Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 28 August 2014
Volume 8 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00149

Is there a tape recorder in your head? How the brain stores and retrieves musical melodies

  • 1Department of Neuroscience, Georgetown University Medical Center, Washington, DC, USA
  • 2Institute for Advanced Studies, Technical University Munich, Garching, Germany

Music consists of strings of sound that vary over time. Technical devices, such as tape recorders, store musical melodies by transcribing event times of temporal sequences into consecutive locations on the storage medium. Playback occurs by reading out the stored information in the same sequence. However, it is unclear how the brain stores and retrieves auditory sequences. Neurons in the anterior lateral belt of auditory cortex are sensitive to the combination of sound features in time, but the integration time of these neurons is not sufficient to store longer sequences that stretch over several seconds, minutes or more. Functional imaging studies in humans provide evidence that music is stored instead within the auditory dorsal stream, including premotor and prefrontal areas. In monkeys, these areas are the substrate for learning of motor sequences. It appears, therefore, that the auditory dorsal stream transforms musical into motor sequence information and vice versa, realizing what are known as forward and inverse models. The basal ganglia and the cerebellum are involved in setting up the sensorimotor associations, translating timing information into spatial codes and back again.

Musical Melodies as Sequences and Objects

Musical melodies are sequences of sound with particular rhythm, loudness and timbre. As such, they are concatenations of discrete elements over time, which can continue for seconds or minutes. However, we can learn to recognize melodies as a single entity, as we recognize extended objects in either the visual or auditory modality, and we can assign a name to them (“Twinkle, twinkle, little star” or “Yankee doodle”). In this more holistic view, a melody is an entity that requires integration of its elements over time and, ultimately, coding by a specific, limited ensemble of neurons in the brain. This latter representation is likely to be situated in the auditory ventral stream, where representations of “auditory objects” have been found (Tian et al., 2001; Zatorre et al., 2004). In a hierarchical model, information about spectral structure and temporal modulation, including pitch, are stored in early ventral areas and in core (Leaver and Rauschecker, 2010; Schindler et al., 2013); higher-order object information, e.g., about timbre, which would reveal the identity of an instrument or singer, is most likely found in the anterior-most regions of superior temporal cortex (Leaver and Rauschecker, 2010) and in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (Cohen et al., 2009; Plakke et al., 2013). Even in the most hierarchical model, however, it seems unlikely to find single neurons responding selectively to lengthy melodies, just as it seems unreasonable to expect single neurons to respond to specific sentences in the language domain. So how is the identity of a sound sequence warranted in the brain?

For speech, regions in the anterior superior temporal cortex (aSTC) have been found that respond to phonemes or words, including short standard phrases (DeWitt and Rauschecker, 2012), but not to whole sentences. The latter would seem to reside in the auditory dorsal stream instead, where representations of sequences have been found (Schubotz et al., 2004). Activation of dorsal-stream regions, including supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas (SMA, pre-SMA) or ventral and dorsal premotor cortex (vPMC, dPMC), has also been reported during singing (Perry et al., 1999), listening to music (Chen et al., 2008), and during anticipatory imagery of music (Leaver et al., 2009).

But how does the storage process of lengthy sound sequences really happen? This is not at all a trivial question, and the brain mechanisms governing the processing, storage and retrieval of sequences are far from understood. It may be advantageous, therefore, to briefly consider how technical devices do this.

How Tape Recorders Work

A tape recorder is an audio storage device that records and plays back sounds, including music and speech, using magnetic tape as a storage medium. It records a fluctuating audio signal by moving the tape across a “tape-head” that polarizes the magnetic domains in the tape in proportion to the audio signal (modified from Wikipedia). Electric current flowing in the tape-head creates a fluctuating magnetic field, which causes the magnetic material on the tape to align in a manner proportional to the original signal, as the tape is moving past the head. The original signal can be re-produced by running the tape back across the tape head, where the reverse process occurs—the magnetic imprint on the tape induces a small current in the reading head, which approximates the original signal and is then amplified for playback on a loudspeaker (from Wikipedia).

Thus, a tape recorder stores music by moving a storage medium (the tape) past a device (the head) that represents the sound waves in the form of a fluctuating electro-magnetic field. A turntable or CD player follows the same principle of using the movement of a recording medium to translate time into space, this time in the form of a spiral track. In all cases, the recording process can be inverted into a playback process by the reverse mechanism, moving the recorded medium past the reading device at the same speed, thus recreating the original signal.

The important message to be gleaned from this is that technical devices store musical melodies (as well as other sequences) by re-coding time of occurrence into spatial positions. Furthermore, storage and retrieval of the sequence utilize the same mechanism, differing only by inversion. Applied to the brain, it is attractive to think that information is stored in the same places where the original activation takes place, and that recording and read-out are also accomplished by similar, but inverse mechanisms. But how is the order of events in a time sequence preserved? At first, the only way to form temporal associations between stored items would seem to be by “chaining” the events together, whereby one event becomes the cue for the next one (Ebbinghaus, 1964). Read-out takes the form of cued recall. Although this idea has been criticized (Lashley, 1951; Terrace, 2005), it still provides one possible mechanism for storing a sequence, but it remains unclear how it is implemented in the brain.

Obviously, unlike a tape recorder or CD player, the brain does not have any moving parts for the translation of time into space. Then again, digital storage devices (solid-state or flash drives) no longer require moving tapes or spinning discs. These devices store audio as a stream of numbers representing the amplitude of the audio signal at equal time intervals. The numbers get stored in the order they are received, and a “controller” assures that they are read out in the same order later. This form of storing a sequence requires a positional code, i.e., the re-coding of event time into position in space, something that has been postulated variously for models of short-term memory as well (Henson and Burgess, 1997).

In summary, technical devices universally store sequences by re-coding time of occurrence into spatial positions, and the fundamental question arises: How does the brain translate temporal events in a sequence into spatial patterns or a spatial gradient?

Neural Mechanisms for the Encoding of Sequential Order

Temporal Combination Sensitivity as the Most Elementary Mechanism

Most simplistically, music consists of two essential elements: frequency (or pitch) and rhythm. However, while rhythm (duration of tones and the intervals between them) is obviously important, we can still recognize a melody (within limits) even when rhythmic elements are omitted. Recent results confirm that pitch and rhythm are indeed processed and stored independently (Schellenberg et al., 2014). Thus, the most essential element for the recognition of a melody is the order of the notes it consists of. If that order is changed, or the melody is played in reverse, recognition is impaired or fails altogether. Again, there is commonality between music and language (c.f. Patel, 2008; Patel and Iversen, 2014), as language comprehension also becomes impossible when its elements are played in reverse (either at the word or sentence level) (Bornkessel-Schlesewsky and Schlesewsky, 2013).

A neural mechanism that is commonly invoked for implementing this reversal sensitivity is the combination of inputs over time (temporal combination sensitivity, TCS). Just as in its twin mechanism, spectral combination sensitivity (Margoliash and Fortune, 1992), the target neuron acts as a logical AND-gate which fires only if several inputs are active simultaneously. In the time domain, delay lines can be used to hold up some of the inputs long enough until all other inputs have arrived (Figure 1). These asymmetric delays have the effect of creating selectivity for temporal order on a short time scale in the order of hundreds of milliseconds. Thus, temporally asymmetric delays can be created by spatial asymmetries on a miniature scale similar to direction selectivity in the visual system. This mechanism creates FM detectors with pronounced selectivity for the direction of an FM sweep (Tian and Rauschecker, 2004; see also Tian et al., 2013 for further analogies between elemental detectors in visual and auditory cortex).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Auditory direction selectivity of a cortical neuron as cellular basis for sequence selectivity. (A) Schematic drawing of a neuron in the lateral belt of rhesus monkey auditory cortex, illustrating temporal combination sensitivity (TCS). Input from lower-order neurons is integrated at the level of the lateral belt in a nonlinear fashion (Rauschecker et al., 1995). The belt neuron acts as a logical AND-gate and fires only if the membrane potential surpasses a given threshold. Temporal delay lines generate order sensitivity such that a sound sequence will excite the neuron only if presented in a specific order (from Rauschecker, 2012). (B, C) Example of a response by a neuron in the lateral belt to a species-specific vocalization. Spectrograms of the call and its temporal components are shown in (B) together with the reversed call (on extreme right). The neuron’s response (shown in (C)) to individual “syllables” and to the reversed call is strongly diminished.

Premotor Areas as Sequencing Machines

While the above TCS mechanism works well at durations corresponding to syllable or word level, it breaks down when the strings of sound become longer. Under those circumstances, one may assume that chaining mechanisms come into play, where the end of one short sequence triggers the beginning of the next, like in a game of dominoes. Such mechanisms have been postulated in particular for the motor system, where the execution of smooth movements requires precise timing and order of muscle activations. Brain substrates that play a role for the learning, planning and execution of such sequential behavior are thought to be the cerebellum, the striatum, and various regions of premotor and prefrontal cortex (Hikosaka et al., 1996; Sakai et al., 1999; Fuster et al., 2000; Yin, 2010). While premotor and prefrontal areas are most important for planning and execution, cerebellum and basal ganglia are involved at different stages of learning of a motor sequence. In particular, cerebellum and striatum differ by the time scales they apply to the transformation of temporal into spatial patterns.

It is important to keep in mind that music is often created by another person making it. That is, someone is producing the music before we can listen to it, and a melody is first and foremost a motor sequence that happens to produce sounds. This is true even if we produce the music ourselves. We produce music by virtue of activating muscles that move our vocal cords, lips and jaws (during singing or whistling) or, depending on the type of musical instrument played, we move our arms, fingers, feet, and sometimes our lips in coordination with our breathing apparatus (This is similar again in speech, where we learn to produce a sound by moving our muscles of the lips, tongue etc. in coordination with the vocal cords and breathing muscles). Hearing another person produce these sounds may trigger the same or similar muscle movements, with the goal of producing the same sounds. This may happen either as a form of imitation, or directly as a result of sensorimotor interaction that, by necessity, intertwines perception and action during the production of these sounds. In other words, the feedback from hearing (and to some extent proprioception) is a necessary prerequisite for normal production of producible sounds. The process can best be appreciated in reference to speaking or singing, where we have the same “instrument”, our vocal apparatus, at our disposal as the models we are trying to emulate. However, even when listening to a musical instrument that we are not capable of playing ourselves, we can produce the same melody by generating tones in the same order and with the same timing as the ones we listen to.

It will be interesting to find out when this ability to re-produce sound sequences first develops. Although young infants have the ability to recognize familiar melodies as early as 2 months of age, they do not develop relative pitch until ~6 months of age and not without exposure to music (Plantinga and Trainor, 2009).

Sensorimotor Learning in Nonhuman Primates

Studies in monkeys have shown that during learning of a new sensorimotor association the basal ganglia are very active (Pasupathy and Miller, 2005). The same has been shown by functional imaging studies in humans that are learning new sequences (Leaver et al., 2009; Yin, 2010). These results assign a role to the basal ganglia in the chaining or stitching together of new sensorimotor associations or, more succinctly, in the transformation of temporal order information into a spatial code (Kalm and Norris, 2014). After a sequence is well learned, activation of premotor and prefrontal regions becomes increasingly prominent, while basal ganglia activation weakens (Figure 2A; Leaver et al., 2009). This reflects the formation of chunks of sequence items, consistent with human learning and imaging studies (Janata and Grafton, 2003), which are stored in frontal areas like pre-SMA and SFG (Sakai et al., 1999; Sakai and Passingham, 2003). The activation moves more rostral as the sequence becomes more familiar (Leaver et al., 2009). This is consistent with a caudal-to-rostral hierarchy within prefrontal cortex (Badre and D’Esposito, 2009), where rostral areas control activity in more caudal modality-specific areas (Sakai and Passingham, 2003).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Participation of auditory dorsal stream in coding of musical sequences. (A) Activation of areas in the auditory dorsal stream by anticipation of familiar music. Activated areas include the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas (SMA, pre-SMA), the inferior parietal lobule (IPL), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), globus pallidus and putamen (GP/Pu) of the basal ganglia, and the cerebellum (CB) (from Leaver et al., 2009). (B) Illustration of the auditory ventral and dorsal streams in the human brain (modified from Rauschecker and Scott, 2009). This expanded model originated from the original dual-pathway model of Rauschecker and Tian (2000) by generalizing the role of the dorsal stream to one of sensorimotor integration and control, which includes processing of space and motion as well as storage and retrieval of sound sequences, the latter especially relevant for processing of music.

It is currently unclear if it is possible to learn a new melody or sequence without engaging these sensorimotor mechanisms by just passively listening to it. As a melody becomes increasingly familiar, it often becomes impossible to suppress the urge to sing along. While the learning of a new song or a new piece played on an instrument results in the building of “muscle memory” by tuning the motor and premotor structures of the brain, this may not happen in individuals that lack the corresponding skills. It would be interesting to see if there are certain forms of amusia that lack the ability to reproduce or recognize music, and whether this is actually a weakness of their sensorimotor memory and also affects their general ability to remember sequences (c.f. Tremblay-Champoux et al., 2010). Interestingly, some forms of congenital amusia involve structural changes in the inferior frontal region (Hyde et al., 2007), but more research is needed to possibly tie these changes to a domain-general deficit in sequence processing.

Singing in Birds

Vocal learning is not unique to humans. It is common in a variety of animal species (Patel and Iversen, 2014), especially birds. Some songbird species (such as zebra finches or starlings) learn their melodies from a conspecific teacher, usually their father (Comins and Gentner, 2010; Adret et al., 2012); others (such as parrots or bullfinches) can also imitate words or melodies they hear from humans (Eda-Fujiwara et al., 2012; Nicolai et al., 2014).

A wealth of neurobiological studies in several songbird species suggests that their neural apparatus for audio-motor learning is quite similar in principle to that of humans and nonhuman primates, consisting of premotor-basal-ganglia circuits that work in conjunction with higher auditory centers to encode the memorized songs (Achiro and Bottjer, 2013). In particular, recent data from zebra finches show that vocal motor circuits also participate in the encoding of auditory experience of the vocal model (Roberts and Mooney, 2013). Thus, a universal circuit model is beginning to emerge from these comparative studies that might ultimately lead to an understanding of storage and retrieval of sound sequences in biological systems.

Synthesis: Melodies in Ventral and Dorsal Streams

Much evidence suggests that the dual auditory processing streams originally postulated for the monkey (Rauschecker, 1997, 1998; Romanski et al., 1999; Rauschecker and Tian, 2000) also exist in humans. The ventral auditory stream is important for the encoding of complex spectral information, including pitch (Bendor and Wang, 2005), and ultimately for the identification of sound objects. The dorsal stream was originally defined by its involvement in auditory spatial processing (Rauschecker and Tian, 2000) and movement in space (Warren et al., 2002). This is still believed to be correct (Rauschecker, 2012), but the role of the dorsal stream has been expanded to include sensorimotor integration and control in more general terms (Rauschecker and Scott, 2009; Rauschecker, 2011), including the representation of sequences.

A particularly interesting and important feature of the expanded dorsal stream is that it represents both inverse and forward models (Figure 2B). The forward model is what has classically been referred to as an “efference copy” (von Holst and Mittelstaedt, 1950; Troyer and Doupe, 2000). Whenever premotor cortex neurons fire in preparation of an action, they not only send their message towards the motor cortex for potentially real action, but they also inform sensory systems about the consequences of this action. Conversely, an inverse model (Grush, 2004) instructs the motor system about sensory signals that are relevant for reaching its goals. Both of these signals are compared within the dorsal stream, presumably in parietal cortex, and play a role for optimal state estimation by minimizing the resulting error signal (Rauschecker and Scott, 2009).

The ability of posterior parietal cortex to perform transformations in space may also come to bear in terms of melodic “space”. We can easily recognize a melody when it is played in a different key, that is, when pitch relations between notes are preserved. An imaging study contrasting a transposed melody to the original melody revealed greater activation in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS; Foster and Zatorre, 2010), which points to the role of the IPS in subtracting the effects of the transposition.

Finally, the question arises whether musical melodies, once they are learned, are simply defined by their existence as concatenated sequences in sensorimotor regions of the auditory dorsal stream. The fact that they can be sung or played, imagined and anticipated almost automatically on a given cue seems to demonstrate that this is indeed the case. However, as mentioned in the Introduction section, we can also put a name or a label on a familiar melody, which suggests that there is a second form of existence for music in the brain besides concatenated sounds. The “chunks” formed in rostral prefrontal cortex that become apparent in fMRI studies of highly familiar music, may be the endpoint of the sequencing process in the dorsal stream. At the same time, however, they may also be the starting point of a feedback process (via the inferior frontal cortex) into the ventral auditory pathway, where more information is added, for instance, about the timbre of musical instruments playing a specific tune or about its emotional connotations. This object-identification process would enable a musical melody not just to receive a name, but also to trigger memories of all things past that are associated with that melody.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Achiro, J. M., and Bottjer, S. W. (2013). Neural representation of a target auditory memory in a cortico-basal ganglia pathway. J. Neurosci. 33, 14475–14488. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0710-13.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Adret, P., Meliza, C. D., and Margoliash, D. (2012). Song tutoring in presinging zebra finch juveniles biases a small population of higher-order song-selective neurons toward the tutor song. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 1977–1987. doi: 10.1152/jn.00905.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Badre, D., and D’Esposito, M. (2009). Is the rostro-caudal axis of the frontal lobe hierarchical? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 659–669. doi: 10.1038/nrn2667

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bendor, D., and Wang, X. (2005). The neuronal representation of pitch in primate auditory cortex. Nature 436, 1161–1165. doi: 10.1038/nature03867

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., and Schlesewsky, M. (2013). Reconciling time, space and function: a new dorsal-ventral stream model of sentence comprehension. Brain Lang. 125, 60–76. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.01.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chen, J. L., Penhune, V. B., and Zatorre, R. J. (2008). Moving on time: brain network for auditory-motor synchronization is modulated by rhythm complexity and musical training. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 226–239. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, Y. E., Russ, B. E., Davis, S. J., Baker, A. E., Ackelson, A. L., and Nitecki, R. (2009). A functional role for the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in non-spatial auditory cognition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 106, 20045–20050. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907248106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Comins, J. A., and Gentner, T. Q. (2010). Working memory for patterned sequences of auditory objects in a songbird. Cognition 117, 38–53. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.06.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

DeWitt, I., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2012). Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 109, E505–E514. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1113427109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ebbinghaus, H. (1964). Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology. (New York: Dover) (Original work published as: Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Über das Gedächtnis. Leipzig, Germany: Duncker and Humblot).

Eda-Fujiwara, H., Imagawa, T., Matsushita, M., Matsuda, Y., Takeuchi, H. A., and Satoh, R. (2012). Localized brain activation related to the strength of auditory learning in a parrot. PLoS One 7:e38803. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038803

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Foster, N. E., and Zatorre, R. J. (2010). A role for the intraparietal sulcus in transforming musical pitch information. Cereb. Cortex 20, 1350–1359. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp199

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fuster, J. M., Bodner, M., and Kroger, J. K. (2000). Cross-modal and cross-temporal association in neurons of frontal cortex. Nature 405, 347–351. doi: 10.1038/35012613

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: motor control, imagery and perception. Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 377–396; discussion 396–442. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x04000093

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Henson, R., and Burgess, N. (1997). “Representations of serial order,” in 4th Neural Computation and Psychology Workshop, eds J. A. Bullinaria, D. W. Glasspool and G. Houghton (London: Springer), 283–300.

Hikosaka, O., Sakai, K., Miyauchi, S., Takino, R., Sasaki, Y., and Pütz, B. (1996). Activation of human presupplementary motor area in learning of sequential procedures: a functional MRI study. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 617–621.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J. P., Zatorre, R. J., Griffiths, T. D., Evans, A. C., and Peretz, I. (2007). Cortical thickness in congenital amusia: when less is better than more. J. Neurosci. 27, 13028–13032. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3039-07.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Janata, P., and Grafton, S. T. (2003). Swinging in the brain: shared neural substrates for behaviors related to sequencing and music. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 682–687. doi: 10.1038/nn1081

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kalm, K., and Norris, D. (2014). The representation of order information in auditory-verbal short-term memory. J. Neurosci. 34, 6879–6886. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4104-13.2014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lashley, K. (1951). “The problem of serial order in behavior,” in Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior, ed L. Jeffress (Oxford: Wiley and Sons), 112–136.

Leaver, A. M., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2010). Cortical representation of natural complex sounds: effects of acoustic features and auditory object category. J. Neurosci. 30, 7604–7612. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0296-10.2010.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leaver, A. M., Van Lare, J., Zielinski, B., Halpern, A. R., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2009). Brain activation during anticipation of sound sequences. J. Neurosci. 29, 2477–2485. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4921-08.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Margoliash, D., and Fortune, E. S. (1992). Temporal and harmonic combination-sensitive neurons in the zebra finch’s HVc. J. Neurosci. 12, 4309–4326.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Nicolai, J., Gundacker, C., Teeselink, K., and Güttinger, H. R. (2014). Human melody singing by bullfinches (Pyrrhula pyrrula) gives hints about a cognitive note sequence processing. Anim. Cogn. 17, 143–155. doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0647-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pasupathy, A., and Miller, E. K. (2005). Different time courses of learning-related activity in the prefrontal cortex and striatum. Nature 433, 873–876. doi: 10.1038/nature03287

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, Language and the Brain. New York: Oxford University Press.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Patel, A. D., and Iversen, J. R. (2014). The evolutionary neuroscience of musical beat perception: the Action Simulation for Auditory Prediction (ASAP) hypothesis. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:57. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00057

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Perry, D. W., Zatorre, R. J., Petrides, M., Alivisatos, B., Meyer, E., and Evans, A. C. (1999). Localization of cerebral activity during simple singing. Neuroreport 10, 3979–3984. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199911080-00035

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plakke, B., Diltz, M. D., and Romanski, L. M. (2013). Coding of vocalizations by single neurons in ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Hear. Res. 305, 135–143. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2013.07.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plantinga, J., and Trainor, L. J. (2009). Melody recognition by two-month-old infants. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 125, EL58–EL62. doi: 10.1121/1.3049583

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rauschecker, J. P. (1997). Processing of complex sounds in the auditory cortex of cat, monkey and man. Acta Otolaryngol. Suppl. 532, 34–38. doi: 10.3109/00016489709126142

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rauschecker, J. P. (1998). Cortical processing of complex sounds. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 8, 516–521. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(98)80040-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rauschecker, J. P. (2011). An expanded role for the dorsal auditory pathway in sensorimotor integration and control. Hear. Res. 271, 16–25. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.09.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rauschecker, J. P. (2012). “Processing streams in the auditory cortex,” in Neural Correlates of Auditory Cognition, eds Y. E. Cohen, R. R. Fay and A. N. Popper (New York, Heidelberg: Springer Handbook of Auditory Research), 7–44.

Rauschecker, J. P., and Scott, S. K. (2009). Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: non-human primates illuminate human speech processing. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 718–724. doi: 10.1038/nn.2331

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rauschecker, J. P., and Tian, B. (2000). Mechanisms and streams for processing of “what” and “where” in auditory cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 97, 11800–11806. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.22.11800

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rauschecker, J. P., Tian, B., and Hauser, M. (1995). Processing of complex sounds in the macaque nonprimary auditory cortex. Science 268, 111–114. doi: 10.1126/science.7701330

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roberts, T. F., and Mooney, R. (2013). Motor circuits help encode auditory memories of vocal models used to guide vocal learning. Hear. Res. 303, 48–57. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2013.01.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Romanski, L. M., Tian, B., Fritz, J., Mishkin, M., Goldman-Rakic, P. S., and Rauschecker, J. P. (1999). Dual streams of auditory afferents target multiple domains in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 1131–1136. doi: 10.1038/16056

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sakai, K., Hikosaka, O., Miyauchi, S., Sasaki, Y., Fujimaki, N., and Pütz, B. (1999). Presupplementary motor area activation during sequence learning reflects visuo-motor association. J. Neurosci. 19, RC1.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Sakai, K., and Passingham, R. E. (2003). Prefrontal interactions reflect future task operations. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 75–81. doi: 10.1038/nn987

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schellenberg, E. G., Stalinski, S. M., and Marks, B. M. (2014). Memory for surface features of unfamiliar melodies: independent effects of changes in pitch and tempo. Psychol. Res. 78, 84–95. doi: 10.1007/s00426-013-0483-y

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schindler, A., Herdener, M., and Bartels, A. (2013). Coding of melodic gestalt in human auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2987–2993. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs289

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schubotz, R. I., Sakreida, K., Tittgemeyer, M., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2004). Motor areas beyond motor performance: deficits in serial prediction following ventrolateral premotor lesions. Neuropsychology 18, 638–645. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.4.638

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Terrace, H. S. (2005). The simultaneous chain: a new approach to serial learning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 202–210. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.02.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tian, B., Kusmierek, P., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2013). Analogues of simple and complex cells in the auditory cortex of the rhesus monkey. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 110, 7892–7897. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221062110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tian, B., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2004). Processing of frequency-modulated sounds in the lateral auditory belt cortex of the rhesus monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 2993–3013. doi: 10.1152/jn.00472.2003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tian, B., Reser, D., Durham, A., Kustov, A., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2001). Functional specialization in rhesus monkey auditory cortex. Science 292, 290–293. doi: 10.1126/science.1058911

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tremblay-Champoux, A., Dalla Bella, S., Phillips-Silver, J., Lebrun, M.-A., and Peretz, I. (2010). Singing proficiency in congenital amusia: imitation helps. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 27, 463–476. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2011.567258

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Troyer, T. W., and Doupe, A. J. (2000). An associational model of birdsong sensorimotor learning I. Efference copy and the learning of song syllables. J. Neurophysiol. 84, 1204–1223.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

von Holst, E., and Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenzprinzip: wechselwirkungen zwischen zentralnervensystem und peripherie. Naturwissenschaften 37, 464–476. doi: 10.1007/bf00622503

CrossRef Full Text

Warren, J. D., Zielinski, B. A., Green, G. G. R., Rauschecker, J. P., and Griffiths, T. D. (2002). Analysis of sound source motion by the human brain. Neuron 34, 1–20.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Yin, H. H. (2010). The sensorimotor striatum is necessary for serial order learning. J. Neurosci. 30, 14719–14723. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3989-10.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zatorre, R. J., Bouffard, M., and Belin, P. (2004). Sensitivity to auditory object features in human temporal neocortex. J. Neurosci. 24, 3637–3642. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5458-03.2004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: auditory dorsal stream, premotor cortex, basal ganglia, temporal combination sensitivity, sound sequence, auditory object, auditory ventral stream, prefrontal cortex

Citation: Rauschecker JP (2014) Is there a tape recorder in your head? How the brain stores and retrieves musical melodies. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:149. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00149

Received: 26 October 2013; Accepted: 04 August 2014;
Published online: 28 August 2014.

Edited by:

Jonathan B. Fritz, University of Maryland, USA

Reviewed by:

Preston E. Garraghty, Indiana University, USA
David J. Freedman, University of Chicago, USA
Jonathan B. Fritz, University of Maryland, USA

Copyright © 2014 Rauschecker. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Josef P. Rauschecker, Department of Neuroscience, Georgetown University Medical Center, 3970 Reservoir Road, NW, Rm. WP19 NRB, Washington, DC 20007, USA e-mail: rauschej@georgetown.edu

Download