be related to academic performance using a 5-point scale from not at all to very much. Concepts include study habits and feeling of motivation and focus experienced while studying. Questions on amount of time studying were also included in the final survey. Lastly, two questions on studying at the assigned desk type and whether the participant would use the desk type again when studying were assessed on a 5-point scale from strongly disagree to strongly agree.

Statistical Analyses

Data analysis was performed using the IBM SPSS statistical program (SPSS 22; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). A 2 × 3 mixed factors analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare the two desk groups across the three exams. Independent samples t-tests were used to compare differences between the groups on all other measures except the sleep-related data. The data from the weekly surveys were averaged across the study. A global morale score was calculated by averaging the component questions on motivation, energy, enthusiasm, eagerness, and morale. A global engagement score was calculated by averaging the component questions on caring about studying, personal responsibility for studying, commitment to studying, importance of studying, and devotion to studying. Independent samples t-test were used to compare the desk groups for average sleep quantity and sleep quality across the 10-week period. Sleep quantity was calculated from the reported time the participant usually went to bed and the time they usually woke. As an additional analysis comparing the desk conditions, sleep data were collapsed across the first 5 weeks and last 5 weeks and compared. Two 2 (desk condition) × 2 (time, first 5 weeks and second 5 weeks) mixed factors ANOVAs were used to examine potential differences in self-reported sleep quantity and quality across the study. The Greenhouse-Geisser within-groups test is reported.

Results

Assigned Study Time

Participants in the Fitdesk condition spent an average of 1.92 (SD = 0.42) hours per week studying on the Fitdesk during the study. Similarly, participants in the traditional desk condition spent an average of 1.94 (SD = 0.38) hours per week studying on the traditional desk during the study. There was no significant difference in time studying between the two conditions.

Academic Performance

Descriptive statistics for all test scores are reported in Table 1. There were no significant differences in performance between the FitDesk and traditional desk groups on the pretest and exam 1. There were also no significant differences between the groups on the three exams during the study or on the posttest and final class grades.

Subjective Measures

Descriptive statistics for all subjective measures are reported in Table 1. Based on the preliminary survey, the participants in the FitDesk group were not significantly different from the traditional desk group on motivation to do well in the class, study habits, exercise routines, sleep habits, or physical health.

Weekly survey results showed that participants using the FitDesk reported greater physical exertion when studying than the participants using the traditional desk, t(115) = 3.08, p = 0.003. It is important to note; however, that although the FitDesk users reported higher levels of perceived physical exertion, their average subjective rating was only “weak” physical exertion on the RPE. For comparison, the traditional desk users reported a subjective rating of “very weak” physical exertion. Traditional desk users reported feeling more motivated than those using the FitDesk, t(115) = 2.56, p = 0.012. However, there was no significant difference in the global morale score or global engagement between groups. Traditional desk users also reported feeling more committed to studying, t(115) = 2.67, p = 0.009, as well as more able to get completely absorbed in studying, t(115) = 4.09, p < 0.001.

Final survey results showed no significant differences between groups in motivation, focus, feeling of preparedness for the tests, or feeling that the type of desk used improved performance in the class. There were also no significant differences between the two groups in the time spent studying for class outside of the prescribed desk times. Traditional desk users reported feeling that they were more successful in studying, t(115) = 2.40, p = 0.018, and reported that studying at their desk was more effective, t(115) = 2.52, p = 0.013, than the FitDesk users. Traditional desk users also reported that the 2-h weekly prescribed study time helped them perform better in class than FitDesk users did, t(115) = 2.40, p = 0.018. Finally, both groups agreed that they would use their respective desks in the future when studying, though the traditional desk users reported higher levels than FitDesk users, t(115) = 2.49, p = 0.014.

Independent samples t-tests indicated that there was no significant difference in sleep quantity or sleep quality between groups when averaging across the 10 weeks of the study. However, sleep quality was decreasing toward the end of the semester in the traditional desk group (Figure 1). The 2 × 2 mixed factors ANOVA showed no main effects or interaction between time and desk condition on sleep quantity. In contrast, there was a main effect of desk condition on sleep quality. FitDesk users' sleep quality was stable between the first half and second half of the study while participants using a traditional desk reported a significant decrease in sleep quality from the first half to the second half of the study, F(1, 57) = 7.82, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.121 (Table 2). There was no main effect of time or significant interaction between groups on sleep quality.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Sleep Quality across the first 5 weeks and second 5 weeks of the study. Values indicated as mean ± SE ranging from 1 (very poor) to 5 (very good).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Sleep Survey items and descriptive statistics.

Discussion

The current results indicate that individuals studied for equal periods of time on the FitDesk and traditional desks and those who studied at the FitDesk performed equally well on course exams as individuals who studied at a traditional desk. As expected, the FitDesk participants reported greater levels of physical exertion on the weekly surveys; however, their level of exertion was reported as “weak” on the RPE. In general, on the weekly and final surveys, the traditional desk participants reported being better able to study. This was seen in greater levels of motivation, commitment, and being absorbed on the weekly surveys as well as feeling more successful in studying and that studying at the traditional desk was more effective on the final survey. However, it is important to note that not all subjective measures were enhanced in the traditional desk group. The findings from the weekly surveys suggest that morale and engagement were the same in both groups and the findings from the final survey suggest there was no difference in motivation, focus, feeling of preparedness, or feeling that the type of desk improved performance. Finally, when collapsing across the 10-week study, there was no difference in either sleep quantity or sleep quality between the two conditions. When examining the last 5 weeks of the study; however, there was a decrease in sleep quality in the traditional desk participants but not the FitDesk participants.

The current findings provide support for our first hypothesis that studying at an activity workstation would not negatively impact academic performance. The FitDesk and traditional desk groups were equivalent in terms of performance in the course prior to the onset of the study and did not differ in the amount of time that they studied outside of the required desk times in the library. As such, the current data suggest that students could be less sedentary by increasing light physical activity when studying, a normally sedentary task, without negatively affecting academic performance. The current results also support conclusions from previous studies suggesting that activity workstations do not negatively impact cognitive performance (Carr et al., 2014; Pilcher and Baker, 2016).

It should be noted that the FitDesk group reported more time studying outside of the mandatory study periods in the library; however, this difference was insignificant due to a large degree of variability among the subjects. Because there is no significant difference between the FitDesk and traditional desk groups time studying outside of the library, it is difficult to interpret these results. It is possible that some students using the FitDesk felt that they were less attentive when studying and needed extra study time. Additional studies can be designed to more fully address this issue. Furthermore, other researchers have found a possible link between type of exercise and type of cognitive performance (Chang et al., 2012) indicating that the effects of using activity workstations may depend on the level of physical exertion and on specific task characteristics. The current study indicates that adding light physical activity when studying for 2 h a week is unlikely to have a strong positive or negative effect on course grades. Thus, adding activity workstations to a normally sedentary environment (e.g., a university library) could allow college students to become more active when studying and could be one method of improving health-related behaviors in young adults.

In general the traditional desk participants in the current study felt that they were more motivated and successful at studying. However, the traditional desk participants did not do better on the tests nor were they studying significantly more than the FitDesk participants, suggesting that their subjective responses could have been affected by the normalcy of studying at a traditional desk. As such, this difference could be another example of the disconnect seen in many college students between what is effective studying and what simply feels easier (Bjork et al., 2013; Dunlosky et al., 2013). Students often use an ease-of-acquisition heuristic when studying which results in an illusion of having learned the material because it feels easier (Kornell and Bjork, 2009), yet without having actually learned and understood the material. It is possible that using the FitDesk also creates a feeling of working harder; however, academic performance was not negatively impacted in the FitDesk group in the current study. Given the potential health and well-being benefits of participating in light physical activity while studying, it is important to determine the potential effects of using activity workstations and how they could be more fully implemented in university and other sedentary environments.

In spite of no changes in sleep quantity in response to desk condition, the sleep quality results in the current study indicate that light activity when studying could benefit sleep. This could be an important finding since the majority of college students report some level of sleep-related disturbances (Buboltz et al., 2001; Forquer et al., 2008) and sleep quality in college students is related to health and well-being (Pilcher et al., 1997). Furthermore, sleep could be related to blood pressure control (McCubbin et al., 2010) and parasympathetic activity (Walker et al., 2009), suggesting that stabilizing sleep quality could positively impact long-term health in college students. This is the first study that reports a possible link between changes in sleep quality with light physical activity when studying in college students. More research is needed to determine if increasing light physical activity in college students positively impacts sleep and other measures of health and well-being.

The current study has several limitations. One limitation was the participants used the FitDesk and traditional desk in the university library and were not monitored at all times. This allowed the participants greater accessibility to the study space and more naturalistic findings; however, it was possible for the participants to report that they were using a desk but not do so. The use of QR codes helped control for this natural limitation of a field-based study but future studies could be designed with more checks to better ensure that participants are using the desks when they scan the QR codes. A second limitation is the participants were asked to only study for their introductory psychology class during their study sessions; however, there were limits in how well we could control what the participants actually did when using the desks. To help control for this, we asked students to briefly summarize what they studied when they logged out and all students reported studying for their introductory psychology class. It seems that neither group would be more or less likely to follow the instructions and both groups did equally well on the course tests suggesting that this potential issue did not negatively affect the current results. Another possible limitation is that we did not attempt to monitor or limit participants based on current body weight or current levels of physical fitness. It must be noted, however, that we did include several questions about exercise and exercise habits in the preliminary studies and did not find any differences between the two groups. Future studies could be designed to control for body weight and other health-related issues. Finally, because the sleep measures were taken every week, we chose to use three questions as is done in many sleep research studies to limit the time and effort needed by the participants when completing the sleep logs (Pilcher et al., 2007; Odle-Dusseau et al., 2010). Future studies can be designed to use more detailed sleep surveys such as the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index.

Conclusions

Our findings provide a unique comparison between the use of active and traditional workstations across an academic semester in a normally sedentary environment. FitDesk users experienced the potential benefits of increased light physical activity without sacrificing academic performance. Traditional desk users reported more subjective ease while studying, but this did not lead to better academic performance and may only highlight a misunderstanding associated with effective studying for many college students. These results suggest that implementing activity workstations on college campuses could have health and well-being benefits for the students without negatively affecting academic performance. Furthermore, research suggests that the appropriate use of cognitive control could lead to a less sedentary lifestyle (Buckley et al., 2014) and more positive sleep behaviors (Pilcher et al., 2015). Making active workstations available in normally sedentary environments could provide an option for a less sedentary lifestyle and more positive health behaviors. It is important to note that one explanation for some of the similarities between the FitDesk and traditional desk groups on some of the variables in the current study could be the limited required time studying at the assigned desk. For the purposes of this study, we choose 2 h to better fit into a variety of student study schedules. Future studies could examine the possible effects of a longer study period comparing the use of a traditional desk to the FitDesk. More research is also needed investigating how activity workstations can be implemented on college campuses and in other sedentary environments such as doctors' waiting rooms, waiting areas in airports, and sedentary job settings. The current results suggests that activity workstations may be a tool for combating sedentary lifestyle behavior in modern society without interrupting daily routine or negatively impacting performance.

Ethics Statement

Clemson University's institutional review board approved the study. All participants signed the informed consent form prior to the start of the study.

Author Contributions

JP conceived of the research question, designed the research protocol, oversaw data collection, organization, and analyses, and wrote the paper. DM assisted with data management and analyses and with writing the paper. SB assisted with the research protocol, and data collection, organization, and analyses and manuscript revision. PM assisted with data analyses and with writing the paper. HF assisted with the research protocol and data management and manuscript revision. All authors have approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

Funding was provided by the Creative Inquiry and Undergraduate Research program at Clemson University.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Rosaria C. Bryan, Cameron N. Drummond, Vanessa K. MacPherson, and Jesse H. Walker for their assistance with data collection and data management.

References

Bjork, R. A., Dunlosky, J., and Kornell, N. (2013). Self-regulated learning: beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 417–444. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borg, G. (1990). Psychophysical scaling with applications in physical work and the perception of exertion. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 16, 55–58. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.1815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borg, G. A. (1982). Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 14, 377–381. doi: 10.1249/00005768-198205000-00012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Britt, T. W., Adler, A. B., Bliese, P. D., and Moore, D. (2013). Morale as a moderator of the combat exposure – PTSD symptom relationship. J. Trauma Stress 26, 1–8. doi: 10.1002/jts.21775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Britt, T. W., McKibben, E. S., Greene-Shortridge, T. M., Beeco, A., Bodin, A., Calcaterra, J., et al. (2010). Self-engagement as a predictor of performance and emotional reactions to performance outcomes. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 49, 237–257. doi: 10.1348/014466609X438090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buboltz, W. C., Brown, F., and Soper, B. (2001). Sleep habits and patterns of college students: a preliminary study. J. Am. Coll. Health 50, 131–135. doi: 10.1080/07448480109596017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckley, J., Cohen, J. D., Kramer, A. F., McAuley, E., and Mullen, S. P. (2014). Cognitive control in the self-regulation of physical activity and sedentary behavior. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:747. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, L. J., Maeda, H., Luther, B., Rider, P., Tucker, S. J., and Leonhard, C. (2014). Acceptability and effects of a seated active workstation during sedentary work: a proof of concept study. Int. J. Workplace Health Manag. 7, 2–15. doi: 10.1108/IJWHM-03-2013-0008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carson, V., Wong, S. L., Winkler, E., Healy, G. N., Colley, R. C., and Tremblay, M. S. (2014). Patterns of sedentary time and cardiometabolic risk among Canadian adults. Prev. Med. 65, 23–27. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, Y. K., Labban, J. D., Gapin, J. I., and Etnier, J. L. (2012). The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Brain Res. 1453, 87–101. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, M. J., Fan, X., and Moe, S. T. (2002). Criterion-related validity of the Borg ratings of perceived exertion scale in healthy individuals: a meta-analysis. J. Sports Sci. 20, 873–899. doi: 10.1080/026404102320761787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. H., Guth, J., Siekemeyer, L., Kellems, B., Brehm, S. B., and Ohlinger, C. M. (2011). Metabolic cost and speech quality while using an active workstation. J. Phys. Act. Health 8, 332–339. doi: 10.1123/jpah.8.3.332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., and Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students' learning with effective learning techniques: promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 14, 4–58. doi: 10.1177/1529100612453266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forquer, L. M., Camden, A. E., Gabriau, K. M., and Johnson, M. (2008). Sleep patterns of college students at a public university. J. Am. Coll. Health 56, 563–565. doi: 10.3200/JACH.56.5.563-565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fountaine, C. J., Liguori, G. A., Mozumdar, A., and Schuna, J. M. Jr. (2011). Physical activity and screen time sedentary behaviours in college students. Int. J. Exerc. Sci. 4, 102–112.

Google Scholar

Fox, K. R. (1999). The influence of physical activity on mental well-being. Public Health Nutr. 2, 411–418. doi: 10.1017/S1368980099000567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Healy, G. N., Wijndaele, K., Dunstan, D. W., Shaw, J. E., Salmon, J., Zimmet, P. Z., et al. (2008). Objectively measured sedentary time, physical activity, and metabolic risk. Diabetes Care 31, 369–371. doi: 10.2337/dc07-1795

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, J. O., Wyatt, H. R., Reed, G. W., and Peters, J. C. (2003). Obesity and the environment: where do we go from here? Science 299, 853–855. doi: 10.1126/science.1079857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogan, C. L., Catalino, L. I., Mata, J., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2015). Beyond emotional benefits: physical activity and sedentary behaviour affect psychosocial resources through emotions. Psychol. Health 30, 354–369. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2014.973410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishizaki, M., Morikawa, Y., Nakagawa, H., Honda, R., Kawakami, N., Haratani, T., et al. (2004). The influence of work characteristics on body mass index and waist to hip ratio in Japanese employees. Ind. Health 42, 41–49. doi: 10.2486/indhealth.42.41

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kornell, N., and Bjork, R. A. (2009). A stability bias in human memory: overestimating remembering and underestimating learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 138, 449–468. doi: 10.1037/a0017350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mansoubi, M., Pearson, N., Biddle, S. J., and Clemes, S. (2014). The relationship between sedentary behaviour and physical activity in adults: a systematic review. Prev. Med. 69, 28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.08.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marchard, G. C., Nardi, N. M., Reynolds, D., and Pamoukov, S. (2014). The impact of the classroom built environment on student perceptions and learning. J. Environ. Psychol. 40, 187–197. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.06.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCubbin, J. A., Pilcher, J. J., and Moore, D. D. (2010). Blood pressure increases during a simulated night shift in persons at risk for hypertension. Int. J. Behav. Med. 17, 314–320. doi: 10.1007/s12529-010-9117-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mullineaux, D. R., Barnes, C. A., and Barnes, E. F. (2001). Factors affecting the likelihood to engage in adequate physical activity to promote health. J. Sports Sci. 19, 279–288. doi: 10.1080/026404101750158376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Odle-Dusseau, H. N., Bradley, J. L., and Pilcher, J. J. (2010). Subjective perceptions of the effects of sustained performance under sleep-deprivation conditions. Chronobiol. Int. 27, 318–333. doi: 10.3109/07420520903502226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oka, R. K., De Marco, T., Haskell, W. L., Botvinick, E., Dae, M. W., Bolen, K., et al. (2000). Impact of a home-based walking and resistance training program on quality of life in patients with heart failure. Am. J. Cardiol. 85, 365–369. doi: 10.1016/S0002-9149(99)00748-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owen, N., Bauman, A., and Brown, W. (2009). Too much sitting: a novel and important predictor of chronic disease risk? Br. J. Sports Med. 43, 81–83. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2008.055269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pengpid, S., Peltzer, K., Kassean, H. K., Tsala, J. P., Sychareun, V., and Müller-Riemenschneider, F. (2015). Physical inactivity and associated factors among university students in 23 low-, middle- and high-income countries. Int. J. Public Health 60, 539–549. doi: 10.1007/s00038-015-0680-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pilcher, J. J., and Baker, V. C. (2016). Task performance and meta-cognitive outcomes when using activity workstations and traditional desks. Front. Psychol. 7:957. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pilcher, J. J., Band, D., Odle-Dusseau, H. N., and Muth, E. R. (2007). Human performance under sustained operations and sleep deprivation conditions: toward a model of controlled attention. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 78(5 Suppl.), B15–B24.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Pilcher, J. J., Ginter, D. R., and Sadowsky, B. (1997). Sleep quality versus sleep quantity: relationships between sleep and measures of health, well-being and sleepiness in college students. J. Psychosom. Res. 42, 583–596. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(97)00004-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pilcher, J. J., Morris, D. M., Donnelly, J., and Feigl, H. B. (2015). Interactions between sleep habits and self-control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:284. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rebar, A. L., Vandelanotte, C., van Uffelen, J., Short, C., and Duncan, M. J. (2014). Associations of overall sitting time and sitting time in different contexts with depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 7, 105–110. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2014.02.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, R. E., Mark, R. S., and Temmel, C. P. (2012). Adult sedentary behaviour: a systematic review. Am. J. Prev. Med. 42, e3–e28. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2011.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, L. P., Ng, S. W., and Popkin, B. M. (2014). No time for the gym? Housework and other non-labor market time use patterns are associated with meeting physical activity recommendations among adults in full-time, sedentary jobs. Soc. Sci. Med. 120, 126–134. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stone, E. J., McKenzie, T. L., Welk, G. J., and Booth, M. (1998). Effects of physical activity interventions in youth: review and synthesis. Am. J. Prev. Med. 15, 298–315. doi: 10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00082-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Straker, L., Levine, J., and Campbell, A. (2009). The effects of walking and cycling computer workstations on keyboard and mouse performance. Hum. Factors 51, 831–844. doi: 10.1177/0018720810362079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torbeyns, T., Bailey, S., Bos, I., and Meeusen, R. (2014). Active workstations to fight sedentary behaviour. Sports Med. 44, 1261–1273. doi: 10.1007/s40279-014-0202-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tudor-Locke, C., and Schuna, J. M. (2012). Steps to preventing Type 2 diabetes: exercise, walk more, or sit less? Front. Endocrinol. 3:142. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2012.00142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tudor-Locke, C., Schuna, J. M., Frensham, L. J., and Proenca, M. (2014). Changing the way we work: elevating energy expenditure with work-station alternatives. Int. J. Obes. 38, 755–765. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, M. W., Carr, L. J., Clark, R., and Weng, T. (2014). Revenge of the “sit” II: Does lifestyle impact neuronal and cognitive health through distinct mechanisms associated with sedentary behavior and physical activity? Ment. Health Phys. Act. 7, 9–24. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2014.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, A. D., Muth, E. R., Odle-Dusseau, H. N., Moore, D. W., and Pilcher, J. J. (2009). The effects of 28 hours of sleep deprivation on respiratory sinus arrhythmia during tasks with low and high controlled attention demands. Psychophysiology 46, 217–224. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00718.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warburton, D. E. R., Nicol, C. W., and Bredin, S. S. D. (2006). Health benefits of physical activity: the evidence. Can. Med. Assoc. J. 174, 801–809. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.051351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sedentary behavior, physical activity, performance, sleep, motivation, activity workstations

Citation: Pilcher JJ, Morris DM, Bryant SA, Merritt PA and Feigl HB (2017) Decreasing Sedentary Behavior: Effects on Academic Performance, Meta-Cognition, and Sleep. Front. Neurosci. 11:219. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00219

Received: 17 January 2017; Accepted: 31 March 2017;
Published: 09 May 2017.

Edited by:

Jan Polák, Charles University in Prague, Czechia

Reviewed by:

Melissa Irene March, University Hospitals Case Medical Center, USA
Magdalena Alicja Maj, Baylor College of Medicine, USA

Copyright © 2017 Pilcher, Morris, Bryant, Merritt and Feigl. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: June J. Pilcher, jpilche@clemson.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.