Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Integr. Neurosci., 22 August 2014
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2014.00066

Frontal eye field, where art thou? Anatomy, function, and non-invasive manipulation of frontal regions involved in eye movements and associated cognitive operations

  • 1Centre de Recherche de l'Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, CNRS UMR 7225, INSERM UMRS 975 and Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Paris, France
  • 2Laboratory for Cerebral Dynamics Plasticity and Rehabilitation, School of Medicine, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA
  • 3Cognitive Neuroscience and Information Technology Research Program, Open University of Catalonia, Barcelona, Spain

The planning, control and execution of eye movements in 3D space relies on a distributed system of cortical and subcortical brain regions. Within this network, the Eye Fields have been described in animals as cortical regions in which electrical stimulation is able to trigger eye movements and influence their latency or accuracy. This review focuses on the Frontal Eye Field (FEF) a “hub” region located in Humans in the vicinity of the pre-central sulcus and the dorsal-most portion of the superior frontal sulcus. The straightforward localization of the FEF through electrical stimulation in animals is difficult to translate to the healthy human brain, particularly with non-invasive neuroimaging techniques. Hence, in the first part of this review, we describe attempts made to characterize the anatomical localization of this area in the human brain. The outcome of functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI), Magneto-encephalography (MEG) and particularly, non-invasive mapping methods such a Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) are described and the variability of FEF localization across individuals and mapping techniques are discussed. In the second part of this review, we will address the role of the FEF. We explore its involvement both in the physiology of fixation, saccade, pursuit, and vergence movements and in associated cognitive processes such as attentional orienting, visual awareness and perceptual modulation. Finally in the third part, we review recent evidence suggesting the high level of malleability and plasticity of these regions and associated networks to non-invasive stimulation. The exploratory, diagnostic, and therapeutic interest of such interventions for the modulation and improvement of perception in 3D space are discussed.

Introduction: FEF, a Crossroads for Eye Movements and Visuo-Spatial Cognition

The frontal eye field (FEF) is an area of the frontal cortex in animals over which electrical stimulation is able to trigger eye movements. Electrophysiological studies in the monkey defined the FEF as an area containing visual, motor, and visuo-motor cells (Bruce and Goldberg, 1985) essential for the preparation and triggering of eye movements. This site operates as a crucial site of networks integrating other regions located in widespread locations. In humans for example, such gaze control systems include in the frontal lobe the supplementary eye field (SEF), the pre-supplementary eye field (pre-SEF), the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), the cingulate eye field (CEF) within the anterior cingulate cortex and the dorso-medial frontal cortex, and in the parietal lobe, the parietal eye field (PEF) and areas of the posterior parietal cortex (PPC). Finally, subcortical structures, such as the superior colliculus (SC) in the midbrain are also considered essential to trigger eye movements. All these areas operate cooperatively, nonetheless some of them contribute to the triggering of eye movements under specific situations: the PEF for example has a role in reflexive saccades, the FEF participates in voluntary saccades, the SEF contributes to the development of more complex motor programs involving gaze (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 2002). Other areas, such as the CEF and the DLPFC, are more generally dedicated to cognitive aspects (e.g., motivation, memory) of oculomotor control (Gaymard et al., 1998b).

The anatomy of input and output projections within nodes of this network has been particularly well characterized in the monkey brain, and has revealed itself as a highly complex constellation of widespread interactions. The predominant neural inputs to the FEF originate in other cortical eye fields, including the SEF, the PEF, the middle superior temporal area, and the principal sulcus region (Schall et al., 1993; Tian and Lynch, 1996). The FEF also receives weak connections from the middle temporal area (MT), which may act as a relay between the striate / extrastriate cortices and the parietal cortex and FEF (Tian and Lynch, 1996). The FEF projects to many areas within the frontal cortex (Stanton et al., 1993), the occipital and parietal cortices such as V2/V3/V4, the middle temporal area (MT), the medial superior temporal area (MST) and the superior temporal visual area (Stanton et al., 1995). Finally, important reciprocal connections have been demonstrated between the FEF and the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) and more generally with the parietal cortex (Huerta et al., 1987; Cavada and Goldman-Rakic, 1989; Stanton et al., 1995; Tian and Lynch, 1996). Subcortically, the FEF projects directly to the brainstem (pons) (Leichnetz et al., 1984; Segraves, 1992). It also sends afferents to the SC (Schlag-Rey et al., 1992), either directly (Segraves and Goldberg, 1987) or indirectly via the basal ganglia (Stanton et al., 1988), and to other subcortical nuclei within the thalamus, subthalamus and tegmentum (Stanton et al., 1988). The FEF receives inputs from subcortical sites, including the substantia nigra, the SC. Finally, the cerebellum projects to thalamic regions innervating the FEF (Lynch et al., 1994).

Most of the earlier knowledge about the FEF was built-up on the basis of non-human primates experiments. A major emphasis has been put on the role of the FEF in the preparation and execution of saccades (Bizzi, 1968; Bruce and Goldberg, 1985). However, FEF also participates in the control of all the other types of eye movements, such as smooth pursuit or optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) (Bizzi, 1968; MacAvoy et al., 1991) and fixation (Izawa et al., 2004a,b, 2009). The intracortical stimulation of several subareas within the FEF is also able to trigger vergence movements (changes of the depth of the gaze) (Crosby et al., 1952, cited by Robinson and Fuchs, 1969). More recently, Gamlin and Yoon (2000) showed that a region within the pre-arcuate cortex in rhesus monkeys, immediately rostral to the saccade-related region in the anterior bank of the arcuate sulcus, is involved in vergence, accommodation and the sensorimotor transformations required for these movements. Moreover, Ferraina et al. (2000) showed that most neurons in a region of the anterior bank of the arcuate sulcus where saccades could be evoked with low current stimulation were also sensitive to disparity. The caudal portion of the FEF that contains smooth pursuit neurons also carries binocular signals related to vergence movement (Kurkin et al., 2003) and the majority of FEF pursuit neurons would respond to both frontal pursuit and pursuit in depth (Fukushima et al., 2002). There is also evidence that near and far spaces are differentially encoded in the frontal cortex including the FEF (Pigarev et al., 1979; Rizzolatti et al., 1983). Thus, the FEF appears to be involved in every sort of eye movements in 3D space.

It is expected that human FEF will be recruited, as in animals, for all types of ocular behavior: saccades, fixation, smooth pursuit, OKN, vergence. However, within each type, the specific experimental set-ups conditioning different categories of eye movements (e.g., reflexive, voluntary) will modulate the involvement of the FEF. Indeed, different cortical oculomotor areas are differentially recruited according to the category of eye movements (Gaymard et al., 1998a). The fact that the cognitive context is modulating the involvement of the FEF is reminiscent of the other roles played by the FEF in visuo-spatial attention, visual awareness, and perceptual modulation.

Before entering into the details on the various roles of the FEF (part II) and how its activity can be modulated for clinical purposes (part III), we will describe the efforts made to localize this area in both non-human and human primates (part I). As expected from an area contributing to numerous functions, the exact localization will strongly depend on the methods and specific paradigms used to assess it.

Localization of FEF

The primate FEF is defined physiologically as the portion of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex from which low-intensity intracortical stimulation is able to elicit rapid eye movements. Using this invasive approach, the monkey FEF has been located by some studies in the frontal lobe along the anterior border of the arcuate fissure, which would correspond to Brodmann's area 8, or overlapping with both areas 8 and 6 (or, using Walker's nomenclature, with areas 8A and 45) (for a review, see Tehovnik et al., 2000). According to the results of neuroimaging studies, the human FEF is mostly thought to be located in the superior pre-central sulcus near the caudal end of the superior frontal sulcus, which corresponds to Brodmann's area 6. However, as will be described in the second part of this review, the FEF contributes not only to several aspects of eye movements but also to different cognitive domains, and the exact location of the FEF will strongly depend not only on the methods (e.g., stimulation vs. neuroimaging) but also on the tasks (e.g., type of eye movements and type of control conditions, see e.g., Paus, 1996) and activation criteria (e.g., intensity of stimulation, see Blanke et al., 2000 for a discussion) used. Overall, it is still not entirely clear whether the reported inter-species differences in FEF location can be related to genuine anatomical differences between non-human primates and humans, caused by the use of different mapping methods or they simply reflect interindividual differences, which have not always been systematically studied in large cohorts of animals and human participants.

In the next pages, we will review some of the numerous studies that have attempted to determine the anatomical location of the FEF employing: microstimulation, intracranial recordings, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), positron emission tomography (PET), magnetoencephalography (MEG) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). A summary of the localizations reported in these studies can be found in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Localization of FEF across studies, techniques and species.

Microstimulation and Recordings in Non-Human Primates: the Original Definition

In 1874, Ferrier summarized stimulation studies performed on several animal species including cats, dogs and rabbits as follows: “In the superior frontal convolution, in advance of the centre for certain forward movements of the arm, as well as in the corresponding part of the middle frontal convolution, are areas, stimulation of which causes lateral (crossed) movements of the head and eyes and dilatation of the pupils.” (Ferrier, 1874).

More than one hundred years later, other microstimulation studies evoking eye movements (Robinson and Fuchs, 1969; MacAvoy et al., 1991; Gottlieb et al., 1993; Izawa et al., 2004a,b, 2009), electrophysiological recordings during visual stimulation and/or eye movements (Bizzi, 1968; Wurtz and Mohler, 1976; Bruce and Goldberg, 1985; Segraves and Goldberg, 1987) and studies comparing cells discharge patterns during behavior or its alteration during the stimulation of these same neuronal populations (Bruce et al., 1985; Gottlieb et al., 1994) confirmed the existence of an FEF located in the posterior part of the pre-arcuate sulcus. They distinguished visual (modulated by functional significance), motor and visuo-motor neural populations for saccade, pursuit and fixation/saccade suppression, somehow spatially segregated and with different stimulation thresholds, which depended on the activation state of the monkey at the time of the stimulation. Each sub-region showed its specific organization. For saccades for instance, stimulation of ventro-lateral regions evoked small amplitude saccade whereas stimulation of dorso-medial regions induced large saccades; moreover the direction of the saccades varied as a function of the depth of stimulation in the arcuate sulcus (Tehovnik et al., 2000). Interestingly there is also evidence that in some primate species (e.g., owl monkeys), the stimulation of the dorsal premotor area, posterior to the usually defined FEF, can also evoke saccades, suggesting that such posterior area, potentially closer to the human FEF, could also belong to the non-human primates FEF (Preuss et al., 1996).

Although microstimulation is considered a gold standard technique to reveal a causal relation between a region and a brain function, it has potential limitations (see Amiez and Petrides, 2009; for review). First, the extent and number of responding areas depends on stimulation intensity, whose traditional threshold level (50 μ) is set up arbitrarily. Second, within the same study or across studies and depending on the experimental design chosen, some cortical areas have been less systematically sampled than others, a fact that could have biased output maps overemphasizing the role of certain locations while undermining the contribution of others. Third and last, intracortical stimulation can evoke eye movements from direct FEF activation, but also by activating intracortical white matter pathways connecting the FEF to other areas (Luna et al., 1998), a phenomenon that could easily blur the borders of cortical representations and lead to mislocalizations.

Microstimulation in Humans

Microstimulation procedures have not been solely restricted to a use in animal models. They have also been occasionally performed in epileptic patients, either per-operatively or outside of surgery rooms in more ecological conditions via chronically implanted subdural electrodes in fully awake patients. Using the first procedure, Foerster (1936, cited by Blanke et al., 2000) induced eye movements only from the posterior part of the middle frontal gyrus, whereas Rasmussen and Penfield (1948 cited by Blanke et al., 2000) report to have induced similar effects from all frontal gyri and the pre-central gyrus. With implanted subdural electrodes at the level of and in front of the motor representation, Godoy et al. (1990) evoked contralateral conjugate eye movements (mostly saccades), and sometimes accompanying head version following eye deviation. Blanke et al. (2000) investigated systematically the current intensity needed to elicit unilateral eye movements and found, consistently with monkey studies, that the eye fields inducing saccades and smooth eye movements are located in the posterior part of the middle frontal gyrus and neighboring portions of the superior frontal gyrus but not in the inferior frontal gyrus or in the pre-central sulcus.

Thus, microstimulation in well-controlled settings in human patients can yield results equivalent to those demonstrated in non-human primates with similar interventions. As also mentioned above for the animal, the intensity used for intracortical stimulation in humans arbitrarily determines the number and the size of the cortical clusters that activated directly or indirectly by connectivity are ultimately causally associated to the FEF. In addition, such studies are also constrained by the spatial location, distribution, and coverage of the implanted electrodes, which are strictly guided on the basis of clinical and not scientific criteria, and limited by the scarcity of time available for testing and the lack of large cohort of similarly implanted patients available to provide statistical evidence. Moreover, for ethical reasons, such procedures are only performed in human patients who have undergone developmental or acquired anatomical and functional alterations and do not necessarily provide accurate information on the healthy brain. In view of such limitations, non-invasive neuroimaging techniques, such as PET, fMRI, MEG and also non-invasive neurostimulation by TMS have become particularly popular in cognitive neuroanatomy and have been employed in the quest to locate the FEF in both humans and to a lesser extent in animals.

Neuroimaging

The spreading of neuroimaging techniques such as PET and more recently fMRI has allowed the evaluation of FEF location and function in healthy human brains. The gradual increase of spatial resolution has permitted defining progressively smaller and better-delimited regions corresponding to the FEF. Within the large FEF region characterized by means of PET, several subareas associated with eye movements have been revealed using fMRI.

The variability of FEF location and function, found across different PET studies, has been reviewed by Paus (1996). Pioneering explorations using PET reported large activations in the human lateral frontal cortex during saccade execution. Most of these studies defined the FEF as part of the pre-central sulcus in the frontal lobe (Petit et al., 1995, 1996). Nonetheless this region has been sometimes localized in the anterior portion of the pre-central gyrus around the pre-central sulcus (Fox et al., 1985; Anderson et al., 1994; Law et al., 1997), or within the posterior portion of the pre-central gyrus around the central sulcus (Sweeney et al., 1996). A large range of eye movement types have shown to activate the FEF: fixation (Petit et al., 1995), reflexive or memory saccades (Anderson et al., 1994), saccades with or without visual cues (Fox et al., 1985), suppressed or imagined saccades (Law et al., 1997), anti-saccades (O'Driscoll et al., 1995; Sweeney et al., 1996), predictive saccades and gaze pursuit (O'Driscoll et al., 2000). Some of these studies showed that the intensity of FEF activation was neither influenced by target presence, cue type, task complexity (Fox et al., 1985) nor by whether the saccades were voluntary or previously learned (Petit et al., 1996), whereas other studies showed, on the contrary, a modulation of FEF activation from fixation to reflexive or volitional saccades (O'Driscoll et al., 1995; Sweeney et al., 1996).

The higher spatial resolution of fMRI recordings in humans has allowed researchers to restrict the site hosting the FEF along the pre-central sulcus (Darby et al., 1996; Muri et al., 1996). It also permitted to identify within this sulcus, several sub-areas subtending potentially distinct functions related to saccadic activity. Petit and Haxby (1999) and Petit et al. (1997) reported the FEF as located at the junction of the pre-central sulcus and the superior frontal sulcus extending laterally to the pre-central gyrus. They described a saccade-related FEF and a smaller, more inferior, and more lateral gaze pursuit-related FEF, which according to another study could overlap (Berman et al., 1999). Rosano et al. (2002) found a restricted area within the pre-central sulcus, integrating the saccade area, as located mainly on the rostral bank close to the cortical surface, and the pursuit area situated deeper in the sulcus, suggesting similar superficial/deep activation as the one characterizing non-human primates. Activation restricted to the pre-central sulcus was also shown in individual subjects in the study from Luna et al. (1998) contrasting simple visually-guided saccades to fixation. They described a consistent activation of the superior portion of the pre-central sulcus and a less consistent activation of the inferior portion of the pre-central sulcus. Similarly, different clusters of activation within the pre-central sulcus were found in other studies (Corbetta et al., 1998; Beauchamp et al., 2001). A meta-analysis performed on PET and fMRI datasets confirmed that for both visually and voluntarily-triggered saccades, the FEF lies in the pre-central sulcus close to its intersection with the superior frontal sulcus, potentially extending onto the superior and inferior subregions of the superficial portions of the pre-central gyrus (Grosbras et al., 2005). It should be noted, however, that some recent studies localized the superior FEF within the ventral portion of the superior pre-central sulcus, either at the end or at the most posterior region of the middle frontal gyrus, instead of at the level of its intersection with the superior frontal sulcus (Amiez et al., 2006).

Neuroimaging approaches show some limitations as compared to neurostimulation methods to determine the brain regions involved in a given saccadic behavior. First, neuroimaging methods are less sensitive than neurostimulation approaches in the detection of small saccade-related areas (Luna et al., 1998); second, group-averaging strategies employed in neuroimaging approaches to increase statistical power may come at the risk of shifting activation sites in case of strong interindividual anatomical differences (Luna et al., 1998; Amiez and Petrides, 2009); third, whereas brain stimulation mostly reveals contralaterally-evoked saccades, fMRI studies are built on protocols embedding bilateral and repetitive eye movements conditions and compared to a gaze fixation baseline. Hence differences in region size and shifted FEF localizations (Blanke et al., 2000) could be well caused by either the influence in contrast analyses from cells within the FEF involved in fixation and/or the mix up of activity related to different saccade directions within the same analyses. Another important concern raised by Tehovnik et al. (2000) and Amiez and Petrides (2009) is that in neuroimaging protocols, no instruction is given regarding blinking and return-to-center saccades in between trials (which is often accompanied by blinks). This could also explain rather posterior mislocalizations of the FEF, which would mistakenly encompass activity from regions within the motor strip involved in eyelid motion. In favor of this possibility, a PET protocol with multiple saccades, inducing comparable blinks frequency in the saccade and the control condition, found activity within the middle frontal gyrus (Kawashima et al., 1998). This observation is consistent with a more anterior location for the FEF in the frontal lobe (Tehovnik et al., 2000) and argues in favor of important blinking-related biases in prior PET and fMRI explorations.

In spite of the above-mentioned problems, neuroimaging studies still have the advantage of providing normalized coordinates corresponding to group mean activation peaks (see Figure 1) that can be easily compared across studies and used as targets for subsequent non-invasive brain stimulation approaches on search of causality. In that vein, the meta-analysis of 8 PET studies involving 62 healthy participants designed by (Paus, 1996) suggested a reference location in Talairach coordinates (Table 2). Subsequent fMRI have contributed Tailarach coordinates reflecting similar or more posterior loci for the main (e.g. the superior) FEF site during the active performance of saccades or, on the contrary, more anterior location when blinks were avoided (Table 2).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Localization of FEF according to several studies on the MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) brain template viewed from top (A), front (B), right (C) and left (D). Color codes as follows. Green: meta-analysis of PET studies from Paus (1996); Blue: fMRI study of Luna et al. (1998); Red: fMRI study of Petit and Haxby (1999); Yellow: MEG study of Ioannides et al. (2004); Purple: coordinates estimated by Tehovnik et al. (2000) based on the PET study of Kawashima et al. (1998). A sphere of 1 cm radius is positioned at the center of FEF activation from each study. SPM (Statistical Parametric Mapping, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) with MarsBar toolbox was used to design the spheres and MRIcroGL software (http://www.mccauslandcenter.sc.edu/mricrogl/) was used for glass brain illustration.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Coordinates of left and right FEF from a few neuroimaging studies.

Reconciling Non-Human Primates' and Humans' Locations for the FEF?

In brief, the non-human primate FEF, localized mainly thanks to microstimulation studies, lies in a more rostral location (Brodmann's area 8) than the human FEF, localized mainly thanks to neuroimaging studies (Brodmann's area 6). A suggestion to reconcile such discrepancies between monkey and human reports is that the more posterior FEF location in humans has been erroneously attributed to Brodmann's area 6. Following that line, a study focused on the delimitation of cytoarchitectonics areas in post-mortem human brains containing the superior element of the pre-central sulcus and the caudal end of the superior frontal sulcus (Rosano et al., 2003). This study suggested that the pre-central sulcus might represent a transitional area between the rostral granular cortex and the caudal agranular cortex. Thus, the FEF would be located within a region that appears to have a similar chemoarchitecture (Stanton et al., 1989; Rosano et al., 2003) in both species, even if lying in a more caudal location in humans.

Other studies have suggested that discrepancies between monkeys and humans in FEF location arise from methodological differences rather than from a genuine inter-specie divergence. We already mentioned that microstimulation in humans can yield results equivalent to those demonstrated in non-human primates with similar interventions (see Section “Microstimulation in Humans). Do monkey fMRI recordings also reveal similar activations than the ones shown in humans with this same mapping technique? Koyama et al. (2004) conducted an fMRI study in macaque monkeys and revealed three saccade-related foci of activation. One was located in the bank of the arcuate sulcus, approximately in Brodmann's area 8, which corresponds to the classical non-human primate FEF, whereas the remaining two laid in premotor areas, and more precisely, in the inferior and superior precentral sulci within Brodmann's area 6. Thus, monkey fMRI studies reveal indeed activations similar to those found in humans. Further studies are needed to conclude on whether the discrepancy between non-human primates and humans results mainly arises from different cytoarchitectonics areas in different species or from the use of different methods. Probably, a deeper exploration of the multiple foci associated with the FEF will help to clarify its role and localization across species.

Finally, the use of a third methodology can shed a new light on the interpretation of results arising from monkeys' microstimulation and humans' fMRI studies. Taking advantage of the exquisite temporal resolution of MEG and the possibility of localizing source signals with a reasonable spatial resolution Ioannides et al. (2004, 2005) suggested an anterior location similar to the one found in microstimulation studies (e.g., in Ioannides et al., 2004 in 3 subjects, see Figure 1 and Table 2 for Talairach coordinates; however, note the high inter-individual variability of the Y coordinate between 24 and −3 for the right FEF). According to a MEG single subject study of this same group, the activity associated to the FEF could shift along a rostro-caudal axis, from the rostral site identified in microstimulation studies to the caudal region reported in fMRI studies, during the saccade preparation time (Ioannides et al., 2010), suggesting an unexpected confounding role of this variable. Most importantly, this study suggested that both the rostral (usually described for the non-human primates) and the caudal (usually described for the humans) sites can be identified in humans at different timing.

TMS: in Search of a Causal Functional Localizer in Healthy Humans

In order to overcome the limitations of invasive human microstimulation but still benefit from its causation power, some researchers have turned to TMS as a causal brain mapping technique. TMS is based on a non-invasive induction of small currents intracortically in order to modulate brain activity at specific cortical areas with a relatively good spatial resolution, in the order of 1.2–3.5 mm radius (Wagner et al., 2007; Bijsterbosch et al., 2012). Depending on variables such as the stimulated area, magnetic pulse intensity, pre or post event time window chosen for pulse delivery, or the temporal distribution of individual pulses employed either in short bursts or long stimulation patterns, TMS can have an immediate (i.e., the so-called online) or lasting (so-called offline) facilitatory or disruptive impact on neurophysiological activity and consequently on the performance driven by the targeted cortical region and its associated network of areas (Valero-Cabre et al., 2005, 2007). Thanks to these properties, this technique is used to explore the causal contribution of different cortical areas and associated anatomical systems to human behavior in healthy individuals, whereas in clinical applications TMS has been employed to manipulate patterns of activity and drive therapeutically interesting outcomes for neurological or neuropsychiatric conditions (Valero-Cabre et al., 2011).

As TMS operates by using a magnetic field to non-invasively induce electrical current within the cortex, it has been hypothesized that, as intracranial electrical stimulation does, magnetic stimulation should also be able to trigger eye movements. However, TMS delivered systematically into frontal locations where the FEF is located has surprisingly proven unable to trigger eye movements (Muri et al., 1991; Wessel and Kompf, 1991), to disturb central fixation (Zangemeister et al., 1995) or to modify saccade or smooth pursuit movement in flight (Wessel and Kompf, 1991). Only under facilitating conditions, e.g., during the performance of a double-step saccade task, has rTMS been reported to be able to induce multistep short-latency eye movements in a few subjects (Li et al., 1997). This result strongly suggests that the organization of the systems within the FEF devoted to eye movement is different than that characterizing the primary motor cortex for limb movements. Indeed, the latter projects directly to spinal motor neurons and can thus easily trigger hand movements when the primary motor cortex is stimulated with TMS. In contrast, circuits leading to gaze movements include intermediate synaptic chains and structures and hence might not be that easy to activate with the same technique. Additionally, it has been also argued that such differences in activation could also be attributed to the fact that the TMS-induced currents may have been either insufficiently high or too poorly focalized to effectively activate polysynaptic chains down to saccadic motor neurons (Muri et al., 1991; Wessel and Kompf, 1991).

Although TMS cannot directly induce eye movements in healthy humans, it can effectively interfere with the processing of visually and non-visually guided saccades. Such modulatory phenomena have been employed to design new causal methods to localize the FEF in healthy humans. In such procedures, a TMS coil is moved around the approximate location of the FEF. Pulses are delivered with intensity generally at or slightly above the resting motor threshold (RMT), i.e., the intensity at which they induce overt evoked hand muscle activations in half of the trials when stimulating the primary motor cortex. Like in microstimulation studies, the choice of intensity is somehow arbitrary. Indeed, it is likely that simulating at 100 or 120% of RMT will lead to different results. More importantly, stimulating at an intensity based on the RMT does not warrant consistent results across participants as it is known that, except under certain circumstances (Deblieck et al., 2008) the TMS-measured excitability of one area is poorly predicting the TMS-measured excitability of another area (Stewart et al., 2001; Boroojerdi et al., 2002; Antal et al., 2004; Kahkonen et al., 2005). Notwithstanding this limitation, TMS procedures allow identifying the FEF as the area in which stimulation significantly modifies some saccadic outcome parameters, generally the latency of a specific type of saccade.

Using such methods, the greatest delays in saccade latencies have been obtained when targeting an area on or 2 cm anterior to the inter-aural line, approximately 6 cm lateral to the vertex, situated between areas over which TMS could generate motor-evoked potentials in hand's and face's muscles (Thickbroom et al., 1996). The authors of these reports did not exclude that the FEF could also extend more rostrally, and that such projections cannot be easily assessed either because rostral stimulation would cause blinks, or because the anterior portions of FEF are involved in other aspects of saccade programming. Other studies localized the FEF within areas situated 2 cm (Ro et al., 1999) or 1.5 cm (Ro et al., 2002) rostral to the motor hand area. However, such site, probably belonging to the middle-frontal gyrus and close to the pre-central sulcus, could not be localized in every tested participant. Moreover, this localization suffers from important interindividual differences, mostly within the coronal or dorsal to medial plane, consistent with reports from neuroimaging studies (Paus, 1996). Studies by O'Shea et al. (2004) and Silvanto et al. (2006) targeting the FEF based on anatomical landmarks within the middle frontal gyrus, just rostral from the junction of the pre-central and the superior frontal sulci, reported that such area corresponds to about 3–4 cm rostral to the individual motor hand area representation. In spite of its rostral location, the reported mean Talairach coordinates locate very close to the coordinates reported by Paus (1996) in their meta-analysis (Table 2). Further work to causally define the FEF location in individual participants by means of TMS employing individualized MRI guidance and studies directly comparing TMS and fMRI FEF localizers within the same population of subjects remain to be performed.

Consequences for Studying Motor, Visual or Cognitive Properties of the FEF

Table 1 summarizes the findings for the above-cited literature concerning the search for FEF localization. Variability across species, methods, hemispheres, and individuals in the number of foci associated with FEF and their exact localization raises concerns about how we can explore its role in eye movements or cognitive function.

In TMS studies exploring the causal contributions of FEF in eye movements or cognitive processes such as attentional orienting, consciousness or decision making (see Section on the Role of the FEF), the gold standard would be to use a similar mapping methodology to identify the exact location of this region prior to its manipulation. Based on this notion, for instance, Olk et al. (2006) took the time to identify an area around its a priori anatomical location on which TMS induced longer latencies for contralateral than ipsilateral saccades. However, in order to limit the duration of the experiments, other studies employed relative coordinates leading to the average location, expressed as the distance in cm from the motor hand area (which can be easily identified with TMS) and successfully reported significant effects on quantitative measures of eye movements (Wipfli et al., 2001; Nyffeler et al., 2006a,b; Nagel et al., 2008; van Donkelaar et al., 2009). Similarly, in another study, the FEF was localized by probing a series of frontal cortical sites rostral to the motor hand area until evoked hand motor responses disappeared (Leff et al., 2001). Nevertheless, one of the most commonly used strategies consisted in targeting those locations identified in anatomical MRIs by means of on sulci/gyri configurations (O'Shea et al., 2004), or on the basis of normalized coordinates from neuroimaging studies or meta-analyses (Grosbras and Paus, 2002), or employing individual functional localizers based on fMRI acquisitions performed during eye movements tasks (Gagnon et al., 2006).

In conclusion, potential conflicting results across studies concerning the function of the FEF might be related, among other factors, to variability in the way it is localized. This observation has to be kept in mind when interpreting the results that will be presented in the following part.

Role of FEF

This section will review the role of the FEF in eye movements and in visuo-spatial attention, visual awareness, and perceptual modulation.

Role of FEF in Oculomotor Tasks

In humans, knowledge on the role of FEF in several types of eye movements (summarized in Table 3) has been mainly derived from clinical cases in which the FEF has been damaged or from applying TMS on the FEF of healthy persons. These studies are reported in Tables 4, 5 and the conclusions derived from them are reported below.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Types of eye movements and experimental paradigms to elicit them.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Effects of FEF lesions on eye movements.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Effects of TMS over the FEF on eye movements.

Lesions studies

Most of lesion studies describing the role of the FEF have been focusing on oculomotor deficits that are reported in Table 4. The general pictures emerging from this literature is that FEF lesions very mildly affect the most reflexive saccades but might delay eye movements for which a voluntary component is introduced, for instance concerning fixation disengagement. Thus, although the triggering of reflexive saccades is more likely under the control of the PPC (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 2004; Muri and Nyffeler, 2008), the FEF could still play a role, revealed under specific cognitive conditions. In that vein, the FEF has been hypothesized to play a context-dependent modulatory influence over different cortical and subcortical structures involved in different categories of reflexive saccades. Such role could be revealed by switching cost or benefit when alternating between gap and overlap pro-saccades (Vernet et al., 2009). The role of FEF in reflexive saccade inhibition remains controversial, the DLPFC being a more likely candidate to control such inhibition (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 2004; Muri and Nyffeler, 2008). Finally, the FEF (together with the DLPFC and other subcortical structures) is more commonly thought as a controller for voluntary saccades such as predictive, memory-guided and anti-saccades (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 2004; Muri and Nyffeler, 2008). In addition, the FEF is involved in the computation of the amplitude of all types of eye movements.

Despite their undeniable value, several aspects limit the strength of the conclusions that can be drawn from lesion studies. First, lesions are rarely limited to the FEF, making it difficult to isolate the specific involvement of the FEF in the observed deficits. Second, different deficits might be observed during the acute and chronic phase following the lesions. Transient hypo-perfusion of areas connected to the damaged area, a phenomenon known as diaschisis or, on the contrary, complex plastic reorganization within the impaired network, render the role of the FEF difficult to isolate from the role of the entire network. Other cortical and subcortical areas, or the contralesional FEF, seem to play an important role in developing compensatory mechanisms (for a review see Muri and Nyffeler, 2008). In monkey studies, in which more spatially precise transient inactivation or lesions can be performed, acutely observed deficits (Sommer and Tehovnik, 1997; Dias and Segraves, 1999) rapidly disappeared, except for complex tasks such as memory-guided saccades or saccades toward flashed targets, or if lesions to the FEF were combined with lesions to other areas (for a review see Tehovnik et al., 2000; Muri and Nyffeler, 2008).

TMS studies

The most commonly reported effect of TMS over the FEF during a saccadic task is a modulation of its preparation latency. Because of the alerting effect linked to the clicking sound and taping sensation associated with the coil discharge, it is known that TMS can have unspecific (i.e., not related to the effects of the electrical currents induced on brain tissue) effects on reaction times and eye movement latencies. Thus, shorter latencies could be related to crossmodal facilitation, whereas longer latencies could result from the participants waiting for TMS discharge as for a “go” signal. Thus, it is important to ensure that the effects on latencies are either stronger or in the reverse direction than the effects obtained in a control condition, such as sham stimulation or the active stimulation of a control brain area unrelated to saccadic control or execution. Using such cautionary measures, TMS over FEF has been shown to modulate the latency of different types of saccades.

TMS studies exploring the role of FEF in eye movements are reported in Table 5. As with patients' studies, whether TMS over FEF can delay reflexive saccades toward suddenly appearing visual targets remains unclear and most of the effects on latency modulations have been shown on pro-saccades involving some degree of voluntary or intentional component. In anti-saccade modulations, whether TMS stimulation of the FEF disturbs the suppression of the reflexive pro-saccade or the preparation of the voluntary anti-saccade or both is not entirely clear. In general, TMS is believed to interfere with several stages in the execution of saccades, including the perceptual analysis of the cues or targets and the motor preparation (burst signal). Occasional facilitatory effects on saccade latency have been attributed to suppression of fixation activity (within the FEF or the FEF projections to the SC). While most reports demonstrated, in accordance with microstimulation studies, effects on contralateral saccades, some studies demonstrate ipsilateral or bilateral effects that could be related to a modulation of fixation cells activity or to transcallosal modulation of both FEFs. Interestingly, TMS can modulate the latency of several eye movements performed in 3D space. Finally, the FEF is not only involved in fixation, fixation release and the triggering of voluntary eye movements but also contributes to the computation of eye movements dynamics (gain, velocity).

In conclusion, non-invasive neurostimulation studies employing TMS largely confirmed, in healthy humans and with higher spatial and temporal resolution, the insights drawn from patient studies. The unquestioned role of the FEF in the triggering of voluntary eye movements as well as the still controversial role of this region in reflexive movement inhibition and initiation is reminiscent of the blurred frontiers between reflexive and voluntary movements and of the importance of entire oculomotor networks for the control of eye movements, in which the relative contribution of each node is modulated by the cognitive context. The rest of this Section on the Role of FEF will explore how the FEF is involved in a very diverse set of higher cognitive functions (see also Table 6 reporting TMS studies on these topics).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Effects of TMS over the FEF on visuo-spatial attention, visual awareness and perceptual modulation.

Visual Activity and Saliency Map within the FEF

The FEF encodes visual signals and is believed to participate in the visuo-motor transformation for the preparation of eye movements, as suggested by the influence of FEF on the accuracy of eye movements (see effects of lesions and TMS on other eye movement's parameters in Tables 4, 5). Beyond this contribution, the FEF can be considered as a visual area in itself, with early visual-evoked responses reported in anesthetized animals, peaking even before activity reaches V2 or V4 (Schmolesky et al., 1998). Moreover, the projections from the FEF to V4 could be characterized as feed-forward connections, i.e., going from lower to higher hierarchical levels (Barone et al., 2000). Wurtz and Mohler (1976) reported that some of the visual cells within the FEF displayed an enhanced response to a visual stimulus when a saccade was made toward the receptive field rather than away from it. Such selective enhancement would demonstrate the ability of the FEF (and similarly, also that of the SC) to evaluate stimulus significance and use such information for saccade preparation. Although there is a clear relationship between visual and movement properties of the FEF in terms of spatial selection, there is also some degree of dissociation. Bruce and Goldberg (1985) described in the FEF a continuum of visuo-motor cells, from purely visual to purely motor cells, the latter cells being less sharply tuned to direction and amplitude than the former cells, and with visuo-motor cells showing intermediate tuning. In humans, Blanke et al. (1999), recording with intracranial electrodes visual-evoked potentials in epileptic patients, showed strong visual responses for contralateral visual stimuli (consistent with the direction of the electrically-elicited eye movements) but also responses of lower amplitude after ipsilateral visual stimulation.

The visual activity encoded within the FEF has been primarily related to the computing of a saliency map, where neural activity codes for the location of a behaviorally relevant target displayed among distractors during a typical visual search task (for a review see Schall and Bichot, 1998; Thompson and Bichot, 2005). There would be a gradual suppression of distractor-related activity paralleled by an enhancement of target-related activity. Saccades are generally performed toward the “winner” of this saliency map. However, similar computations are performed even when no saccades are required or when a saccade should be performed away from the ultimately selected “winner” target. Indeed, in a go/no go visual search task, although visual response within the FEF is enhanced when the saccade is executed (go trials), discrimination of the target occurs within similar timing in both go and no go trials (Thompson et al., 1997). There would be an early (around 50 ms) non-discriminative visual response within the FEF followed by a later (around 100–150 ms) discriminative selection of the target among distractor regardless of its visual features (Thompson et al., 1996; Thompson and Bichot, 2005), but even the early response can show discriminative properties in experienced animals (Bichot et al., 1996). When saccades are triggered toward the target, the variability in saccade latencies is poorly correlated with the speed of discrimination of the FEF cells and seems to be rather related to distinct motor preparation stages (Thompson et al., 1996).

Walker et al. (2009) brought direct causal evidence in humans that the FEF might be participating in the elaboration of a saliency map for the selection of a target of an upcoming saccade. Indeed, when a competing visual distractor appeared in the same direction as the saccade goal but at unpredictable locations, saccade trajectories deviated away from the distractor. The magnitude of this distractor-related deviation of saccade trajectory was increased by single-pulse TMS over the right FEF. The interpretation is that stimulation of the FEF might have disrupted the process of enhancing target salience or could have increased the inhibition associated with the distractor.

The FEF at the Heart of the Coupling Between Attention and Eye Movements?

The saliency map described above could reflect the deployment of visuo-spatial attention. The premotor theory of attention postulates that orientation of spatial attention derives from the same mechanisms dedicated to action: attention is oriented to a given point in space when the oculomotor program for moving the eyes to that point is ready to be executed (Rizzolatti et al., 1987). In this perspective, FEF “visual” activation could be attributed to the preparation of saccade programs, which may or may not be overtly executed, rather than to the visual analytic processes in the FEF.

Many behavioral and neurophysiological studies support this theory, according to which covert attention shifts without eye movements, conceived as a specific and distinct process with a mechanism of its own, might simply be an artificial separation of otherwise unified underlying processes. Among the many behavioral pieces of evidences in accordance with the premotor theory of attention, one could cite the enhanced visual discrimination performance when a discrimination stimulus and a saccade target converge to the same object whereas it declines steeply when they refer to items at different locations, arguing against the ability to direct visual attention to one location while simultaneously preparing a saccade towards another location (Deubel and Schneider, 1996). Neuroimaging studies often find similar activations, including in the FEF, for eye movements and attentional shifts, and a remarkable level of overlap of the underlying circuits of these operations, as summarized in a meta-analysis on PET and fMRI studies (Grosbras et al., 2005). Interestingly, the involvement of the FEF in contralateral attention shifts would be particularly marked when participants have to overtly respond to a target, for instance with a manual response (Corbetta et al., 1993) or when the attentional task is particularly demanding (Donner et al., 2000).

However, there is also evidence against a strict interpretation of the premotor theory of attention. For instance, TMS during saccade preparation was able to modulate discrimination performance at the target location: while TMS over the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) ipsilateral to the saccade's direction increased general performance, non-invasive stimulation over the FEF contralateral to the saccade's direction specifically decreased or enhanced discrimination on the target location depending on the exact stimulation parameters (Neggers et al., 2007; Van Ettinger-Veenstra et al., 2009). Thus, the FEF plays a role in mediating the coupling between visuo-spatial attention and eye movements and such coupling can be modulated by TMS (Neggers et al., 2007; Van Ettinger-Veenstra et al., 2009). Other arguments against a motor preparation toward the target location to which attention is oriented can be found in microstimulation experiments with monkeys (Juan et al., 2004) or TMS experiments in humans (Juan et al., 2008). In the first study (Juan et al., 2004), monkeys had to perform a visual search and a saccade toward (pro-saccade) or away from (anti-saccade) a visual target depending on its orientation. Microstimulation of the FEF at variable timings after target onset evoked, in anti-saccade trials, saccades progressively toward the endpoint of the correct saccades but never toward the visual target. Using a similar task in humans, Juan et al. (2008) showed that double-pulse TMS over the right FEF can delay saccade latencies in two distinct time-windows: an early window (40–80 ms after target onset) in which the delay in pro-saccades was interpreted as a disruption of the visual stimulus processing and also a late window (200–160 ms before the expected saccades) in which a delay in pro- and anti-saccades was interpreted as a disruption of saccade preparation.

Context-Dependent Role of FEF During Visual Search

Whether or not eye movement preparation is strictly linked to attention orientation does not question the involvement of the FEF in visual discrimination performance, either directly or indirectly though its massive set of anatomical projections toward the visual cortex. Several TMS studies in humans have been designed to accurately describe the role of the left and right FEF in covert voluntary attentional orienting and visual discrimination performance. For instance, Muggleton et al. (2003) showed that rTMS at 10 Hz for 500 ms over the right FEF during the presentation of a search array disrupted visual search. These authors showed that a decrease of the visual sensitivity explained by a higher number of false positives (i.e., incorrect detections reported by participants when the target was absent) and attributed to a reduced ability to process the items. Interestingly, only specific subtypes of visual search impaired by the stimulation, such as conjunction search was impaired (i.e., when the target shares the same color than about half of the distractors and the same orientation than the remaining distractors) and, to a lesser extent, interleaved feature search (i.e., when the color of target and distractors is randomly attributed at each trial). On the contrary, rTMS had no effect on constant feature search (i.e., when the target and distractors always look the same across trials). The authors concluded that the right FEF is particularly important for visual search when the visual target is neither salient nor predictable. Using double-pulse TMS paradigms, such findings were confirmed for an early time window of up to 80 ms after search array onset, i.e., much earlier than the involvement of the PPC in visual search (O'Shea et al., 2004; Kalla et al., 2008).

Using similar visual search paradigms, the role of FEF in visual priming (form of implicit memory that facilitates the detection of a target that shares common features with a recently inspected search target) or, on the contrary in switch detection has also been addressed in TMS approaches. Indeed, fMRI experiments reported a suppression of BOLD response in fronto-parietal networks, including the FEF, during simultaneous color and location repetition (Kristjansson et al., 2007). Non-invasive brain stimulation studies showed that 10 Hz rTMS patterns for 500 ms over the left (but not the right) FEF disrupted spatial priming, as measured by increased reaction times, when applied during the presentation of the search array (O'Shea et al., 2007) or during the inter-trial interval (Campana et al., 2007). This result suggested that the memory trace is probably distributed through visual and oculomotor networks typically required for those behaviors and that the FEF would be an area of convergence and integration during the preparation of an overt response (O'Shea et al., 2007). Finally, the left FEF would also be involved in the ability to detect a color switch (or select a new target) as identical rTMS patterns delivered to the left FEF applied in-between trials increased switching costs by slowing down the response time for switch trials (Muggleton et al., 2010).

However, the right/left hemisphere frontal asymmetries described above are questioned by other studies showing that rTMS at 10 Hz for 500 ms, over the right but not the left FEF, from the beginning of an array onset, disrupts spatial priming and that similar rTMS over both right and left FEF increases reaction time when the target position is random (Lane et al., 2012). Interestingly, the same team also demonstrated that such involvement of the right FEF is independent of the depth (near vs. far space) at which the task is performed, whereas the right PPC would be involved in near space and right ventral occipital cortex in far space (Lane et al., 2013).

Finally, it is possible that the FEF is more directly involved in spatial memory, in particular in trans-saccadic memory. Indeed, Prime et al. (2010) showed a decrease of the number of items participants could remember when left or right FEF were disrupted around saccadic time. Such effect could however be related to the stimulation of a spatial working memory area that has been identified just rostral to the FEF (Courtney et al., 1998).

Top-Down Modulation of Visual Areas

Several studies suggested that the contributions of the FEF to discrimination performance are mediated by its output projections to the visual cortex. Indeed, electrophysiological evidence in both animals and humans demonstrated a relation between activity within the FEF and excitability of occipital brain areas. Moore and Armstrong (2003) showed that the intracortical stimulation of the FEF in monkeys at current intensities below those required to evoke saccades (i.e. subthreshold stimulation), enhanced visual responses in visual area V4. Such enhancements were retinotopically specific. If the endpoint of the saccade evoked by suprathreshold stimulation of the FEF overlapped with the receptive field of a V4 cell, subthreshold FEF stimulation enhanced this V4 cell's visual responses. This type of top-down modulation of visual cortex excitability could explain earlier findings in non-human primates consisting in enhanced perception (decreased threshold for detecting a luminance change) of peripheral visual stimulus after subthreshold FEF stimulation, only when the visual stimulus was displayed within the “motor field” of the stimulated FEF (Moore and Fallah, 2001).

Although TMS cannot reach the spatial resolution required to target neural populations within the FEF subtending specific visual or motor fields, several studies in healthy humans, combining TMS with EEG (Taylor et al., 2007), TMS with fMRI (Ruff et al., 2006) or employing double coil TMS and psychophysics, showed similar top-down influence of the FEF on visual areas and visual performance. Short 5-pulse trains of 10 Hz rTMS applied over the right FEF during a cueing period of a covert orienting task modulated attention-related ongoing EEG posterior potentials before visual stimulation, as well as the potentials evoked by the visual stimulus (Taylor et al., 2007). Similar short 5-pulse trains of 9 Hz TMS over the right FEF modulated the BOLD activity recorded with fMRI within visual areas V1-V4 led to activity increases for retinotopic representations of the peripheral visual fields combined with activity decreases of central retinotopic locations (Ruff et al., 2006). A follow up experiment showed that TMS over the right FEF enhanced perceived contrast for peripheral relative to central visual stimuli (Ruff et al., 2006), hence proving that such activity modulation was behaviorally relevant. Finally, the double-coil TMS technique can be used to simultaneously induce activity within the left or right FEF and measure the excitability of MT/V5. Stimulation of the FEF 20–40 ms prior to stimulation of MT/V5 decreased the intensity of MT/V5 stimulation required to elicit phosphenes, demonstrating that the FEF has a direct modulatory effect on the excitability of this motion visual area (Silvanto et al., 2006).

Modulation of Visual Performance and Awareness

In line with animal and human studies showing respectively, enhanced perception and increased activity in visual areas following FEF stimulation, several reports have also shown that TMS over the left and right FEF was able to speed-up discrimination and/or increase detection and visual awareness (Grosbras and Paus, 2002, 2003; Chanes et al., 2012). Grosbras and Paus (2002) reported that a TMS pulse delivered to the left or right FEF 53 ms prior to target onset could decrease reaction time in a forced-choice discrimination task. These same authors showed that a TMS pulse delivered to the left or right FEF 40 ms before the onset of a masked target could also increase sensitivity in a visual detection task (Grosbras and Paus, 2003). Similarly, Chanes et al. (2012) provided evidence showing that a TMS pulse delivered to the right FEF 80 ms before a low-contrast target could increase visual perceptual sensitivity in a detection task. These studies have shown that right FEF stimulation generally leads to bilateral effects whereas left FEF stimulation results in an increase of performance solely for stimuli presented in the contralateral visual hemifield (Grosbras and Paus, 2002, 2003; Chanes et al., 2012). In addition, interactions between TMS effects and the manipulation of visuo-spatial attentional orienting in space before target presentation have been found in studies that combined a strategy to modulate attentional processes by means of spatially informative visual cues and by means of non-invasive neurostimulation delivered to specific cortical regions. More specifically, the increase of performance after right FEF stimulation reported in the above-mentioned studies occurred specifically for validly cued (or attended) locations, and also following spatially neutral cues, but not for unattended locations following invalid cueing (Grosbras and Paus, 2002; Chanes et al., 2012).

Enhancement of perception may result from a global increase of background activity, drifting closer to a perceptual threshold, hence allowing any incoming weak signal to reach it more easily; in addition to this global injection of activity, TMS outcomes are also highly dependent on the state of the targeted regions and their mixed populations of neurons. Accordingly, TMS may selectively boost specific clusters of neurons according to their level of activity (O'Shea and Walsh, 2004). In this context, prior reports have suggested that the visual performance and awareness enhancement occurred directly by changing activity in FEF (manipulating genuine processes purported by this frontal region) or indirectly via connections between the FEF and visual regions modulating the input gain of incoming visual signals (Grosbras and Paus, 2003). Interestingly, interindividual differences in the direction and magnitude of the TMS driven facilitatory effects reported in Chanes et al. (2012) correlated significantly with the probability of anatomical connection between the FEF and the SC estimated by means of white matter probabilistic tractography. Such result suggests a key role for white matter connectivity between the stimulated area and other key brain structures to explain at the network level the strength of TMS modulatory influences on visual performance (Quentin et al., 2013).

It was also suggested that TMS effects could actually enhance perception in a way similar to what occurs naturally during cover shifts of attention (Grosbras and Paus, 2003). Following that line of thought, Chanes et al. (2013) conceived an experiment in which TMS was used as a way to emulate activity that would mimic neurophysiological spatio-temporal patterns signaling processes of attentional orienting. This study was based on a prior report by Buschman and Miller (2007) demonstrating, in non-human primates, high-beta (~30 Hz) and gamma (~50 Hz) fronto-parietal synchronizations subtending top-down and bottom-up attentional processes, respectively. By using short trains of stimulation at those same frequencies, Chanes et al. (2013) observed an increase of perceptual sensitivity during a low-contrast target detection task following right-FEF stimulation at 30 Hz, which would be consistent with an increase of endogenous attention and/or facilitated access to visual consciousness. Interestingly, the strength of the individual TMS improvements correlated significantly with the volume of the first branch of the superior longitudinal fasciculus, which links the stimulated FEF with areas of the posterior parietal cortex within and in the vicinity of the IPS, supporting the idea of a frequency-specific fronto-parietal synchronization induced by rhythmic TMS subtending visual performance ameliorations (Quentin et al., 2014). Moreover, stimulation of the right FEF at 50 Hz induced a relaxation of response criterion, as if sensory evidence in favor of target presence were increased whether or not the target was actually present (Chanes et al., 2013). This result provides support for a frequency-based multiplexing of two distinct processes, such as visual sensitivity and response criterion with bearing on visual performance, emerging from neuronal resources within the same area. Whereas short trains of rTMS have been widely used to drive stronger behavioral effects (see e.g., Smith et al., 2005), they can also be applied, as in Chanes et al. (2013), as a novel way to manipulate rhythmic brain activity, in line with evidence of oscillatory entrainment with such technique (Thut et al., 2011).

To reconcile these studies showing improvements in visual perception and awareness with TMS of the FEF with previously cited reports showing impairment of visual discrimination during visual search tasks (e.g., Muggleton et al., 2003; O'Shea et al., 2004; Kalla et al., 2008), it should be reminded that TMS lacks the spatial resolution to selectively enhance perception in one particular area of the perceptual space. Thus in visual search paradigms, distractors might benefit as much as targets from TMS-driven visual enhancement, decreasing the relative benefit for the latter and leading to perceptual impairments instead of enhancements.

Along the same lines, in addition to increased visual performance at cued locations, TMS over the FEF should disrupt the inhibition of stimulus processing at unattended locations. This hypothesis has been confirmed by the study by Smith et al. (2005), that in agreement with this notion, showed that in a visual detection task, short trains of rTMS at 20 Hz for 200 ms starting 50 ms before cue onset (and not around the timing of target onset as in the previously cited studies) over the left FEF were able to decrease the reaction time cost of invalid cueing before contralateral targets. Such disruption of the inhibition for unattended locations could also explain the results reported by Ro et al. (2003) who showed that single TMS pulses, delivered over the right FEF, showing that single 600 ms after the cue and 150 ms prior to target onset, decreased the inhibition of return phenomenon. This well-known attentional process consists in a worsening of visual performance at locations that had been cued a certain interval of time preceding target onset. It is probably caused by a disengagement of attention and is thought to prevent the re-exploration of an already scrutinized region of the space. The above-mentioned studies support the notion that increased visual detection performance at unattended spatial locations could result from TMS interfering with active mechanisms of inhibition and exploration suppression of unattended spatial locations subtended by the FEF.

To summarize, the FEF cannot be only conceived as an area important for preparing and triggering eye movements but also as an essential region contributing to cognitive processes such as attentional orienting, visual awareness, conscious access, perceptual performance, and decision making. However, as mentioned before, these processes are probably mediated by activity within largely distributed cortico-cortical and cortico-subcortical networks. In particular, fronto-parietal systems are particularly relevant, and effects similar to those of the FEF reviewed in this paper, have been found in specific PPC regions such as the IPS (Chica et al., 2011; Bourgeois et al., 2013a,b). If there is full agreement on the fact that dorsal frontal and posterior parietal areas operate commonly and in synchrony in attentional and visual performance modulation processes, other studies emphasize the differences between the contributions of these two regions. For instance, whereas the dominance of the right PPC in attentional orienting tasks is well known, inter-hemispheric asymmetry is less evident with regards to the contributions of the FEF (Gitelman et al., 1999), and such aspect might prove highly task dependent. Moreover, the FEF could be more involved than the IPS in intentional behavior when overt responses are required (Corbetta et al., 1993). Future studies will be necessary to further understand the common and distinctive role of these two highly interconnected areas.

Finally, it should be mentioned that although the FEF is involved in both eye movements and visual cognition, few studies have explicitly explored simultaneously the combined role of the FEF in both types of function (with the exception of the studies reviewed in a prior section addressing the role of the FEF in the coupling between attention and eye movements). The cognitive context modulates the role of the FEF in eye movements but direct report of conscious perception, for instance, is not performed in eye movements studies. Conversely, studies on cognition rarely explore eye movements (even if correct visual fixation is often assessed with eye-trackers). In future studies, exploration of microsaccades and other fixation eye movements (Martinez-Conde et al., 2013) might shed new light on the relevance of the experimental dissociation between eye movements and cognition.

Improving Visual Perception and Awareness

In the last part of this review, we will briefly address some links between FEF activity, eye movements and visuo-spatial awareness. Such evidence will allow us to elaborate on the rational behind the potential use of non-invasive brain stimulation and eye movement training in 3D space for the treatment and rehabilitation of visuo-spatial disorders. As an example, we will focus on the case of hemi-spatial visual neglect, often suffered by patients after right hemisphere damage.

Deficits of Visuo-Spatial Awareness: the Case of Hemispatial Neglect

As reviewed above, the FEF is not only a key node contributing to the planning and execution of eye movements but it is also involved in attentional orienting and several aspects of visual cognition. Surprisingly however, a large majority of the FEF lesion studies in non-human and human primates focused on the consequences of frontal damage on oculomotor deficits, neglecting the exploration of other behavioral consequences. Nevertheless, it has been shown that lesions damaging right attentional networks can often induce attentional orienting and visual awareness disorders such as visuo-spatial neglect. This condition is a highly impairing syndrome consisting in an inability to orient attention to regions of the contralesional space and thus become aware of sensory stimuli presented herein. It is common after stroke lesions impacting cortical or subcortical regions, particularly in the right hemisphere. In a multicenter study developed in a cohort of 1281 acute stroke patients, signs of visuo-spatial neglect occurred in 43% of right brain-injured patients and also in 20% of left brain-damaged patients. Apparent spontaneous recovery of these deficits seems often to occur, but does not necessarily eliminate all signs and deficits, particularly visual extinction, a fact that becomes evident when more challenging and robust-to-learning computer-based tasks are employed instead of paper and pencil tasks to evaluate patient status. Indeed, at 3 months, signs of moderate neglect are still present in 17% of right brain-injured patients and 5% of left brain-injured patients (Ringman et al., 2004). Neglect interferes with the rehabilitation of deficits in other domains, such as motor and sensory, that can also be present in such patients and, if it endures, it can lead to poor clinical recovery outcomes and preclude a reintegration to normal or adapted life. However, despite considerable therapeutic advances in behavioral, sensorial and pharmacological treatments, many patients remain enduringly impaired after rehabilitation (Fierro et al., 2006).

One of the most influential hypotheses to understand visuo-spatial neglect suggests the existence of an impairment in the balance between the orienting attentional bias of each hemisphere towards the contralateral hemispace (Kinsbourne, 1970). This explanation has received support from animal studies (Sprague, 1966; Rizzolatti et al., 1983). In humans, a revealing single patient case revealed how a second stroke involving the left FEF a few days after a right parietal stroke was able to fully compensate the clinical signs of severe neglect induced by the first lesion (Vuilleumier et al., 1996).

Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation to Improve Visuo-Spatial Awareness

In the context of a hemispheric imbalance, the use of non-invasive brain stimulation, namely rTMS and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has proven particularly promising. Indeed, both techniques have demonstrated efficacy in modulating transiently brain regional excitability (Nitsche et al., 2008; Rossi et al., 2009). Inhibitory stimulation over the intact hemisphere (mostly at the level of parietal areas) has shown promise in decreasing the excessive inhibition over viable regions of the damaged hemisphere and by virtue of this effect relieving neglect symptoms (e.g., Oliveri et al., 2001; Sparing et al., 2009; Cazzoli et al., 2012; Koch et al., 2012).

However, the ability of FEF-TMS to increase visual perception and awareness, at least in healthy subjects on a trial-by-trial basis (Grosbras and Paus, 2002, 2003; Chanes et al., 2012) could revive the interest in targeting this node of the attentional network for rehabilitation purposes. Indeed, neglect is a “network” impairment: there is a striking variability of areas whose lesions results in neglect. Temporal, parietal or frontal cortical lesions or subcortical lesions, or combined lesions of these areas are the most likely to induce enduring neglect 3 months post-stroke (Ringman et al., 2004). Aside from the impairments resulting from the lesions themselves, neglect symptoms could also arise from diaschisis (i.e., abolition of neural activity in areas that are distant but anatomically connected to the lesioned area) and disconnections/hypoperfusion affecting the whole fronto-parietal network. Recent evidence from an fMRI study suggests that functional connectivity in damage and also intact fronto-parietal attentional orienting networks is impaired in neglect patients (He et al., 2007) and the disconnection theory should be strongly considered to explain the pathophysiology of neglect (Doricchi et al., 2008). A recent explanatory model of visuospatial neglect (Corbetta and Shulman, 2011) suggests that neglect primarily arises from damage to the right hemisphere-dominant non-spatial ventral attention network. This ventral network would be interacting with the dorsal fronto-parietal network (encompassing the FEF and the IPS) that controls spatial attention. Thus, structural damage to the right ventral network would result in functional resting and task-related activity asymmetries in the dorsal network, leading to the typical lateralized attention deficit of neglect syndrome.

The existence of fronto-parietal synchronization at specific frequencies associated with attentional processes (Buschman and Miller, 2007) and the possibility to inject such rhythms with TMS over the FEF to increase visual awareness (Chanes et al., 2013) also open new perspectives for treatment. In addition, MEG experiments in 5 right stroke neglect patients revealed that target omissions correlated with a build-up of low beta activity in left frontal locations before target presentation (Rastelli et al., 2013). Thus, neglect occurrence seems to arise from abnormal oscillatory activity and consequently could be manipulated with rhythmic TMS. Whereas traditional rTMS protocols have shown complex effects on brain oscillations (Thut and Pascual-Leone, 2010; Vernet et al., 2013), the use of rhythmic TMS or Transcranial Alternate Current Stimulation (tACS) combined with EEG demonstrated a possibility to entrain physiologically relevant oscillations at a chosen frequency (Thut et al., 2011; Helfrich et al., 2014). Similarly, the use of double-coil (bifocal) stimulation is a promising tool to modulate synchronization between distant brain areas (Plewnia et al., 2008). Future studies are needed to explore the possibility to induce such modulations in clinical populations lasting long enough to be clinically relevant.

Performing 3D Eye Movements to Improve Visuo-Spatial Awareness

Another topic of interest for the treatment of visual awareness disorders would consist in further exploring the link between eye movements and conscious perception. Indeed, eye movements training can be natural way to activate the FEF and other areas of attentional networks. The resulting plasticity might in turn improve visual awareness. Vergence is particularly fragile and subject to aging, fatigue and neurological insults (Scheiman et al., 2005a,b,c; Yang et al., 2010) and hence reeducation of eye movements in depth might promote better visual navigation in space and as a consequence improve conscious perception.

The link between exploratory movements performed in 3D space and the gathering of visuo-spatial information goes beyond a simple sharing of brain resources. Indeed, efferent copies of vergence movements, proprioception on convergence state (Priot et al., 2012), or simply disparity indices on which vergence movement can be programmed (Ziegler and Hess, 1997) are all candidates to be involved in our ability to assess depth. Conversely, our understanding of the 3D space can trigger movements in accordance with our perception. However, there are striking examples of dissociations between our perception and action as for instance the famous Müller-Lyer illusion, in which erroneous judgments about an object size and correct manual seizing movement may coexist. This type of experience led to the traditional dissociation between vision for perception and vision for action (Milner and Goodale, 2008). However, such dissociation is controversial and might be an experimental artifact. Indeed, without any visual feedback, hand and eye movements (and consequently the “vision for action”) can also be affected by the illusion (Bruno and Franz, 2009; Bruno et al., 2010). Similarly, in illusions where the perceived depth is different from the actual depth, the vergence movements are sometimes subject to the depth cues of the physical world (Wismeijer et al., 2008), and sometimes to the illusory percept (Sheliga and Miles, 2003). An interesting interpretation is that there are two types of convergence: a fast one to serve action and a slower one, which would allow the construction of a conscious percept (Wagner et al., 2009).

Besides the deficit of awareness in contralesional space, neglect patients seem to also suffer from problems that are specific to the depth at which tasks are performed: neglect symptoms might be sometimes more severe in the near space or in the far space (see Aimola et al., 2012 for a review). Probably, the evaluation paradigms and tasks, the effector used, and the location and extent of the right hemisphere damage could explain the differences found by different studies with regards to this issue (see Aimola et al., 2012 for a discussion). Experimental evidence supports the notion that the representations of peripersonal and extrapersonal spaces are subtended respectively, by rather dorsal or ventral regions within fronto-parietal systems (Aimola et al., 2012). Hence, performance dissociation between tasks performed in near and far space could reveal the specificity of the disconnection patterns between areas required for the task and areas devoted to monitor specific portions of the space (Weiss et al., 2000). In any case, it is possible that neglect patients show deficits in visual navigation in depth, which could aggravate their difficulties in exploring fronto-parallel space for certain depths. Oculomotor training focusing on vergence movements in depth (Jainta et al., 2011) could be another interesting path to explore for a more effective rehabilitation of awareness disorders.

Conclusion

In this review, we focused on the role and localization of the human FEF, a cortical area which is part of highly distributed saccadic and visuo-spatial networks with important bearing on the control of eye movements in 3D space and contributing importantly to several aspects of attentional and visual cognition. A particular emphasis was placed on TMS studies, which have allowed a successful causal exploration of the contributions of this frontal region. We provided evidence that the results of such studies with regards to their ability to map FEF cortical location are not necessarily similar to those of other mapping techniques employed in neuroscience, such as lesion studies, microstimulation, intracranial recordings, PET, fMRI, and EEG/MEG in both humans and non-human primates. Similarities and discrepancies across the results provided by different techniques, the use of different paradigms and/or experimental models were presented and discussed. Finally, we speculated on the posibility to manipulate FEF activity with non-invasive neurostimulation and oculomotor training in order to improve visuo-spatial awareness in 3D space in healthy population and to promote functional recovery in stroke patients.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Marine Vernet and Romain Quentin were supported by the “Fondation pour la Recherche Médicale.” The activities of the laboratory of Dr. Valero-Cabré are supported by grants from eraNET NEURON Beyondvis by ANR, FP6, and IHU-A-Translationnel.

Abbreviations

CEF, cingulate eye field; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; EEG, electroencephalography; FEF, frontal eye field; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; IPS, intraparietal sulcus; LIP, lateral intraparietal area; MEG, magneto-encephalography; MST, medial superior temporal; PEF, parietal eye field; PET, positron emission tomography; PPC, posterior parietal cortex; rTMS, repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation; SC, superior colliculus; SEF, supplementary eye field; tACS, transcranial alternate current stimulation; tDCS, transcranial direct current stimulation; TMS, transcranial magnetic stimulation.

References

Aimola, L., Schindler, I., Simone, A. M., and Venneri, A. (2012). Near and far space neglect: task sensitivity and anatomical substrates. Neuropsychologia 50, 1115–1123. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.01.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amiez, C., Kostopoulos, P., Champod, A. S., and Petrides, M. (2006). Local morphology predicts functional organization of the dorsal premotor region in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 26, 2724–2731. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4739-05.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amiez, C., and Petrides, M. (2009). Anatomical organization of the eye fields in the human and non-human primate frontal cortex. Prog. Neurobiol. 89, 220–230. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.07.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Anderson, T. J., Jenkins, I. H., Brooks, D. J., Hawken, M. B., Frackowiak, R. S., and Kennard, C. (1994). Cortical control of saccades and fixation in man. A PET study. Brain 117(Pt 5), 1073–1084. doi: 10.1093/brain/117.5.1073

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Antal, A., Nitsche, M. A., Kincses, T. Z., Lampe, C., and Paulus, W. (2004). No correlation between moving phosphene and motor thresholds: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuroreport 15, 297–302. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200402090-00017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barone, P., Batardiere, A., Knoblauch, K., and Kennedy, H. (2000). Laminar distribution of neurons in extrastriate areas projecting to visual areas V1 and V4 correlates with the hierarchical rank and indicates the operation of a distance rule. J. Neurosci. 20, 3263–3281. Available online at: http://www.jneurosci.org/content/20/9/3263.long

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Beauchamp, M. S., Petit, L., Ellmore, T. M., Ingeholm, J., and Haxby, J. V. (2001). A parametric fMRI study of overt and covert shifts of visuospatial attention. Neuroimage 14, 310–321. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0788

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berman, R. A., Colby, C. L., Genovese, C. R., Voyvodic, J. T., Luna, B., Thulborn, K. R., et al. (1999). Cortical networks subserving pursuit and saccadic eye movements in humans: an FMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 209–225.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bichot, N. P., Schall, J. D., and Thompson, K. G. (1996). Visual feature selectivity in frontal eye fields induced by experience in mature macaques. Nature 381, 697–699. doi: 10.1038/381697a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bijsterbosch, J. D., Barker, A. T., Lee, K. H., and Woodruff, P. W. (2012). Where does transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) stimulate? Modelling of induced field maps for some common cortical and cerebellar targets. Med. Biol. Eng. Comput. 50, 671–681. doi: 10.1007/s11517-012-0922-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bizzi, E. (1968). Discharge of frontal eye field neurons during saccadic and following eye movements in unanesthetized monkeys. Exp. Brain Res. 6, 69–80. doi: 10.1007/BF00235447

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blanke, O., Morand, S., Thut, G., Michel, C. M., Spinelli, L., Landis, T., et al. (1999). Visual activity in the human frontal eye field. Neuroreport 10, 925–930. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199904060-00006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blanke, O., Spinelli, L., Thut, G., Michel, C. M., Perrig, S., Landis, T., et al. (2000). Location of the human frontal eye field as defined by electrical cortical stimulation: anatomical, functional and electrophysiological characteristics. Neuroreport 11, 1907–1913. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200006260-00021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boroojerdi, B., Meister, I. G., Foltys, H., Sparing, R., Cohen, L. G., and Topper, R. (2002). Visual and motor cortex excitability: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 113, 1501–1504. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00198-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bourgeois, A., Chica, A. B., Valero-Cabre, A., and Bartolomeo, P. (2013a). Cortical control of Inhibition of Return: exploring the causal contributions of the left parietal cortex. Cortex 49, 2927–2934. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.08.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bourgeois, A., Chica, A. B., Valero-Cabre, A., and Bartolomeo, P. (2013b). Cortical control of inhibition of return: causal evidence for task-dependent modulations by dorsal and ventral parietal regions. Cortex 49, 2229–2238. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.10.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bruce, C. J., and Goldberg, M. E. (1985). Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 53, 603–635.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bruce, C. J., Goldberg, M. E., Bushnell, M. C., and Stanton, G. B. (1985). Primate frontal eye fields. II. Physiological and anatomical correlates of electrically evoked eye movements. J. Neurophysiol. 54, 714–734.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bruno, N., and Franz, V. H. (2009). When is grasping affected by the Muller-Lyer illusion? A quantitative review. Neuropsychologia 47, 1421–1433. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bruno, N., Knox, P. C., and de Grave, D. D. (2010). A metanalysis of the effect of the Muller-Lyer illusion on saccadic eye movements: no general support for a dissociation of perception and oculomotor action. Vision Res. 50, 2671–2682. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Buschman, T. J., and Miller, E. K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315, 1860–1862. doi: 10.1126/science.1138071

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Campana, G., Cowey, A., Casco, C., Oudsen, I., and Walsh, V. (2007). Left frontal eye field remembers “where” but not “what”. Neuropsychologia 45, 2340–2345. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cavada, C., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1989). Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: II. Evidence for segregated corticocortical networks linking sensory and limbic areas with the frontal lobe. J. Comp. Neurol. 287, 422–445. doi: 10.1002/cne.902870403

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cazzoli, D., Muri, R. M., Schumacher, R., von Arx, S., Chaves, S., Gutbrod, K., et al. (2012). Theta burst stimulation reduces disability during the activities of daily living in spatial neglect. Brain 135, 3426–3439. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws182

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chanes, L., Chica, A. B., Quentin, R., and Valero-Cabre, A. (2012). Manipulation of pre-target activity on the right frontal eye field enhances conscious visual perception in humans. PLoS ONE 7:e36232. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036232

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chanes, L., Quentin, R., Tallon-Baudry, C., and Valero-Cabre, A. (2013). Causal frequency-specific contributions of frontal spatiotemporal patterns induced by non-invasive neurostimulation to human visual performance. J. Neurosci. 33, 5000–5005. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4401-12.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chica, A. B., Bartolomeo, P., and Valero-Cabre, A. (2011). Dorsal and ventral parietal contributions to spatial orienting in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 31, 8143–8149. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5463-10.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Corbetta, M., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E., Snyder, A. Z., Ollinger, J. M., Drury, H. A., et al. (1998). A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron 21, 761–773. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80593-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Corbetta, M., Miezin, F. M., Shulman, G. L., and Petersen, S. E. (1993). A PET study of visuospatial attention. J. Neurosci. 13, 1202–1226.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2011). Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 569–599. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113731

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Courtney, S. M., Petit, L., Maisog, J. M., Ungerleider, L. G., and Haxby, J. V. (1998). An area specialized for spatial working memory in human frontal cortex. Science 279, 1347–1351. doi: 10.1126/science.279.5355.1347

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Crosby, E. C., Yoss, R. E., and Henderson, J. W. (1952). The mammalian midbrain and isthmus regions. Part II. The fiber connections. D. The pattern for eye movements on the frontal eye field and the discharge of specific portions of this field to and through midbrain levels. J. Comp. Neurol. 97, 357–383. doi: 10.1002/cne.900970205

CrossRef Full Text

Darby, D. G., Nobre, A. C., Thangaraj, V., Edelman, R., Mesulam, M. M., and Warach, S. (1996). Cortical activation in the human brain during lateral saccades using EPISTAR functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 3, 53–62. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Deblieck, C., Thompson, B., Iacoboni, M., and Wu, A. D. (2008). Correlation between motor and phosphene thresholds: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 662–670. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20427

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Deubel, H., and Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Res. 36, 1827–1837. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00294-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dias, E. C., and Segraves, M. A. (1999). Muscimol-induced inactivation of monkey frontal eye field: effects on visually and memory-guided saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 81, 2191–2214.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Donner, T., Kettermann, A., Diesch, E., Ostendorf, F., Villringer, A., and Brandt, S. A. (2000). Involvement of the human frontal eye field and multiple parietal areas in covert visual selection during conjunction search. Eur. J. Neurosci. 12, 3407–3414. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00223.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Doricchi, F., Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Tomaiuolo, F., and Bartolomeo, P. (2008). White matter (dis)connections and gray matter (dys)functions in visual neglect: gaining insights into the brain networks of spatial awareness. Cortex 44, 983–995. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.03.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ferraina, S., Pare, M., and Wurtz, R. H. (2000). Disparity sensitivity of frontal eye field neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 625–629. Available online at: http://jn.physiology.org/content/83/1/625.long

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Ferrier, D. (1874). On the Localisation of the Functions of the Brain. Br. Med. J. 2, 766–767. doi: 10.1136/bmj.2.729.766

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fierro, B., Brighina, F., and Bisiach, E. (2006). Improving neglect by TMS. Behav. Neurol. 17, 169–176. doi: 10.1155/2006/465323

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Foerster, O. (1936). “Motorische Felder und Bahnen,” in Handbuch der Neurologie, eds O. Bumke and O. Foerster (Berlin: Springer), 46–141.

Fox, P. T., Fox, J. M., Raichle, M. E., and Burde, R. M. (1985). The role of cerebral cortex in the generation of voluntary saccades: a positron emission tomographic study. J. Neurophysiol. 54, 348–369.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Fukushima, K., Yamanobe, T., Shinmei, Y., Fukushima, J., Kurkin, S., and Peterson, B. W. (2002). Coding of smooth eye movements in three-dimensional space by frontal cortex. Nature 419, 157–162. doi: 10.1038/nature00953

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gagnon, D., Paus, T., Grosbras, M. H., Pike, G. B., and O'Driscoll, G. A. (2006). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of frontal oculomotor regions during smooth pursuit. J. Neurosci. 26, 458–466. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2789-05.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gamlin, P. D., and Yoon, K. (2000). An area for vergence eye movement in primate frontal cortex. Nature 407, 1003–1007. doi: 10.1038/35039506

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gaymard, B., Ploner, C. J., Rivaud-Pechoux, S., and Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. (1999). The frontal eye field is involved in spatial short-term memory but not in reflexive saccade inhibition. Exp. Brain Res. 129, 288–301. doi: 10.1007/s002210050899

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gaymard, B., Ploner, C. J., Rivaud, S., Vermersch, A. I., and Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. (1998a). Cortical control of saccades. Exp. Brain Res. 123, 159–163. doi: 10.1007/s002210050557

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gaymard, B., Rivaud, S., Cassarini, J. F., Dubard, T., Rancurel, G., Agid, Y., et al. (1998b). Effects of anterior cingulate cortex lesions on ocular saccades in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 120, 173–183. doi: 10.1007/s002210050391

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gerits, A., Ruff, C. C., Guipponi, O., Wenderoth, N., Driver, J., and Vanduffel, W. (2011). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of macaque frontal eye fields decreases saccadic reaction time. Exp. Brain Res. 212, 143–152. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2710-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gitelman, D. R., Nobre, A. C., Parrish, T. B., LaBar, K. S., Kim, Y. H., Meyer, J. R., et al. (1999). A large-scale distributed network for covert spatial attention: further anatomical delineation based on stringent behavioural and cognitive controls. Brain 122(Pt 6), 1093–1106. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.6.1093

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Godoy, J., Luders, H., Dinner, D. S., Morris, H. H., and Wyllie, E. (1990). Versive eye movements elicited by cortical stimulation of the human brain. Neurology 40, 296–299. doi: 10.1212/WNL.40.2.296

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gottlieb, J. P., Bruce, C. J., and MacAvoy, M. G. (1993). Smooth eye movements elicited by microstimulation in the primate frontal eye field. J. Neurophysiol. 69, 786–799.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gottlieb, J. P., MacAvoy, M. G., and Bruce, C. J. (1994). Neural responses related to smooth-pursuit eye movements and their correspondence with electrically elicited smooth eye movements in the primate frontal eye field. J. Neurophysiol. 72, 1634–1653.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Grosbras, M. H., Laird, A. R., and Paus, T. (2005). Cortical regions involved in eye movements, shifts of attention, and gaze perception. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 140–154. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20145

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grosbras, M. H., and Paus, T. (2002). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field: effects on visual perception and attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 1109–1120. doi: 10.1162/089892902320474553

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grosbras, M. H., and Paus, T. (2003). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field facilitates visual awareness. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, 3121–3126. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2003.03055.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Guitton, D., Buchtel, H. A., and Douglas, R. M. (1985). Frontal lobe lesions in man cause difficulties in suppressing reflexive glances and in generating goal-directed saccades. Exp. Brain Res. 58, 455–472. doi: 10.1007/BF00235863

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

He, B. J., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Epstein, A., Shulman, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2007). Breakdown of functional connectivity in frontoparietal networks underlies behavioral deficits in spatial neglect. Neuron 53, 905–918. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.02.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Helfrich, R. F., Schneider, T. R., Rach, S., Trautmann-Lengsfeld, S. A., Engel, A. K., and Herrmann, C. S. (2014). Entrainment of brain oscillations by transcranial alternating current stimulation. Curr. Biol. 24, 333–339. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.041

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Henik, A., Rafal, R., and Rhodes, D. (1994). Endogenously generated and visually guided saccades after lesions of the human frontal eye fields. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 400–411. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1994.6.4.400

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Huerta, M. F., Krubitzer, L. A., and Kaas, J. H. (1987). Frontal eye field as defined by intracortical microstimulation in squirrel monkeys, owl monkeys, and macaque monkeys. II. Cortical connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 265, 332–361. doi: 10.1002/cne.902650304

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ioannides, A. A., Corsi-Cabrera, M., Fenwick, P. B., del Rio Portilla, Y., Laskaris, N. A., Khurshudyan, A., et al. (2004). MEG tomography of human cortex and brainstem activity in waking and REM sleep saccades. Cereb. Cortex 14, 56–72. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg091

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ioannides, A. A., Fenwick, P. B., and Liu, L. (2005). Widely distributed magnetoencephalography spikes related to the planning and execution of human saccades. J. Neurosci. 25, 7950–7967. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1091-05.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ioannides, A. A., Fenwick, P. B., Pitri, E., and Liu, L. (2010). A step towards non-invasive characterization of the human frontal eye fields of individual subjects. Nonlin. Biomed. Phys. 4(Suppl. 10), S11. doi: 10.1186/1753-4631-4-S1-S11

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Izawa, Y., Suzuki, H., and Shinoda, Y. (2004a). Suppression of visually and memory-guided saccades induced by electrical stimulation of the monkey frontal eye field. II. Suppression of bilateral saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 2261–2273. doi: 10.1152/jn.00085.2004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Izawa, Y., Suzuki, H., and Shinoda, Y. (2004b). Suppression of visually and memory-guided saccades induced by electrical stimulation of the monkey frontal eye field. I. Suppression of ipsilateral saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 2248–2260. doi: 10.1152/jn.01021.2003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Izawa, Y., Suzuki, H., and Shinoda, Y. (2009). Response properties of fixation neurons and their location in the frontal eye field in the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 2410–2422. doi: 10.1152/jn.00234.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jainta, S., Bucci, M. P., Wiener-Vacher, S., and Kapoula, Z. (2011). Changes in vergence dynamics due to repetition. Vision Res. 51, 1845–1852. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.06.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jaun-Frutiger, K., Cazzoli, D., Muri, R. M., Bassetti, C. L., and Nyffeler, T. (2013). The frontal eye field is involved in visual vector inversion in humans–a theta burst stimulation study. PLoS ONE 8:e83297. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083297

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Juan, C. H., Muggleton, N. G., Tzeng, O. J., Hung, D. L., Cowey, A., and Walsh, V. (2008). Segregation of visual selection and saccades in human frontal eye fields. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2410–2415. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Juan, C. H., Shorter-Jacobi, S. M., and Schall, J. D. (2004). Dissociation of spatial attention and saccade preparation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 15541–15544. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0403507101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kahkonen, S., Komssi, S., Wilenius, J., and Ilmoniemi, R. J. (2005). Prefrontal TMS produces smaller EEG responses than motor-cortex TMS: implications for rTMS treatment in depression. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 181, 16–20. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2197-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kalla, R., Muggleton, N. G., Juan, C. H., Cowey, A., and Walsh, V. (2008). The timing of the involvement of the frontal eye fields and posterior parietal cortex in visual search. Neuroreport 19, 1067–1071. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328304d9c4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kawashima, R., Tanji, J., Okada, K., Sugiura, M., Sato, K., Kinomura, S., et al. (1998). Oculomotor sequence learning: a positron emission tomography study. Exp. Brain Res. 122, 1–8. doi: 10.1007/s002210050485

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kinsbourne, M. (1970). A model for the mechanism of unilateral neglect of space. Trans. Am. Neurol. Assoc. 95, 143–146.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Koch, G., Bonni, S., Giacobbe, V., Bucchi, G., Basile, B., Lupo, F., et al. (2012). Theta-burst stimulation of the left hemisphere accelerates recovery of hemispatial neglect. Neurology 78, 24–30. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31823ed08f

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Koyama, M., Hasegawa, I., Osada, T., Adachi, Y., Nakahara, K., and Miyashita, Y. (2004). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of macaque monkeys performing visually guided saccade tasks: comparison of cortical eye fields with humans. Neuron 41, 795–807. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00047-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kristjansson, A., Vuilleumier, P., Schwartz, S., Macaluso, E., and Driver, J. (2007). Neural basis for priming of pop-out during visual search revealed with fMRI. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1612–1624. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl072

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kurkin, S., Takeichi, N., Akao, T., Sato, F., Fukushima, J., Kaneko, C. R., et al. (2003). Neurons in the caudal frontal eye fields of monkeys signal three-dimensional tracking. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1004, 262–270. doi: 10.1196/annals.1303.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lane, A. R., Ball, K., Smith, D. T., Schenk, T., and Ellison, A. (2013). Near and far space: understanding the neural mechanisms of spatial attention. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 356–366. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21433

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lane, A. R., Smith, D. T., Schenk, T., and Ellison, A. (2012). The involvement of posterior parietal cortex and frontal eye fields in spatially primed visual search. Brain Stimul. 5, 11–17. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.01.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Law, I., Svarer, C., Holm, S., and Paulson, O. B. (1997). The activation pattern in normal humans during suppression, imagination and performance of saccadic eye movements. Acta Physiol. Scand. 161, 419–434. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-201X.1997.00207.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leff, A. P., Scott, S. K., Rothwell, J. C., and Wise, R. J. (2001). The planning and guiding of reading saccades: a repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cereb. Cortex 11, 918–923. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.10.918

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leichnetz, G. R., Smith, D. J., and Spencer, R. F. (1984). Cortical projections to the paramedian tegmental and basilar pons in the monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 228, 388–408. doi: 10.1002/cne.902280307

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Li, J., Olson, J., Anand, S., and Hotson, J. (1997). Rapid-rate transcranial magnetic stimulation of human frontal cortex can evoke saccades under facilitating conditions. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 105, 246–254. doi: 10.1016/S0924-980X(97)96127-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Luna, B., Thulborn, K. R., Strojwas, M. H., McCurtain, B. J., Berman, R. A., Genovese, C. R., et al. (1998). Dorsal cortical regions subserving visually guided saccades in humans: an fMRI study. Cereb. Cortex 8, 40–47. doi: 10.1093/cercor/8.1.40

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lynch, J. C., Hoover, J. E., and Strick, P. L. (1994). Input to the primate frontal eye field from the substantia nigra, superior colliculus, and dentate nucleus demonstrated by transneuronal transport. Exp. Brain Res. 100, 181–186. doi: 10.1007/BF00227293

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

MacAvoy, M. G., Gottlieb, J. P., and Bruce, C. J. (1991). Smooth-pursuit eye movement representation in the primate frontal eye field. Cereb. Cortex 1, 95–102. doi: 10.1093/cercor/1.1.95

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Machado, L., and Rafal, R. D. (2004a). Control of fixation and saccades in humans with chronic lesions of oculomotor cortex. Neuropsychology 18, 115–123. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.115

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Machado, L., and Rafal, R. D. (2004b). Control of fixation and saccades during an anti-saccade task: an investigation in humans with chronic lesions of oculomotor cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 156, 55–63. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1765-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Martinez-Conde, S., Otero-Millan, J., and Macknik, S. L. (2013). The impact of microsaccades on vision: towards a unified theory of saccadic function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 83–96. doi: 10.1038/nrn3405

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Milner, A. D., and Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia 46, 774–785. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.10.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moore, T., and Armstrong, K. M. (2003). Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex. Nature 421, 370–373. doi: 10.1038/nature01341

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moore, T., and Fallah, M. (2001). Control of eye movements and spatial attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98, 1273–1276. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.3.1273

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muggleton, N. G., Juan, C. H., Cowey, A., and Walsh, V. (2003). Human frontal eye fields and visual search. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 3340–3343. doi: 10.1152/jn.01086.2002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muggleton, N. G., Juan, C. H., Cowey, A., Walsh, V., and O'Breathnach, U. (2010). Human frontal eye fields and target switching. Cortex 46, 178–184. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.01.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muri, R. M., Hess, C. W., and Meienberg, O. (1991). Transcranial stimulation of the human frontal eye field by magnetic pulses. Exp. Brain Res. 86, 219–223. doi: 10.1007/BF00231057

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muri, R. M., Iba-Zizen, M. T., Derosier, C., Cabanis, E. A., and Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. (1996). Location of the human posterior eye field with functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatr. 60, 445–448. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.60.4.445

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muri, R. M., and Nyffeler, T. (2008). Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of reflexive and volitional saccades as revealed by lesion studies with neurological patients and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Brain Cogn. 68, 284–292. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nagel, M., Sprenger, A., Lencer, R., Kompf, D., Siebner, H., and Heide, W. (2008). Distributed representations of the “preparatory set” in the frontal oculomotor system: a TMS study. BMC Neurosci. 9:89. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-89

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Neggers, S. F., Huijbers, W., Vrijlandt, C. M., Vlaskamp, B. N., Schutter, D. J., and Kenemans, J. L. (2007). TMS pulses on the frontal eye fields break coupling between visuospatial attention and eye movements. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 2765–2778. doi: 10.1152/jn.00357.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., Wassermann, E. M., Priori, A., Lang, N., Antal, A., et al. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 1, 206–223. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nyffeler, T., Bucher, O., Pflugshaupt, T., Von Wartburg, R., Wurtz, P., Hess, C. W., et al. (2004). Single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation over the frontal eye field can facilitate and inhibit saccade triggering. Eur. J. Neurosci. 20, 2240–2244. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03667.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nyffeler, T., Wurtz, P., Luscher, H. R., Hess, C. W., Senn, W., Pflugshaupt, T., et al. (2006b). Repetitive TMS over the human oculomotor cortex: comparison of 1-Hz and theta burst stimulation. Neurosci. Lett. 409, 57–60. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.09.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nyffeler, T., Wurtz, P., Pflugshaupt, T., von Wartburg, R., Luthi, M., Hess, C. W., et al. (2006a). One-Hertz transcranial magnetic stimulation over the frontal eye field induces lasting inhibition of saccade triggering. Neuroreport 17, 273–275. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000199468.39659.bf

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Driscoll, G. A., Alpert, N. M., Matthysse, S. W., Levy, D. L., Rauch, S. L., and Holzman, P. S. (1995). Functional neuroanatomy of antisaccade eye movements investigated with positron emission tomography. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 92, 925–929. doi: 10.1073/pnas.92.3.925

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Driscoll, G. A., Wolff, A. L., Benkelfat, C., Florencio, P. S., Lal, S., and Evans, A. C. (2000). Functional neuroanatomy of smooth pursuit and predictive saccades. Neuroreport 11, 1335–1340. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200004270-00037

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Oliveri, M., Bisiach, E., Brighina, F., Piazza, A., La Bua, V., Buffa, D., et al. (2001). rTMS of the unaffected hemisphere transiently reduces contralesional visuospatial hemineglect. Neurology 57, 1338–1340. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.7.1338

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Olk, B., Chang, E., Kingstone, A., and Ro, T. (2006). Modulation of antisaccades by transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field. Cereb. Cortex 16, 76–82. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi085

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Shea, J., Muggleton, N. G., Cowey, A., and Walsh, V. (2004). Timing of target discrimination in human frontal eye fields. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1060–1067. doi: 10.1162/0898929041502634

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Shea, J., Muggleton, N. G., Cowey, A., and Walsh, V. (2007). Human frontal eye fields and spatial priming of pop-out. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1140–1151. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1140

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Shea, J., and Walsh, V. (2004). Visual awareness: the eye fields have it? Curr. Biol. 14:R279–281. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.03.024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Paus, T. (1996). Location and function of the human frontal eye-field: a selective review. Neuropsychologia 34, 475–483. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00134-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Petit, L., Clark, V. P., Ingeholm, J., and Haxby, J. V. (1997). Dissociation of saccade-related and pursuit-related activation in human frontal eye fields as revealed by fMRI. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 3386–3390.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Petit, L., and Haxby, J. V. (1999). Functional anatomy of pursuit eye movements in humans as revealed by fMRI. J. Neurophysiol. 82, 463–471. Available online at: http://jn.physiology.org/content/82/1/463.long

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Petit, L., Orssaud, C., Tzourio, N., Crivello, F., Berthoz, A., and Mazoyer, B. (1996). Functional anatomy of a prelearned sequence of horizontal saccades in humans. J. Neurosci. 16, 3714–3726.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Petit, L., Tzourio, N., Orssaud, C., Pietrzyk, U., Berthoz, A., and Mazoyer, B. (1995). Functional neuroanatomy of the human visual fixation system. Eur. J. Neurosci. 7, 169–174. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1995.tb01031.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Milea, D., and Muri, R. M. (2004). Eye movement control by the cerebral cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 17, 17–25. doi: 10.1097/00019052-200402000-00005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Ploner, C. J., Muri, R. M., Gaymard, B., and Rivaud-Pechoux, S. (2002). Effects of cortical lesions on saccadic: eye movements in humans. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 956, 216–229. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb02821.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Rivaud, S., Gaymard, B., and Agid, Y. (1991). Cortical control of reflexive visually-guided saccades. Brain 114(Pt 3), 1473–1485. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.3.1473

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Rivaud, S., Gaymard, B., Muri, R., and Vermersch, A. I. (1995). Cortical control of saccades. Ann. Neurol. 37, 557–567. doi: 10.1002/ana.410370504

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pierrot-Deseilligny, C., Rivaud, S., Penet, C., and Rigolet, M. H. (1987). Latencies of visually guided saccades in unilateral hemispheric cerebral lesions. Ann. Neurol. 21, 138–148. doi: 10.1002/ana.410210206

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pigarev, I. N., Rizzolatti, G., and Scandolara, C. (1979). Neurons responding to visual stimuli in the frontal lobe of macaque monkeys. Neurosci. Lett. 12, 207–212. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(79)96063-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plewnia, C., Rilk, A. J., Soekadar, S. R., Arfeller, C., Huber, H. S., Sauseng, P., et al. (2008). Enhancement of long-range EEG coherence by synchronous bifocal transcranial magnetic stimulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 1577–1583. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06124.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pouget, P., Wattiez, N., and Valero-Cabre, A. (2011). Comment on: Exp Brain Res. 2011 May 5th. Transcranial magnetic stimulation of macaque frontal eye fields decreases saccadic reaction time. Gerits A, Ruff CC, Guipponi O, Wenderoth N, Driver J, Vanduffel W. Exp. Brain Res. 214, 481–482. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2840-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Preuss, T. M., Stepniewska, I., and Kaas, J. H. (1996). Movement representation in the dorsal and ventral premotor areas of owl monkeys: a microstimulation study. J. Comp. Neurol. 371, 649–676.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Prime, S. L., Vesia, M., and Crawford, J. D. (2010). TMS over human frontal eye fields disrupts trans-saccadic memory of multiple objects. Cereb. Cortex 20, 759–772. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp148

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Priori, A., Bertolasi, L., Rothwell, J. C., Day, B. L., and Marsden, C. D. (1993). Some saccadic eye movements can be delayed by transcranial magnetic stimulation of the cerebral cortex in man. Brain 116(Pt 2), 355–367. doi: 10.1093/brain/116.2.355

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Priot, A. E., Neveu, P., Sillan, O., Plantier, J., Roumes, C., and Prablanc, C. (2012). How perceived egocentric distance varies with changes in tonic vergence. Exp. Brain Res. 219, 457–465. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3097-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Quentin, R., Chanes, L., Migliaccio, R., Valabregue, R., and Valero-Cabre, A. (2013). Fronto-tectal white matter connectivity mediates facilitatory effects of non-invasive neurostimulation on visual detection. Neuroimage 82, 344–354. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.083

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Quentin, R., Chanes, L., Vernet, M., and Valero-Cabre, A. (2014). Fronto-parietal anatomical connections influence the modulation of conscious visual perception by high-beta frontal oscillatory activity. Cereb. Cortex. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu014. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rasmussen, T., and Penfield, W. (1948). Movement of head and eyes from stimulation of human frontal cortex. Res. Publ. Assoc. Res. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 27(1 vol.), 346–361.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rastelli, F., Tallon-Baudry, C., Migliaccio, R., Toba, M. N., Ducorps, A., Pradat-Diehl, P., et al. (2013). Neural dynamics of neglected targets in patients with right hemisphere damage. Cortex 49, 1989–1996. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.04.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ringman, J. M., Saver, J. L., Woolson, R. F., Clarke, W. R., and Adams, H. P. (2004). Frequency, risk factors, anatomy, and course of unilateral neglect in an acute stroke cohort. Neurology 63, 468–474. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000133011.10689.CE

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rivaud, S., Muri, R. M., Gaymard, B., Vermersch, A. I., and Pierrot-Deseilligny, C. (1994). Eye movement disorders after frontal eye field lesions in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 102, 110–120. doi: 10.1007/BF00232443

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rizzolatti, G., Matelli, M., and Pavesi, G. (1983). Deficits in attention and movement following the removal of postarcuate (area 6) and prearcuate (area 8) cortex in macaque monkeys. Brain 106(Pt 3):655–673. doi: 10.1093/brain/106.3.655

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., and Umilta, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia 25, 31–40. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(87)90041-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ro, T., Cheifet, S., Ingle, H., Shoup, R., and Rafal, R. (1999). Localization of the human frontal eye fields and motor hand area with transcranial magnetic stimulation and magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychologia 37, 225–231. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00097-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ro, T., Farne, A., and Chang, E. (2002). Locating the human frontal eye fields with transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 24, 930–940. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.7.930.8385

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ro, T., Farne, A., and Chang, E. (2003). Inhibition of return and the human frontal eye fields. Exp. Brain Res. 150, 290–296.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Ro, T., Henik, A., Machado, L., and Rafal, R. D. (1997). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the prefrontal cortex delays contralateral endogenous saccades. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 433–440. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.433

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Robinson, D. A., and Fuchs, A. F. (1969). Eye movements evoked by stimulation of frontal eye fields. J. Neurophysiol. 32, 637–648.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rosano, C., Krisky, C. M., Welling, J. S., Eddy, W. F., Luna, B., Thulborn, K. R., et al. (2002). Pursuit and saccadic eye movement subregions in human frontal eye field: a high-resolution fMRI investigation. Cereb. Cortex 12, 107–115. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.2.107

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rosano, C., Sweeney, J. A., Melchitzky, D. S., and Lewis, D. A. (2003). The human precentral sulcus: chemoarchitecture of a region corresponding to the frontal eye fields. Brain Res. 972, 16–30. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(03)02431-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rossi, S., Hallett, M., Rossini, P. M., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2009). Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 2008–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ruff, C. C., Blankenburg, F., Bjoertomt, O., Bestmann, S., Freeman, E., Haynes, J. D., et al. (2006). Concurrent TMS-fMRI and psychophysics reveal frontal influences on human retinotopic visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 16, 1479–1488. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.06.057

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schall, J. D., and Bichot, N. P. (1998). Neural correlates of visual and motor decision processes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 8, 211–217. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80142-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schall, J. D., Morel, A., and Kaas, J. H. (1993). Topography of supplementary eye field afferents to frontal eye field in macaque: implications for mapping between saccade coordinate systems. Vis. Neurosci. 10, 385–393. doi: 10.1017/S0952523800003771

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Scheiman, M., Cotter, S., Rouse, M., Mitchell, G. L., Kulp, M., Cooper, J., et al. (2005a). Randomised clinical trial of the effectiveness of base-in prism reading glasses versus placebo reading glasses for symptomatic convergence insufficiency in children. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 89, 1318–1323. doi: 10.1136/bjo.2005.068197

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Scheiman, M., Mitchell, G. L., Cotter, S., Cooper, J., Kulp, M., Rouse, M., et al. (2005b). A randomized clinical trial of treatments for convergence insufficiency in children. Arch. Ophthalmol. 123, 14–24. doi: 10.1001/archopht.123.1.14

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Scheiman, M., Mitchell, G. L., Cotter, S., Kulp, M. T., Cooper, J., Rouse, M., et al. (2005c). A randomized clinical trial of vision therapy/orthoptics versus pencil pushups for the treatment of convergence insufficiency in young adults. Optom. Vis. Sci. 82, 583–595. doi: 10.1097/01.opx.0000171331.36871.2f

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schlag-Rey, M., Schlag, J., and Dassonville, P. (1992). How the frontal eye field can impose a saccade goal on superior colliculus neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 67, 1003–1005.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Schmolesky, M. T., Wang, Y., Hanes, D. P., Thompson, K. G., Leutgeb, S., Schall, J. D., et al. (1998). Signal timing across the macaque visual system. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 3272–3278.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Segraves, M. A. (1992). Activity of monkey frontal eye field neurons projecting to oculomotor regions of the pons. J. Neurophysiol. 68, 1967–1985.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Segraves, M. A., and Goldberg, M. E. (1987). Functional properties of corticotectal neurons in the monkey's frontal eye field. J. Neurophysiol. 58, 1387–1419.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Sheliga, B. M., and Miles, F. A. (2003). Perception can influence the vergence responses associated with open-loop gaze shifts in 3D. J. Vis. 3, 654–676. doi: 10.1167/3.11.2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Silvanto, J., Lavie, N., and Walsh, V. (2006). Stimulation of the human frontal eye fields modulates sensitivity of extrastriate visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 96, 941–945. doi: 10.1152/jn.00015.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, D. T., Jackson, S. R., and Rorden, C. (2005). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the left human frontal eye fields eliminates the cost of invalid endogenous cues. Neuropsychologia 43, 1288–1296. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.12.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sommer, M. A., and Tehovnik, E. J. (1997). Reversible inactivation of macaque frontal eye field. Exp. Brain Res. 116, 229–249. doi: 10.1007/PL00005752

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sparing, R., Thimm, M., Hesse, M. D., Kust, J., Karbe, H., and Fink, G. R. (2009). Bidirectional alterations of interhemispheric parietal balance by non-invasive cortical stimulation. Brain 132, 3011–3020. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp154

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sprague, J. M. (1966). Interaction of cortex and superior colliculus in mediation of visually guided behavior in the cat. Science 153, 1544–1547. doi: 10.1126/science.153.3743.1544

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stanton, G. B., Bruce, C. J., and Goldberg, M. E. (1993). Topography of projections to the frontal lobe from the macaque frontal eye fields. J. Comp. Neurol. 330, 286–301. doi: 10.1002/cne.903300209

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stanton, G. B., Bruce, C. J., and Goldberg, M. E. (1995). Topography of projections to posterior cortical areas from the macaque frontal eye fields. J. Comp. Neurol. 353, 291–305. doi: 10.1002/cne.903530210

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stanton, G. B., Deng, S. Y., Goldberg, M. E., and McMullen, N. T. (1989). Cytoarchitectural characteristic of the frontal eye fields in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 282, 415–427. doi: 10.1002/cne.902820308

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stanton, G. B., Goldberg, M. E., and Bruce, C. J. (1988). Frontal eye field efferents in the macaque monkey: I. Subcortical pathways and topography of striatal and thalamic terminal fields. J. Comp. Neurol. 271, 473–492. doi: 10.1002/cne.902710402

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stewart, L. M., Walsh, V., and Rothwell, J. C. (2001). Motor and phosphene thresholds: a transcranial magnetic stimulation correlation study. Neuropsychologia 39, 415–419. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00130-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sweeney, J. A., Mintun, M. A., Kwee, S., Wiseman, M. B., Brown, D. L., Rosenberg, D. R., et al. (1996). Positron emission tomography study of voluntary saccadic eye movements and spatial working memory. J. Neurophysiol. 75, 454–468.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Taylor, P. C., Nobre, A. C., and Rushworth, M. F. (2007). FEF TMS affects visual cortical activity. Cereb. Cortex 17, 391–399. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj156

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tehovnik, E. J., Sommer, M. A., Chou, I. H., Slocum, W. M., and Schiller, P. H. (2000). Eye fields in the frontal lobes of primates. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 32, 413–448. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00092-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Terao, Y., Fukuda, H., Ugawa, Y., Hikosaka, O., Hanajima, R., Furubayashi, T., et al. (1998). Visualization of the information flow through human oculomotor cortical regions by transcranial magnetic stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 936–946.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Thickbroom, G. W., Stell, R., and Mastaglia, F. L. (1996). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of the human frontal eye field. J. Neurol. Sci. 144, 114–118. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(96)00194-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thompson, K. G., and Bichot, N. P. (2005). A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. Prog. Brain Res. 147, 251–262. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(04)47019-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thompson, K. G., Bichot, N. P., and Schall, J. D. (1997). Dissociation of visual discrimination from saccade programming in macaque frontal eye field. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 1046–1050.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Thompson, K. G., Hanes, D. P., Bichot, N. P., and Schall, J. D. (1996). Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 4040–4055.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Thut, G., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2010). A review of combined TMS-EEG studies to characterize lasting effects of repetitive TMS and assess their usefulness in cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Brain Topogr. 22, 219–232. doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0115-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thut, G., Veniero, D., Romei, V., Miniussi, C., Schyns, P., and Gross, J. (2011). Rhythmic TMS causes local entrainment of natural oscillatory signatures. Curr. Biol. 21, 1176–1185. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.05.049

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tian, J. R., and Lynch, J. C. (1996). Corticocortical input to the smooth and saccadic eye movement subregions of the frontal eye field in Cebus monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 2754–2771.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Valero-Cabre, A., Pascual-Leone, A., and Coubard, O. A. (2011). Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in basic and clinical neuroscience research. Rev. Neurol. (Paris). 167, 291–316. doi: 10.1016/j.neurol.2010.10.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Valero-Cabre, A., Payne, B. R., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2007). Opposite impact on 14C-2-deoxyglucose brain metabolism following patterns of high and low frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in the posterior parietal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 176, 603–615. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0639-8

CrossRef Full Text

Valero-Cabre, A., Payne, B. R., Rushmore, J., Lomber, S. G., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Impact of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the parietal cortex on metabolic brain activity: a 14C-2DG tracing study in the cat. Exp. Brain Res. 163, 1–12. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2140-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Valero-Cabre, A., Wattiez, N., Monfort, M., Francois, C., Rivaud-Pechoux, S., Gaymard, B., et al. (2012). Frontal non-invasive neurostimulation modulates antisaccade preparation in non-human primates. PLoS ONE 7:e38674. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038674

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Donkelaar, P., Lin, Y., and Hewlett, D. (2009). The human frontal oculomotor cortical areas contribute asymmetrically to motor planning in a gap saccade task. PLoS ONE 4:e7278. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007278

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Ettinger-Veenstra, H. M., Huijbers, W., Gutteling, T. P., Vink, M., Kenemans, J. L., and Neggers, S. F. (2009). fMRI-guided TMS on cortical eye fields: the frontal but not intraparietal eye fields regulate the coupling between visuospatial attention and eye movements. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 3469–3480. doi: 10.1152/jn.00350.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vernet, M., Bashir, S., Yoo, W. K., Perez, J. M., Najib, U., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2013). Insights on the neural basis of motor plasticity induced by theta burst stimulation from TMS-EEG. Eur. J. Neurosci. 37, 598–606. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12069

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vernet, M., Yang, Q., Gruselle, M., Trams, M., and Kapoula, Z. (2009). Switching between gap and overlap pro-saccades: cost or benefit? Exp. Brain Res. 197, 49–58. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1887-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vuilleumier, P., Hester, D., Assal, G., and Regli, F. (1996). Unilateral spatial neglect recovery after sequential strokes. Neurology 46, 184–189. doi: 10.1212/WNL.46.1.184

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wagner, M., Ehrenstein, W. H., and Papathomas, T. V. (2009). Vergence in reverspective: percept-driven versus data-driven eye movement control. Neurosci. Lett. 449, 142–146. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.10.093

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wagner, T., Valero-Cabre, A., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2007). Noninvasive human brain stimulation. Annu. Rev. Biomed. Eng. 9, 527–565. doi: 10.1146/annurev.bioeng.9.061206.133100

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Walker, R., Techawachirakul, P., and Haggard, P. (2009). Frontal eye field stimulation modulates the balance of salience between target and distractors. Brain Res. 1270, 54–63. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.081

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Weiss, P. H., Marshall, J. C., Wunderlich, G., Tellmann, L., Halligan, P. W., Freund, H. J., et al. (2000). Neural consequences of acting in near versus far space: a physiological basis for clinical dissociations. Brain 123(Pt 12), 2531–2541. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.12.2531

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wessel, K., and Kompf, D. (1991). Transcranial magnetic brain stimulation: lack of oculomotor response. Exp. Brain Res. 86, 216–218. doi: 10.1007/BF00231056

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wipfli, M., Felblinger, J., Mosimann, U. P., Hess, C. W., Schlaepfer, T. E., and Muri, R. M. (2001). Double-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation over the frontal eye field facilitates triggering of memory-guided saccades. Eur. J. Neurosci. 14, 571–575. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01671.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wismeijer, D. A., van Ee, R., and Erkelens, C. J. (2008). Depth cues, rather than perceived depth, govern vergence. Exp. Brain Res. 184, 61–70. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1081-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wurtz, R. H., and Mohler, C. W. (1976). Enhancement of visual responses in monkey striate cortex and frontal eye fields. J. Neurophysiol. 39, 766–772.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Yang, Q., and Kapoula, Z. (2011). Distinct control of initiation and metrics of memory-guided saccades and vergence by the FEF: a TMS study. PLoS ONE 6:e20322. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020322

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yang, Q., Vernet, M., Orssaud, C., Bonfils, P., Londero, A., and Kapoula, Z. (2010). Central crosstalk for somatic tinnitus: abnormal vergence eye movements. PLoS ONE 5:e11845. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011845

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zangemeister, W. H., Canavan, A. G., and Hoemberg, V. (1995). Frontal and parietal transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) disturbs programming of saccadic eye movements. J. Neurol. Sci. 133, 42–52. doi: 10.1016/0022-510X(95)00158-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ziegler, L. R., and Hess, R. F. (1997). Depth perception during diplopia is direct. Perception 26, 1125–1130. doi: 10.1068/p261225

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: FEF, brain mapping, transcranial magnetic stimulation, visual performance, visuo-spatial attention, 3D vision, visual awareness, plasticity rehabilitation

Citation: Vernet M, Quentin R, Chanes L, Mitsumasu A and Valero-Cabré A (2014) Frontal eye field, where art thou? Anatomy, function, and non-invasive manipulation of frontal regions involved in eye movements and associated cognitive operations. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:66. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00066

Received: 31 May 2014; Accepted: 01 August 2014;
Published online: 22 August 2014.

Edited by:

Olivier A. Coubard, CNS-Fed, France

Reviewed by:

Albino J. Oliveira-Maia, Champalimaud Foundation, Portugal
Thomas Nyffeler, Bern University Hospital and University of Bern, Switzerland

Copyright © 2014 Vernet, Quentin, Chanes, Mitsumasu and Valero-Cabré. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marine Vernet, Groupe de Dynamiques Cérébrales, Plasticité et Rééducation, Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, Pitié-Salpêtrière, 47 Boulevard de l'Hôpital, 75013 Paris, France e-mail: marine.vernet@gmail.com

Download