Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Syst. Neurosci., 22 August 2016
Volume 10 - 2016 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2016.00069

Putting the “Biology” Back into “Neurobiology”: The Strength of Diversity in Animal Model Systems for Neuroscience Research

  • 1Neuroscience Group, Division of Basic Biomedical Sciences, Sanford School of Medicine, University of South Dakota, Vermillion, SD, USA
  • 2Department of Biology, University of South Dakota, Vermillion, SD, USA

Current trends in neuroscience research have moved toward a reliance on rodent animal models to study most aspects of brain function. Such laboratory-reared animals are highly inbred, have been disengaged from their natural environments for generations and appear to be of limited predictive value for successful clinical outcomes. In this Perspective article, we argue that research on a rich diversity of animal model systems is fundamental to new discoveries in evolutionarily conserved core physiological and molecular mechanisms that are the foundation of human brain function. Analysis of neural circuits across phyla will reveal general computational solutions that form the basis for adaptive behavioral responses. Further, we stress that development of ethoexperimental approaches to improve our understanding of behavioral nuance will help to realign our research strategies with therapeutic goals and improve the translational validity of specific animal models. Finally, we suggest that neuroscience has a role in environmental conservation of habitat and fauna that will preserve and protect the ecological settings that drive species-specific behavioral adaptations. A rich biodiversity will enhance our understanding of human brain function and lead in unpredicted directions for development of therapeutic treatments for neurological disorders.

Introduction

Successful adaptation of animal behavior to environmental demands is reflected in the structure and function of nervous systems. Over the years of neuroscience research, we have come to realize the diversity of adaptive neural solutions that organisms have evolved to overcome environmental challenges and survive. In the burgeoning days of neuroscience research in the 1960’s and 70’s, then called “neurobiology”, researchers used a variety of vertebrate and invertebrate model systems to study the function of neurons and neuronal circuits. The fact that different animal models had particular strengths for addressing specific questions about nervous system function was appreciated. For example, the squid giant axon was selected by Hodgkin and Huxley (1952) to study ionic mechanisms underlying the action potential. The now classic frog neuromuscular junction preparation was developed to understand mechanisms of synaptic transmission (Del Castillo and Katz, 1954). Legions of neuroscientists exploited the “simpler” nervous systems of invertebrates with their identified neurons to define the synaptic and integrative properties of neural circuits controlling motor behavior and learning (Marder, 2012). Studies of these less complex model systems provided fundamental insights into the function of the more derived nervous systems of mammals including the human brain. More recently, there has been substantial reliance of the use of rodent models (mouse, rat) to study all aspects of brain structure and function in modern neuroscience research. In today’s neuroscience environment there is a lack of appreciation of, or reluctance to accept, less popular or atypical animal models. In this Perspective article, we argue that neuroscience research utilizing a rich diversity of animal model systems rather than just a few established ones significantly enhances our ability to discover fundamental molecular and physiological principles essential for nervous system function. Elucidation of highly conserved neuronal mechanisms is widely applicable to understanding normal human brain function and greatly facilitates understanding of neurological dysfunction and disease states.

Animal Model Systems in Neuroscience Research

Neuroscience research is currently dominated by studies of the rat and the mouse brain. This has been the trend for the last few decades. A search for “rodent, nervous system” from PubMed1 shows that approximately 35–40% of all research efforts are directed to those species (Figure 1). In 1970, 27% of published articles on the nervous system used rodent animal models and rose to 41% in the 1990s. In 2015, published work using rodents comprised 32% of all neuroscience articles. Interestingly, utilization of the mouse compared to that of the rat has surged in the last decade and a half due largely to development of powerful transgenic techniques. However, while laboratory mice and rats are housed with standard facility designs and care protocols reducing costs for the universities, they are also highly inbred and have been disengaged from natural environments for many generations thereby lacking genetic and behavioral diversity. Current research on other model systems such as Aplysia, Drosophila, zebrafish, reptiles and birds each comprise less than 1% of the total number of published articles. This reliance on rodent models for neuroscience research has led some to conclude that this trend severely limits the scope of our overall understanding of brain structure and function, especially in the context of evolution of the human brain (Preuss, 2000; Manger et al., 2008).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. History of the percentage of neuroscience articles published from work using rodent model systems. PubMed search terms “rodent, nervous system” were used to derive the percentage of publications from the total number of articles published using “nervous system” between the years 1960–2015. For many decades, about 35–40% of all neuroscience studies have relied on rodent animal models.

Understanding the function of the human brain and dysfunction in disease states is a primary goal of neuroscience research. However, we are restricted in the questions that can be asked when studying humans. Specific animals are selected for study because they are particularly suitable for addressing basic questions about brain function, but the research also has inherent value in conservation and management strategies for that species. While mice and rats are well characterized and bred for research, they are not always the best models for all areas of neuroscience investigation. For example, song learning in juvenile songbirds closely mimics human speech learning (Mello and Clayton, 2015). The neurobiology of birdsong has developed into a rich field of study that tells us much about auditory perception, learning and memory and cognition. More realistic hypotheses for understanding neural substrates underlying long-range spatial navigation in natural environments have also emerged from studies of foraging bats (Geva-Sagiv et al., 2015). An interesting result from this work is that the spatial two-dimensional coding representation observed in place cells and grid cells in the hippocampus in studies of rodents in small laboratory cages misses the more complex three-dimensional multiscale representation required for real-world navigation over long distances. Another important result from the same laboratory (Yartsev et al., 2011) also emphasizes species differences by showing that grid cells are formed in the entorhinal cortex of bats in the absence of theta oscillations, which are ubiquitous in rodent entorhinal cortex, and were previously believed to be required for the formation of grid cells. These results derived from bats argue against a major class of computational models of grid cells based on theta oscillations which may be the result of rodent whisking rhythms (Grion et al., 2016; Kleinfeld et al., 2016) rather than a universal property of hippocampal activity in all species. Finally, studies using the turtle have lead to elucidation of detailed cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying vertebrate associative learning (Ambigapathy et al., 2015; Keifer and Zheng, 2015). It is because of the remarkable ability of the turtle nervous system to withstand hypoxic conditions (since they are a diving species) that large portions of the central nervous system can be removed and studied in a dish for prolonged periods of time. This allows cellular analysis of the function of large portions of intact neural circuits as well as manipulation by pharmacological agents during a neural correlate of classical conditioning. Such in vitro experiments are not possible in mammalian species except in slices. While the neuroanatomical features of such animal models have undergone diversification, the physiological and behavioral species-specific specializations across the animal kingdom is what makes some animal models more attractive for specific areas of study than others.

Recognition of Conserved Core Mechanisms for Gene Regulation

With the emergence of rapidly developing next generation sequencing technology and bioinformatics tools, numerous species (if not all) and particularly atypical model organisms for neuroscience research have become amenable to mechanistic investigations of gene function, expression and regulation in the brain. High throughput sequencing of genomes from numerous representative species is currently underway and data are now freely available in online databases such as GenBank. Emerging evidence indicates that tissue-specific core gene expression is highly conserved across vertebrates. When different tissues are compared across fish, frog, chicken, mouse and human (Chan et al., 2009), one-third of all expressed genes are found to be orthologous (similar genes found in different species, which while not identical, can be traced back to a common ancestral species). Importantly, molecular mechanisms underlying gene expression in brain share common features throughout animal species even though with increased size of genome complexity in mammals and human gene regulation mechanisms are correspondingly expanded. This is particularly true for the transcriptional machinery as shown by genome-wide data across phyla (Hahn, 2004; Adelman and Lis, 2012; Koster et al., 2015). Transcription factors are critical regulators of gene expression and many of these are highly conserved in structure and signaling function even though there is divergence in binding sites for specific transcription factors (Villar et al., 2014). The cAMP-response element-binding protein (CREB), for example, is an evolutionarily conserved transcription factor that is considered to be a master regulator of cellular responses to environmental stimuli as well as learning and memory (Lakhina et al., 2015). This regulator, like other so called “memory genes” (Harvey-Girard et al., 2012; Rittschof et al., 2014; Mello and Clayton, 2015), is essential for learning processes of organisms as diverse as Aplysia, worm, honey bee, bird, turtle, mouse and human. Learning-related epigenetic modifications in chromatin surrounding DNA are also shared across a variety of invertebrate and vertebrate model organisms (Levenson and Sweatt, 2006; Xiao et al., 2012; Ambigapathy et al., 2015). A recent study showed that even complex social behavioral responses to aggression during territory intrusion share common molecular mechanisms in terms of chromosome reorganization, transcription factor activation and neuroendocrine signaling across the honey bee, stickleback fish and mouse (Rittschof et al., 2014). Taken together, such findings have led to the emerging conclusion that core molecular components are major regulators of learning and memory, social behavior, and responses to stress that are evolutionarily conserved with expansion related primarily to species-specific behavioral adaptations. An understanding of basic core mechanisms will reduce the complexity of analysis of a bewildering array of proteins and regulators involved in specific processes, for example, transcription. Hence, future investigations of the molecular basis of specific behaviors among a diversity of species and model animal systems will give us a powerful means to recognize and detail the conserved core mechanisms that are fundamental to neuronal function and essential to human brain function.

Deep Homology in Neural Networks

In addition to mechanisms underlying transcriptional regulation and gene expression, neural circuits are evolutionarily conserved in basic structural design but uniquely elaborated according to species-specific specializations (Preuss, 1995). An example is the common design of neural circuitry for motion detection in the fly and the mouse (Borst and Helmstaedter, 2015). The detection of motion in the visual scene is one of the earliest and most fundamental computational processing stages in vision. In both species, signals from the photoreceptors are divided into ON and OFF pathways and relayed to deeper levels in the CNS where cells become directionally sensitive. Once direction is established, this information is fused at the next synapse in both organisms to form a motion signal in one of the four primary directions. The organizational features and flow of information through different processing layers is schematically very similar in design between the fly and mouse. This is an indication of the robustness of this particular computational solution to the problem of motion detection and establishes the fly as a powerful model for studies of visual motion analysis. Another instructive example of common circuit design for a specific behavioral action is the remarkable homology of the arthropod central complex and vertebrate basal ganglia (Strausfeld and Hirth, 2013). Here, it has been proposed that the organization of the insect protocerebra forms excitatory and inhibitory pathways subject to modulation by dopaminergic inputs that are homologous to the mammalian basal ganglia in structure and in motor function. The authors concluded that adaptive motor program selection is a trait common across phyla and generated by homologous neural circuits. Many conserved features of brain organization are observed throughout amniotes. For example, topographic analysis of thalamocortical projections using tract tracing in turtles showed a medial to lateral specificity of connections (Zhu et al., 2005). This finding indicates the presence of a functionally segregated pattern of thalamocortical projections that is a conserved feature of brain organization among amniotes. Moreover, the avian brain shares similarities in the neural control of learning and memory (Mello and Clayton, 2015) as well as in features of cognition (Clayton and Emery, 2015) compared to that of humans. Studies reveal that songbird learning shows remarkable similarity to human speech learning. Across all vertebrates, behaviorally significant brain regions and connections are maintained over 450 million years of evolution (O’Connell and Hofmann, 2011, 2012). This extraordinary conservation of function yields common homologous neural circuits controlling fundamental processes such as reward and social decision-making. The result is that complex social processes are represented in neural pathways of non-mammalian vertebrates (Korzan and Summers, 2007; Oliveira, 2013). However, brain structures unique to individual species have also emerged. Such is the case made for the prefrontal cortex of primates (Preuss, 1995; Wise, 2008). While homologous regions in the frontal lobe are shared by rats and primates, only primates have evolved a “granular” prefrontal cortex that is the essence of the behavioral flexibility displayed by them. However, it is also true that behavioral flexibility is also displayed by animals that do not have a granular prefrontal cortex or even a neocortex, for example, turtles (Grisham and Powers, 1989) and fish (Carpenter and Summers, 2009). Diversity informs us by revealing common computational solutions for behavior that span species. Continued analysis of homologous neural networks controlling behavior as an analogy for human brain function will reveal general computational solutions (Striedter et al., 2014) that form the basis for adaptive behavioral responses.

Toward Understanding Behavioral Nuance

Development of therapies and drugs for neural disorders has driven scientific methodologies increasingly toward rapid high-volume results and a reduction in the number and kind of animal models used. However, the methods by which we approach behavioral experiments should be carefully attuned to the ecological profile of the animal being examined (Blanchard et al., 2001, 2013; Robertson et al., 2015). An ethoexperimental approach, such as that which can be found in the Blanchards’ Visible Burrow System and BTBR mouse facial expression/nose-nose interaction tests for autistic behavior, as well as the Stress Alternatives Models from the Summers’ lab, captures the dynamism of a natural environment and mechanisms that are specific to highly nuanced behaviors (Pearson et al., 2016). The success of ecologically and ethologically relevant models for elucidating neuropsychiatric and neurological disorders underscores the need for further development of new models that incorporate similar dynamics (Blanchard and Blanchard, 1989; Carpenter and Summers, 2009; Nestler and Hyman, 2010; Beery and Kaufer, 2015; Robertson et al., 2015; Smith et al., 2016). Therefore, we suggest that this approach provides the potential to address diverse neurological and psychiatric disorders by making use of the distinctive natural behavioral repertoire of any species that has elements of behavior and physiology that are driven by neural circuits involved in human disorders. The critical point here is not to simply try to match behavioral face validity from animal model to human translation, but to use natural behaviors belonging to the animal model and match neuroregulatory systems to the human condition (Young et al., 2012; Blanchard et al., 2013). As we continue to investigate the neural mechanisms involved in producing clinically relevant disorders, how we design model systems and behavioral paradigms, specifically by using an ethoexperimental approach to explore translational interactions of physiology, genetics, and environment on behavioral adaptations, will continue to inspire more realistic experimental approaches.

Complexity is always far more prevalent in human and animal behavior than we initially imagine. Derived from genetic background and evolutionary history, environmental stimuli, gene-environment interactions, and internal physiology, complexity is necessary to establish adaptive significance for the individual behavior (Tinbergen, 1963). Recent trends indicate that the number of animal models considered for behavioral studies are extremely limited and are paired with experimental protocols that depend on extremely simplified stimuli (painful or novel) and outcomes to produce high throughput designs for assessing translational relationships with human disorders, despite the lack of obvious complexity in task design (Blanchard and Blanchard, 1989). Examination of recent studies suggests they are of limited clinical validity (Nestler and Hyman, 2010; Haller and Alicki, 2012; Haller and Freund, 2013; Haller et al., 2013). Context and small discrepancies in behavior (nuance) often make distinctive differences in neural activity and the adaptive value of the response for the individual (Blanchard and Blanchard, 1969a,b; Blanchard et al., 1995, 2001, 2011, 2013; Defensor et al., 2011; 2012). We agree with an early statement by Lorenz (1958), that to understand the underlying mechanisms involved, the full range of behavior in contextually appropriate naturalistic settings is necessary. A pertinent example stems from an area highly investigated, sexual behavior in rats, typically using spare, but adequate, social and environmental conditions. However, when more natural and ethological conditions are applied, the results reveal additional highly nuanced behavior in both male and female rats, resulting in differential female receptivity as well as female and male fertility, in which the social and physical environment provides significant information about sexual interactions (Chu and Ågmo, 2015a,b; Chu et al., 2015). Therefore, behavioral paradigms and experimental designs created to study the neural and molecular regulators of distinguishing behaviors should come from critical analyses of nuance (Robertson et al., 2015). Parsing this continuum of behavioral nuance is no small task, as the experimenter must tie the complex and poorly understood symptomatology in humans to observable behaviors in animals. The key is to examine natural behaviors, using extant species in or related to their natural environments, where evolutionary adaptions make the behavioral outcome relevant to the animal and only secondarily related to the human malady under consideration (Blanchard et al., 2013). After relating animal behavior to human symptoms (face validity), effective models ought to also make use of therapies producing parallel results in animals and people (predictive validity), and relate the symptoms and treatments to the specific neurocircuitry and physiology involved (construct validity). For depression and anxiety, one of the hardest standards to meet is construct validity because the complex biological and behavioral substrates are only poorly understood. Often incongruous results stem from studies which seek to explain a single aspect of broadly multifaceted phenomena. Contemporary trials in clinical populations suggest that the translative power of most single niche tests for psychological disorders in animal models is low (Haller and Alicki, 2012; Haller and Freund, 2013; Haller et al., 2013). An innovative emphasis aimed at where the diversity of behavioral nuance between disorders and models converge may become the crucial element in assessing animal model validity (Blanchard et al., 2013).

Alignment of Research Strategies with Therapeutic Goals

Recent meta-analyses of standard animal models for human psychiatric disorders provide clear evidence that customary laboratory-reared rodents do not produce outcomes that consistently translate well to effective clinical trials, much less to successful therapies (Holsboer and Ising, 2010; Haller and Alicki, 2012; Blanchard et al., 2013; Bukalo et al., 2014). These analyses have played a fundamental role in the realignment of research strategies by national funding agencies toward even more anthropocentric approaches and policies. Basic research and serendipity have always played critical roles in discovering new therapeutic approaches and tools. Alternative animal models have long been a part of the mix of basic research approaches that have yielded directed and serendipitous advances. Recent evidence suggests that classification of psychoses into distinctive groups based on biological markers have a better diagnostic capacity than the symptoms presented by patients (Clementz et al., 2016). The biological markers that were meant to produce a more unbiased classification of the psychoses have been developed, at least in part, using non-rodent or non-human animal models (Lindsley et al., 1950; Garcia-Austt, 1954; Gusel’nikov, 1956; Fite et al., 1979). The most important idea is that the more biological markers that can be developed, the more efficient our diagnostic capacity may become for a variety of maladies including psychological disorders. The greatest opportunity for discovering the necessary molecular or neurochemical markers is to include the greatest variety of animals in our research. In a rational approach to increase biological markers through animal models, consider that most rodent models are based on highly inbred rats and mice. These inbred strains do not reflect the full range of behavioral responses of natural populations and therefore almost certainly do not demonstrate the full range of physiological or molecular responses of wild animals (Sgoifo et al., 1996; de Boer et al., 2003; Coppens et al., 2014). It is likely that one reason that models derived from laboratory-raised animals are not well translated into clinical successes is that the human condition is far closer to that of wild animals than to those of inbred rodents that are bred, raised and held under conditions that can only be described as highly deprived with respect to sensory input and behavioral opportunities (Van de Weerd et al., 1997; Kavelaars et al., 1999; van den Berg et al., 1999; de Boer et al., 2003; Meeusen, 2005; Korte et al., 2007; Balcombe, 2010; Blanchard, 2010; MacGillivray et al., 2012; König et al., 2015; Smith et al., 2016).

A Broader Appreciation of Psychiatric Disorders

About three decades ago, significant work in non-mammalian models began to link social stress, neural and endocrine systems with acute and chronic emotional behavior (Greenberg et al., 1979, 1984a,b; Greenberg and Crews, 1990). The work led to evidence that linked specific stress-induced monoamine neurochemistry and hormonal changes to behavioral phenotypes and outcomes associated with social rank relationships (Summers and Greenberg, 1994, 1995; Summers et al., 1997). What followed were numerous studies that demonstrated similar changes in non-mammalian nervous and endocrine systems following social defeat paradigms, as are seen in human cases of depression (Summers et al., 1998, 2003a). It also became clear that behavioral phenotypes centered on stress coping strategies were highly conserved across vertebrate taxa, including humans, and they represented a clear mechanistic link between behavioral responses and affective states (Koolhaas et al., 1997, 1999, 2007; Pottinger and Carrick, 1999; Øverli et al., 2004a,b, 2007). A seminal Society for Integrative and Comparative Biology symposium in 2001 analyzed standard and comparative animal models to ask the question: Is Stress More than a Disease? (Carr and Summers, 2002). Several important conceptual advances were revealed suggesting directions for the focus of future stress-related study including psychological disorders: (1) Examining the comparative endocrinology and evolution of stress helps to identify common themes of neuroendocrine integration and control; (2) Comparing widely used mammalian models with other vertebrates in laboratory or field settings facilitates recognition of adaptations of behavior, reproductive strategy, and life history; (3) Non-mammalian species are sometimes better models for unraveling complex neuroendocrine control mechanisms; and (4) Examining natural populations of animals provides insight into adaptive features of stress not previously observed when studying laboratory mammals. It is commonly accepted now that chronic stress is an etiological precursor to anxiety and depression, but prior to the development of the Social Defeat model of depression, results from several studies on lizards and fish made it clear that human affective behavioral inhibition occurring concomitantly with anxiety and depression is not unique (Øverli et al., 2004a,b, 2007; Summers et al., 2005a; Korzan et al., 2006; Korzan and Summers, 2007). Even more important is that the neurochemical, neuroendocrine and molecular changes associated with human depression and anxiety occur in the same brain regions of fish and lizards following social subordination stress (Summers and Greenberg, 1994; Summers et al., 1997, 1998, 2003a,b, 2005a,b,c; Summers, 2001; Summers and Winberg, 2006; Korzan and Summers, 2007; Robertson et al., 2015). Similarly, recent work on Rainbow trout and Atlantic salmon suggests that for individuals within a population, a suite of symptoms occur that are virtually identical to those of depressed human patients. This depressive syndrome is characterized by chronic brain serotonin (5-HT) dysfunction, increased cortisol production and behavioral inhibition (Riise et al., 2015; Vindas et al., 2016). This depressive state was demonstrated to facilitate tolerating risky social environments. Development of the Social Defeat model of depression in rodents followed the work in lizards and mirror the results seen in fish in which individuals susceptible to chronic social stress display depressive behavioral inhibition in a number of classical tests of anxiety and depression whereas resilient individuals do not (Berton et al., 2007; Krishnan et al., 2007; Lutter et al., 2008). The results from these experiments suggest that depressive and anxious affective states evolved long ago to minimize unpredictable stress exposure in vulnerable individuals and have been conserved across vertebrate phyla. Taken together, the comprehensive scope of results from widely varying vertebrate systems suggests that it is no longer appropriate to reserve the descriptive terms “depression” and “anxiety” for human patients alone. What is more, it is apparent that an understanding of the behavioral inhibition seen in comparative animal models, in combination with the important interpretation of reduced behavioral responses being ecologically and ethologically adaptive, may help advance our understanding of these previously considered human-only psychiatric disorders.

Neuroscience has a Role in Environmental Conservation

Animals respond to their environments through sensory and motor systems combined with integrative neuroplasticity, all uniquely evolved to produce adaptive behavioral outcomes. Environmental conservation of specific habitats and niches preserves and protects the specific behavioral adaptations and underlying neural substrates of the fauna endemic to those habitats. Therefore, one of the great strengths of comparative neuroscience is that evolutionarily conserved neural systems are linked fundamentally to adaptive behavioral responses in identified ecological and ethological settings. As each animal is uniquely suited to a specific environment, there is much to be learned from analyzing the neural function of wild animals while taking into consideration the environments that provided the evolutionary grist to result in the specialized nervous systems unique to each species (Manger, 2011). As humanity has benefited greatly from the discovery of natural compounds effective in treatments for diseases such as cancer, a rich biodiversity in species and habitat is essential in understanding brain function and in developing novel therapeutic advances for neurological disorders.

Author Contributions

JK and CHS wrote the article.

Funding

CHS was supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant MH104485 for his work on mice. This publication was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R15MH104485. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health or the United States Government.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov

References

Adelman, K., and Lis, J. T. (2012). Promoter-proximal pausing of RNA polymerase II: emerging roles in metazoans. Nat. Rev. Genet. 13, 720–731. doi: 10.1038/nrg3293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ambigapathy, G., Zheng, Z., and Keifer, J. (2015). Regulation of BDNF chromatin status and promoter accessibility in a neural correlate of associative learning. Epigenetics 10, 981–993. doi: 10.1080/15592294.2015.1090072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balcombe, J. (2010). Laboratory rodent welfare: thinking outside the cage. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 13, 77–88. doi: 10.1080/10888700903372168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beery, A. K., and Kaufer, D. (2015). Stress, social behavior and resilience: insights from rodents. Neurobiol. Stress 1, 116–127. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2014.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berton, O., Covington, H. E. III, Ebner, K., Tsankova, N. M., Carle, T. L., Ulery, P., et al. (2007). Induction of δFosB in the periaqueductal gray by stress promotes active coping responses. Neuron 55, 289–300. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, R. J. (2010). Animal welfare beyond the cage … and beyond the evidence? J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 13, 89–95. doi: 10.1080/10888700903372184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, R. J., and Blanchard, D. C. (1969a). Crouching as an index of fear. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 67, 370–375. doi: 10.1037/h0026779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, R. J., and Blanchard, D. C. (1969b). Passive and active reactions to fear-eliciting stimuli. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 68, 129–135. doi: 10.1037/h0027676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, R. J., and Blanchard, D. C. (1989). Antipredator defensive behaviors in a visible burrow system. J. Comp. Psychol. 103, 70–82. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.103.1.70

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, D. C., Griebel, G., and Blanchard, R. J. (2001). Mouse defensive behaviors: pharmacological and behavioral assays for anxiety and panic. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 205–218. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00009-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, D. C., Griebel, G., Pobbe, R., and Blanchard, R. J. (2011). Risk assessment as an evolved threat detection and analysis process. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 991–998. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, D. C., Spencer, R. L., Weiss, S. M., Blanchard, R. J., McEwen, B., and Sakai, R. R. (1995). Visible burrow system as a model of chronic social stress: behavioral and neuroendocrine correlates. Psychoneuroendocrinology 20, 117–134. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(94)e0045-b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanchard, D. C., Summers, C. H., and Blanchard, R. J. (2013). The role of behavior in translational models for psychopathology: functionality and dysfunctional behaviors. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 1567–1577. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borst, A., and Helmstaedter, M. (2015). Common circuit design in fly and mammalian motion vision. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1067–1076. doi: 10.1038/nn.4050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bukalo, O., Pinard, C. R., and Holmes, A. (2014). Mechanisms to medicines: elucidating neural and molecular substrates of fear extinction to identify novel treatments for anxiety disorders. Br. J. Pharmacol. 171, 4690–4718. doi: 10.1111/bph.12779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carpenter, R. E., and Summers, C. H. (2009). Learning strategies during fear conditioning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 91, 415–423. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, J. A., and Summers, C. H. (2002). Is stress more than a disease? A comparative look at the adaptativeness of stress. Integr. Comp. Biol. 42, 505–507. doi: 10.1093/icb/42.3.505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, E. T., Quon, G. T., Chua, G., Babak, T., Trochesset, M., Zirngibl, R. A., et al. (2009). Conservation of core gene expression in vertebrate tissues. J. Biol. 8:33. doi: 10.1186/jbiol130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chu, X., and Ågmo, A. (2015a). Sociosexual behaviors during the transition from non-receptivity to receptivity in rats housed in a seminatural environment. Behav. Processes 113, 24–34. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2015.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chu, X., and Ågmo, A. (2015b). Sociosexual behaviors of male rats (Rattus norvegicus) in a seminatural environment. J. Comp. Psychol. 129, 132–144. doi: 10.1037/a0038722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chu, X., Guarraci, F. A., and Ågmo, A. (2015). Sociosexual behaviors and reproductive success of rats (Rattus norvegicus) in a seminatural environment. Physiol. Behav. 151, 46–54. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, N. S., and Emery, N. J. (2015). Avian models for human cognitive neuroscience: a proposal. Neuron 86, 1330–1342. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.04.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clementz, B. A., Sweeney, J. A., Hamm, J. P., Ivleva, E. I., Ethridge, L. E., Pearlson, G. D., et al. (2016). Identification of distinct psychosis biotypes using brain-based biomarkers. Am. J. Psychiatry 173, 373–384. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14091200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coppens, C. M., de Boer, S. F., Buwalda, B., and Koolhaas, J. M. (2014). Aggression and aspects of impulsivity in wild-type rats. Aggress. Behav. 40, 300–308. doi: 10.1002/ab.21527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Boer, S. F., van der Vegt, B. J., and Koolhaas, J. M. (2003). Individual variation in aggression of feral rodent strains: a standard for the genetics of aggression and violence? Behav. Genet. 33, 485–501. doi: 10.1023/A:1025766415159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Defensor, E. B., Corley, M. J., Blanchard, R. J., and Blanchard, D. C. (2012). Facial expressions of mice in aggressive and fearful contexts. Physiol. Behav. 107, 680–685. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Defensor, E. B., Pearson, B. L., Probbe, R. L., Bolivar, V. J., Blanchard, D. C., and Blanchard, R. J. (2011). A novel social proximity test suggests patterns of social avoidance and gaze aversion-like behavior in BTBR T+ tf/J mice. Behav. Brain Res. 217, 302–308. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.10.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Castillo, J., and Katz, B. (1954). Quantal components of the end-plate potential. J. Physiol. 124, 560–573. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1954.sp005129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fite, K. V., Reiner, A., and Hunt, S. P. (1979). Optokinetic nystagmus and the accessory optic system of pigeon and turtle. Brain Behav. Evol. 16, 192–202. doi: 10.1159/000121836

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia-Austt, E. Jr. (1954). Development of electrical activity in cerebral hemispheres of the chick embryo. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 86, 348–352. doi: 10.3181/00379727-86-21095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geva-Sagiv, M., Las, L., Yovel, Y., and Ulanovsky, N. (2015). Spatial cognition in bats and rats: from sensory acquisition to multiscale maps and navigation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 94–108. doi: 10.1038/nrn3888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, N., Chen, T., and Crews, D. (1984a). Social status, gonadal state and the adrenal stress response in the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Horm. Behav. 18, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/0018-506x(84)90045-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, N., Scott, M., and Crews, D. (1984b). Role of the amygdala in the reproductive and aggressive behavior of the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Physiol. Behav. 32, 147–151. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(84)90088-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, N., and Crews, D. (1990). Endocrine and behavioral responses to aggression and social dominance in the green anole lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 77, 246–255. doi: 10.1016/0016-6480(90)90309-a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, N., MacLean, P. D., and Ferguson, J. L. (1979). Role of the paleostriatum in species-typical display behavior of the lizard (Anolis carolinensis). Brain Res. 172, 229–241. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90535-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grion, N., Akrami, A., Zuo, Y., Stella, F., and Diamond, M. E. (2016). Coherence between rat sensorimotor system and hippocampus is enhanced during tactile discrimination. PLoS Biol. 14:e1002384. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grisham, W., and Powers, A. S. (1989). Function of the dorsal and medial cortex of turtles in learning. Behav. Neurosci. 103, 991–997. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.103.5.991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gusel’nikov, V. I. (1956). Electrophysiological characteristics of some sections of the cerebrum in a turtle. Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. Deiat. Im I. P. Pavlova 6, 898–904.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hahn, S. (2004). Structure and mechanism of the RNA polymerase II transcription machinery. Nat. Struct. Mol. Biol. 11, 394–403. doi: 10.1038/nsmb763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haller, J., and Alicki, M. (2012). Current animal models of anxiety, anxiety disorders and anxiolytic drugs. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 25, 59–64. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e32834de34f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haller, J., Aliczki, M., and Gyimesine Pelczer, K. (2013). Classical and novel approaches to the preclinical testing of anxiolytics: a critical evaluation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2318–2330. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haller, J., and Freund, T. F. (2013). Discovery research in neuropsychiatry–anxiety, depression and schizophrenia in focus. Editorial. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37:2311. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey-Girard, E., Giassi, A. C., Ellis, W., and Maler, L. (2012). Organization of the gymnotiform fish pallium in relation to learning and memory: IV. Expression of conserved transcription factors and implications for the evolution of dorsal telencephalon. J. Comp. Neurol. 520, 3395–3413. doi: 10.1002/cne.23107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodgkin, A. L., and Huxley, A. F. (1952). A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve. J. Physiol. 117, 500–544. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1952.sp004764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holsboer, F., and Ising, M. (2010). Stress hormone regulation: biological role and translation into therapy. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 61, 81–109, C1–C11. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavelaars, A., Heijnen, C. J., Tennekes, R., Bruggink, J. E., and Koolhaas, J. M. (1999). Individual behavioral characteristics of wild-type rats predict susceptibility to experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis. Brain Behav. Immun. 13, 279–286. doi: 10.1006/brbi.1998.0534

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keifer, J., and Zheng, Z. (2015). Coincidence detection in a neural correlate of classical conditioning is initiated by bidirectional 3-phosphoinositide-dependent kinase-1 signalling and modulated by adenosine receptors. J. Physiol. 593, 1581–1595. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2014.282947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinfeld, D., Deschenes, M., and Ulanovsky, N. (2016). Whisking, sniffing and the hippocampal θ-rhythm: a tale of two oscillators. PLoS Biol. 14:e1002385. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

König, B., Lindholm, A. K., Lopes, P. C., Dobay, A., Steinert, S., and Buschmann, F. J.-U. (2015). A system for automatic recording of social behavior in a free-living wild house mouse population. Anim. Biotelem. 3, 1–15. doi: 10.1186/s40317-015-0069-0

CrossRef Full Text

Koolhaas, J. M., de Boer, S. F., Buwalda, B., and van Reenen, K. (2007). Individual variation in coping with stress: a multidimensional approach of ultimate and proximate mechanisms. Brain Behav. Evol. 70, 218–226. doi: 10.1159/000105485

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koolhaas, J. M., De Boer, S. F., De Rutter, A. J., Meerlo, P., and Sgoifo, A. (1997). Social stress in rats and mice. Acta Physiol. Scand. Suppl. 640, 69–72.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Koolhaas, J. M., Korte, S. M., De Boer, S. F., Van Der Vegt, B. J., Van Reenen, C. G., Hopster, H., et al. (1999). Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 23, 925–935. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00026-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korte, S. M., Olivier, B., and Koolhaas, J. M. (2007). A new animal welfare concept based on allostasis. Physiol. Behav. 92, 422–428. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korzan, W. J., Overli, O., and Summers, C. H. (2006). Future social rank: forecasting status in the green anole (Anolis carolinensis). Acta Ethol. 9, 48–57. doi: 10.1007/s10211-006-0015-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korzan, W. J., and Summers, C. H. (2007). Behavioral diversity and neurochemical plasticity: selection of stress coping strategies that define social status. Brain Behav. Evol. 70, 257–266. doi: 10.1159/000105489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koster, M. J. E., Snel, B., and Timmers, H. T. (2015). Genesis of chromatin and transcription dynamics in the origin of species. Cell 161, 724–736. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.04.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krishnan, V., Han, M. H., Graham, D. L., Berton, O., Renthal, W., Russo, S. J., et al. (2007). Molecular adaptations underlying susceptibility and resistance to social defeat in brain reward regions. Cell 131, 391–404. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakhina, V., Arey, R. N., Kaletsky, R., Kauffman, A., Stein, G., Keyes, W., et al. (2015). Genome-wide functional analysis of CREB/long-term memory-dependent transcription reveals distinct basal and memory gene expression programs. Neuron 85, 330–345. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levenson, J. M., and Sweatt, J. D. (2006). Epigenetic mechanisms: a common theme in vertebrate and invertebrate memory formation. Cell. Mol. Life Sci. 63, 1009–1016. doi: 10.1007/s00018-006-6026-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindsley, D. B., Schreiner, L. H., Knowles, W. B., and Magoun, H. W. (1950). Behavioral and EEG changes following chronic brain stem lesions in the cat. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2, 483–498. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(50)90086-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenz, K. Z. (1958). The evolution of behavior. Sci. Am. 199, 67–74. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican1258-67

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutter, M., Krishnan, V., Russo, S. J., Jung, S., McClung, C. A., and Nestler, E. J. (2008). Orexin signaling mediates the antidepressant-like effect of calorie restriction. J. Neurosci. 28, 3071–3075. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5584-07.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacGillivray, L., Reynolds, K. B., Rosebush, P. I., and Mazurek, M. F. (2012). The comparative effects of environmental enrichment with exercise and serotonin transporter blockade on serotonergic neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus. Synapse 66, 465–470. doi: 10.1002/syn.21511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manger, P. R. (2011). Collectibles and collections for comparative and evolutionary neurobiological research in Africa. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1225, (Suppl. 1) E85–E93. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05948.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Manger, P. R., Cort, J., Ebrahim, N., Goodman, A., Henning, J., Karolia, M., et al. (2008). Is 21st century neuroscience too focused on the rat/mouse model of brain function and dysfunction? Front. Neuroanat. 2:5. doi: 10.3389/neuro.05.005.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marder, E. (2012). Neuromodulation of neuronal circuits: back to the future. Neuron 76, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meeusen, R. (2005). Exercise and the brain: insight in new therapeutic modalities. Ann. Transplant. 10, 49–51.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mello, C. V., and Clayton, D. F. (2015). The opportunities and challeges of large-scale molecular approaches to songbird neurobiology. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 50, 70–76. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nestler, E. J., and Hyman, S. E. (2010). Animal models of neuropsychiatric disorders. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1161–1169. doi: 10.1038/nn.2647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connell, L. A., and Hofmann, H. A. (2011). The vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: a comparative synthesis. J. Comp. Neurol. 519, 3599–3639. doi: 10.1002/cne.22735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connell, L. A., and Hofmann, H. A. (2012). Evolution of a vertebrate social decision-making network. Science 336, 1154–1157. doi: 10.1126/science.1218889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliveira, R. F. (2013). Mind the fish: zebrafish as a model in cognitive social neuroscience. Front. Neural Circuits 7:131. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Øverli, O., Korzan, W. J., Höglund, E., Winberg, S., Bollig, H., Watt, M., et al. (2004a). Stress coping style predicts aggression and social dominance in rainbow trout. Horm. Behav. 45, 235–241. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2003.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Øverli, O., Korzan, W. J., Larson, E. T., Winberg, S., Lepage, O., Pottinger, T. G., et al. (2004b). Behavioral and neuroendocrine correlates of displaced aggression in trout. Horm. Behav. 45, 324–329. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Øverli, O., Sørensen, C., Pulman, K. G., Pottinger, T. G., Korzan, W., Summers, C. H., et al. (2007). Evolutionary background for stress-coping styles: relationships between physiological, behavioral and cognitive traits in non-mammalian vertebrates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 396–412. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearson, B. L., Crawley, J. N., Eilam, D., Pentkowski, N. S., and Summers, C. H. (2016). Curiosity as an approach to ethoexperimental analysis: behavioral neuroscience as seen by students and colleagues of Bob Blanchard. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.012 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pottinger, T. G., and Carrick, T. R. (1999). Modification of the plasma cortisol response to stress in rainbow trout by selective breeding. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 116, 122–132. doi: 10.1006/gcen.1999.7355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preuss, T. M. (1995). Do rats have prefrontal cortex? The rose-woolsey-akert program reconsidered. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 7, 1–24. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preuss, T. M. (2000). Taking the measure of diversity: comparative alternatives to the model-animal paradigm in cortical neuroscience. Brain Behav. Evol. 55, 287–299. doi: 10.1159/000006664

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riise, S., Vindas, M. A., Nilsson, G. E., and Øverli, Ø. (2015). Can behavioral inhibition in fish help endure a risky social environment? Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 45, 163.112.

Rittschof, C. C., Bukhari, S. A., Sloofman, L. G., Troy, J. M., Caetano-Anolles, D., Cash-Ahmed, A., et al. (2014). Neuromolecular responses to social challenge: common mechanisms across mouse, stickleback fish and honey bee. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 111, 17929–17934. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1420369111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, J. M., Prince, M. A., Achua, J. K., Carpenter, R. E., Arendt, D. H., Smith, J. P., et al. (2015). Nuance and behavioral cogency: how the visible burrow system inspired the stress-alternatives model and conceptualization of the continuum of anxiety. Physiol. Behav. 146, 86–97. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.03.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sgoifo, A., de Boer, S. F., Haller, J., and Koolhaas, J. M. (1996). Individual differences in plasma catecholamine and corticosterone stress responses of wild-type rats: relationship with aggression. Physiol. Behav. 60, 1403–1407. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(96)00229-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. P., Prince, M. A., Achua, J. K., Robertson, J. M., Anderson, R. T., Ronan, P. J., et al. (2016). Intensity of anxiety is modified via complex integrative stress circuitries. Psychoneuroendocrinology 63, 351–361. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strausfeld, N. J., and Hirth, F. (2013). Deep homology of arthropod central complex and vertebrate basal ganglia. Science 340, 157–161. doi: 10.1126/science.1231828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Striedter, G. F., Belgard, T. G., Chen, C.-C., Davis, F. P., Finlay, B. L., Güntürkün, O., et al. (2014). NSF workshop report: discovering general principles of nervous system organization by comparing brain maps across species. Brain Behav. Evol. 83, 1–8. doi: 10.1159/000360152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, C. H. (2001). Mechanisms for quick and variable responses. Brain Behav. Evol. 57, 283–292. doi: 10.1159/000047246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, C. H., Forster, G. L., Korzan, W. J., Watt, M. J., Larson, E. T., Overli, O., et al. (2005a). Dynamics and mechanics of social rank reversal. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 191, 241–252. doi: 10.1007/s00359-004-0554-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, C. H., Korzan, W. J., Lukkes, J. L., Watt, M. J., Forster, G. L., Øverli, Ø., et al. (2005b). Does serotonin influence aggression? Comparing regional activity before and during social interaction. Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 78, 679–694. doi: 10.1086/432139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, C. H., Watt, M. J., Ling, T. L., Forster, G. L., Carpenter, R. E., Korzan, W. J., et al. (2005c). Glucocorticoid interaction with aggression in non-mammalian vertebrates: reciprocal action. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 526, 21–35. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.09.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, C. H., and Greenberg, N. (1994). Somatic correlates of adrenergic activity during aggression in the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Horm. Behav. 28, 29–40. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1994.1003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, C. H., and Greenberg, N. (1995). Activation of central biogenic amines following aggressive interaction in male lizards, Anolis carolinensis. Brain Behav. Evol. 45, 339–349. doi: 10.1159/000113561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, C. H., Larson, E. T., Summers, T. R., Renner, K. J., and Greenberg, N. (1998). Regional and temporal separation of serotonergic activity mediating social stress. Neuroscience 87, 489–496. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(98)00144-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, C. H., Summers, T. R., Moore, M. C., Korzan, W. J., Woodley, S. K., Ronan, P. J., et al. (2003a). Temporal patterns of limbic monoamine and plasma corticosterone response during social stress. Neuroscience 116, 553–563. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00708-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, T. R., Matter, J. M., McKay, J. M., Ronan, P. J., Larson, E. T., Renner, K. J., et al. (2003b). Rapid glucocorticoid stimulation and GABAergic inhibition of hippocampal serotonergic response: in vivo dialysis in the lizard Anolis carolinensis. Horm. Behav. 43, 245–253. doi: 10.1016/s0018-506x(02)00014-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, C. H., and Winberg, S. (2006). Interactions between the neural regulation of stress and aggression. J. Exp. Biol. 209, 4581–4589. doi: 10.1242/jeb.02565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, T. R., Hunter, A. L., and Summers, C. H. (1997). Female social reproductive roles affect central monoamines. Brain Res. 767, 272–278. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(97)00604-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Z. Tierpsychol. 20, 410–433. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1963.tb01161.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van de Weerd, H. A., Van Loo, P. L., Van Zutphen, L. F., Koolhaas, J. M., and Baumans, V. (1997). Nesting material as environmental enrichment has no adverse effects on behavior and physiology of laboratory mice. Physiol. Behav. 62, 1019–1028. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00232-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Berg, C. L., Hol, T., Van Ree, J. M., Spruijt, B. M., Everts, H., and Koolhaas, J. M. (1999). Play is indispensable for an adequate development of coping with social challenges in the rat. Dev. Psychobiol. 34, 129–138. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-2302(199903)34:2<129::aid-dev6>3.0.co;2-l

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villar, D., Flicek, P., and Odam, D. T. (2014). Evolution of transcription factor binding in metazoans–mechanisms and functional implications. Nat. Rev. Genet. 15, 221–233. doi: 10.1038/nrg3481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vindas, M. A., Johansen, I. B., Folkedal, O., Höglund, E., Gorissen, M., Flik, G., et al. (2016). Brain serotonergic activation in growth-stunted farmed salmon: adaption versus pathology. R. Soc. Open Sci. 3:160030. doi: 10.1098/rsos.160030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wise, S. P. (2008). Forward frontal fields: phylogeny and fundamental function. Trends Neurosci. 31, 599–608. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiao, S., Xie, D., Cao, X., Yu, P., Xing, X., Chen, C.-C., et al. (2012). Comparative epigenomic annotation of regulatory DNA. Cell 149, 1381–1392. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.04.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yartsev, M. M., Witter, M. P., and Ulanovsky, N. (2011). Grid cells without theta oscillations in the entorhinal cortex of bats. Nature 479, 103–107. doi: 10.1038/nature10583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, J. W., Amitai, N., and Geyer, M. A. (2012). Behavioral animal models to assess pro-cognitive treatments for schizophrenia. Handb. Exp. Pharmacol. 39–79. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-25758-2_3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, D., Lustig, K. H., Bifulco, K., and Keifer, J. (2005). Thalamocortical connections in the pond turtle Pseudemys scripta elegans. Brain Behav. Evol. 65, 278–292. doi: 10.1159/000084317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: animal models, diversity, neuroscience, central nervous system, evolution, conservation

Citation: Keifer J and Summers CH (2016) Putting the “Biology” Back into “Neurobiology”: The Strength of Diversity in Animal Model Systems for Neuroscience Research. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10:69. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00069

Received: 04 April 2016; Accepted: 02 August 2016;
Published: 22 August 2016.

Edited by:

Jonathan B. Fritz, University of Maryland, College Park, USA

Reviewed by:

Paul Manger, University of the Witwatersrand, South Africa
Raúl G. Paredes, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico

Copyright © 2016 Keifer and Summers. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Joyce Keifer, jkeifer@usd.edu;
Cliff H. Summers, cliff@usd.edu

Download