Skip to main content

CLINICAL TRIAL article

Front. Pediatr., 01 May 2017
Sec. Children and Health
Volume 5 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fped.2017.00094

A Quasi-Experimental Study of a Movement and Preliteracy Program for 3- and 4-Year-Old Children

imageChloe Bedard1* imageEmily Bremer2 imageWenonah Campbell3 imageJohn Cairney1,2,4
  • 1INfant and Child Health (INCH) Lab, Department of Health Research Method, Evidence, and Impact, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
  • 2INfant and Child Health (INCH) Lab, Department of Kinesiology, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
  • 3INfant and Child Health (INCH) Lab, Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
  • 4Faculty of Kinesiology and Physical Education, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada

Objective: Approximately 28% of children are not ready for kindergarten, 91% are inactive according to current guidelines, and 21% are overweight/obese. Early intervention to strengthen movement and preliteracy skills may help to curb the concerning rates of poor school readiness, inactivity, obesity, and subsequently positively impact health across the lifespan. The objective of this pilot study was to evaluate the effectiveness of a motor and preliteracy skill program for a community sample of 3- to 4-year-old children.

Methods: A quasi-experimental study design was used. The program was run for 1 h/week for 10 weeks and consisted of movement skill instruction, free play, and an interactive reading circle with care-giver involvement throughout each session. Movement and preliteracy skills were assessed in all children pre- and post-intervention using the Peabody Developmental Motor Scales-2nd edition, the Preschool Word and Print Awareness tool, and the Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening tool.

Results: Nineteen families (experimental group, n = 8; control group, n = 11) were recruited (mean age = 3 years, 8 m; 47% male). There was a significant effect of group on gross motor raw scores overall [F(1, 16) = 4.67, p < 0.05; ωp2=0.16] and print-concept knowledge [F(1, 16) = 11.9, p < 0.05; ωp2=0.38].

Conclusion: This study was one of the first to examine the impact of a community-based movement skill and preliteracy program with care-giver involvement in preschool children. Future research should continue to explore the effects of the program with larger and more diverse samples on multiple health and developmental outcomes.

Clinical Trial Registration: Play and Preliteracy among Young Children (PLAY) NCT02432443.

Introduction

There is mounting evidence linking health status and behaviors in childhood to future health problems in adolescence, adulthood, and later adulthood (1, 2). For example, children who are overweight or obese are more likely to be overweight as adults and suffer from cardiovascular disease and poor mental health (3, 4). In response to this evidence, a great deal of research has focused on optimizing childhood development in order to improve health outcomes across the lifespan. However, in a Canadian population of children, 28% are not ready for school (5), 91% are insufficiently active according to current guidelines (6), and 21% are considered overweight or obese (7). Given the high prevalence of inactivity, overweight/obesity, and poor school readiness, we can conclude that significant improvements need to be made in order to enhance the health and development of children over time. Extant research has targeted children who have developmental delays or who are of school age, leaving a critical gap in the literature for younger, preschool-aged children with typical development (8). There is value in attending to this large part of the population as the potential impact of intervening in this population is large.

Underlying the rise in physical inactivity, poor school readiness, and obesity rates is hypothesized to be poorly developed motor and cognitive skills, particularly gross motor and preliteracy skills. This hypothesis emerges from the literature relating motor proficiency with physical, psychosocial, and cognitive development as well as evidence linking the development of preliteracy skills with psychosocial and cognitive domains (911). With respect to motor development, it has been shown that the attainment of fundamental movement skills (FMS) such as throwing and catching allow children to learn more complex movements that facilitate independent participation in activities of daily living, athletic pursuits, and physical activity (PA) (12). Engagement in these types of activities improves physical development through increases in PA levels, physical fitness, and healthy body composition (8, 13). In addition, increasing evidence from neuroscience research suggests movement skills in childhood are inextricably linked to other core aspects of development including social, cognitive, and psychological domains (1417). With respect to preliteracy skills, studies have demonstrated relationships between early language and literacy development and later socioemotional regulation (18), social behavior (9), and academic self-perceptions (10).

There is reasonable theoretical, empirical, and practical evidence to promote a component-based program that teaches movement and preliteracy skills. From an embodied cognition perspective, motor and cognitive developments are intrinsically linked as cognitive processes, in particular language, emerge as individuals interact with their physical and social environment (19). As children develop their movement skills and interact with their surroundings, they have more diverse opportunities to learn about their environment and develop their language and other cognitive abilities (20, 21). Early language and preliteracy skills are intimately linked through their role in communication (22) thus, by extension we may apply the embodied cognition perspective to a paired movement skill and preliteracy skill-learning paradigm, in which we may expect enhanced learning. Empirical evidence supports this relationship between movement and preliteracy skills. Callcott et al. (23) demonstrated the synergistic benefits of a blended learning program on both movement and preliteracy outcomes. When preliteracy skill-building activities were used as a medium to teach movement skills, children showed greater improvements compared to a group of children participating in a movement-only program (23). From a practical perspective, including both reading activities and movement skill-building activities in a single program will attend to both parental and kindergarten curricular interests. Parents and educators of young children are concerned with achievement of both motor milestones and school readiness indicators; therefore, by targeting both domains in a single program, we will appeal to various child care providers. Finally, skill-building activities for both movement and preliteracy development are fun and enjoyable for children and their families (24, 25). Learning how to jump and throw and sharing a book with their caregivers are intrinsically appealing to children, which is an important predictor of participation and engagement (26).

It is critical to consider the role of the parents in these programs. Parents act as a child’s primary instructor in the early years when children are learning new skills and with effective strategies they can support their development in a number of domains (2729). The involvement of parents in skill-building programs allows for skill learning to continue outside of the program setting. Systematic reviews have consistently supported the inclusion of parents throughout programs and the dissemination of parent materials to facilitate knowledge translation into the home in order to achieve sustained improvement in movement skill proficiency (27, 30).

In light of all of the aforementioned considerations, we designed an evidence-based intervention targeting movement and preliteracy skills in children 3–4 years of age. The objective of this study is to evaluate the effectiveness of the program.

Methods

Design

A quasi-experimental study design was used to evaluate the program. All children were assessed twice approximately 10–12 weeks apart: children in the experimental group were assessed pre- and post-program; children in the control group were assessed at baseline and again following 10 weeks. Families had the option of participating in the experimental group or control group, depending on their preference for the start date (Summer or Fall). Families in the control group were able to participate in the program after their second appointment was completed.

Participants

A convenience sample of families was recruited through advertisements at Ontario Early Years Centres, Boys and Girls Clubs of Hamilton, various licensed daycares, and the Hamilton Public Libraries from May to July 2015. Children were eligible to participate if they were between the ages of 3 years, 0 months and 4 years, 11 months at baseline and must not have been diagnosed with any developmental delay or other health condition that would prohibit safe participation in the program.

Intervention

The program took place at the Boys and Girls Club of Hamilton and was led by two graduate students (Emily Bremer and Chloe Bedard under Dr. Cairney’s supervision). The program ran for 60 min once per week for 10 consecutive weeks and consisted of three components: direct FMS instruction, unstructured exploratory free play, and a dialogic storybook reading activity. The first segment of the program focused each week on a specific movement skill through single-step skill acquisition strategies (i.e., introducing new skills one-by-one). The free-play segment of the program allowed children the opportunity to self-direct their own activities: children had access to various play items including both gross motor and fine motor equipment (e.g., playground balls and puzzle pieces). The curriculum and teaching strategies used for the first two segments of our intervention have been successfully implemented in previous research to improve the movement skills of preschool-aged children with autism (31, 32); these strategies have been adapted for use with our general population of children. The final segment of our program was a dialogic shared book reading circle with all the children and their parents. Each week used one book to develop one to two preliteracy skills. Specific strategies and books were selected from several evidence-based curricula (25, 33, 34). There was active involvement of at least one parent in the direct instruction and reading components.

Outcome Measures

Demographic and Engagement Survey

The demographic questionnaire was completed at baseline and included questions about the parent and the child on age, gender, ethnicity, parental education and occupation, and household income. A parent engagement questionnaire was administered at both assessments and was developed to assess the use and the frequency of use, of specific activities that were employed in our program to determine if parents use these activities at home.

Preliteracy Skills

Children were administered the Preschool Word and Print Awareness tool (PWPA) test and the Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening: preschool (PALS-PK)-uppercase alphabet recognition task at both assessments to measure print-concept knowledge and alphabet knowledge (35, 36), respectively. The PWPA tests children on their print-concept knowledge, such as print directionality and print function, using 14 items administered in an interactive storybook reading format (35). Raw scores are then transformed into standardized scores with a mean of 100 and SD of 15. The PWPA has strong validity, and the reliability is 0.74 in a sample of children aged 3–5 years (37). The PALS-PK uppercase alphabet recognition task involves children naming each of the 26 letters of the alphabet as they are presented in a random order. The inter-rater reliability coefficient of this task is 0.99 (36). These measurements together took approximately 15 min to complete and were administered by a trained graduate student.

Motor Skills

Children were administered the gross motor subtests of the Peabody Developmental Motor Scales-2nd edition (PDMS-2) (38) at both assessments. The PDMS-2 is a standardized instrument designed to measure the progress of development of gross and fine motor skills in children from birth to age 6. The three gross motor subtests—stationary, locomotion, and object manipulation—were administered by two trained graduate students. The sum of the raw scores of each of the three subtests was used as the dependent variable in the primary analysis. The assessment required approximately 30–45 min to conduct (39). The PDMS-2 has good validity and sensitivity to change as assessed previously in 4-year-old typically developing children and the inter-rater reliability is 0.89 (39, 40).

Attendance and Home Practice

Attendance at the program sessions and the frequency of weekly home practice was measured using attendance checklists and parent-reported questionnaires.

Procedure

The study received ethical approval from the Hamilton Integrated Research Ethics Board. All study appointments took place at the INfant and Child Health (INCH) Lab at McMaster University. After eligibility was confirmed over the telephone, parents were asked to select a time for their first study appointment. Informed written consent was obtained at the first study visit. At each appointment children were administered the PWPA, PALS-PK—uppercase alphabet recognition task to assess preliteracy skills, and the PDMS-2 to assess movement skills. Parents were asked to complete the demographic (at the first assessment only) and the engagement questionnaire. The duration of each appointment was approximately 1–1.5 h. After the first appointment, families were asked to select their group (experimental/summer start or control/fall start). Families in the experimental group began the program within 1–2 weeks of their baseline appointment. After the experimental group completed the program, all families were asked to come in for their second assessment in which they were administered the same tests. See Figure 1 for the flow diagram depicting the study procedures.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow diagram for study procedures.

Statistical Analyses

Basic descriptive statistics of the demographic characteristics of the sample and attendance and at-home practice rates were computed. Between-group differences in baseline characteristics were compared using independent t-tests for continuous variables and chi square (Fischer’s exact test) for categorical variables. The primary analyses were three group by time analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) to assess changes in the children’s gross motor skills (raw scores), print-concept knowledge (standardized scores), and alphabet knowledge between the experimental and control groups at time 2 with time 1 scores entered as a covariate. Post hoc ANCOVAs were used to examine differences across time in each gross motor subtest between groups. Secondary analyses included two group by time ANCOVAs examining differences in changes in parental engagement in both movement and preliteracy skills. The partial omega squared will be used as a measure of effect size as it calculates the variance accounted for by group assignment controlling for the time 1 scores. This effect size is appropriate for analyses with more than one independent variable (i.e., an ANCOVA) (41). Effect sizes are interpreted as small for values over 0.01, medium for values over 0.06, and large for value over 0.14 (42). All analyses were conducted on a per-protocol basis. A two-tailed alpha value of 0.05 was used to determine statistical significance.

Results

Descriptives

Twenty-one families were eligible and consented to participate in the first study appointment (see Figure 2). Of these 21 families, 19 completed the second study visit: 2 families discontinued the program and were lost to follow-up; 1 family withdrew from the intervention due to time constraints, however, attended the second study appointment. The final study sample included 19 children (10 boys) ranging from 3 years, 0 months to 4 years 11 months (mean = 3 years, 8 months, SD = 7.30 months).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Flow diagram to indicate the included and excluded participants.

Table 1 describes the demographic characteristics of the sample by group. There were no significant differences between the two groups for any demographic characteristic, with the exception of parental education (see Table 1). Baseline scores on raw and age-standardized gross motor skills, print-concept knowledge, and alphabet knowledge were not significantly different between groups (see Table 2). The median attendance level was 8 out of 10 program sessions (IQR: 7–9 sessions). The average rate of home practice was 45% (SD = 17%) and 52% (SD = 18%) for the movement and preliteracy activities, respectively.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample demographic characteristics of the experimental and control groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Baseline scores of the experimental and control groups.

Intervention Effects

Mean scores and SDs of the primary outcomes (movement and preliteracy skills) at each time point by group are presented in Table 3. Overall, both group improved their movement and preliteracy skills over the 10 weeks (see Table 3). Parents showed small increases in their levels of engagement, with the exception of parents in the control group and their engagement in motor activities (see Table 3). Results of the ANCOVA on the primary outcomes are presented in Table 4. There was a significant group effect on gross motor raw scores measured at time 2, after controlling for time 1 scores [F(1, 16) = 4.67, p < 0.05; ωp2=0.16]. There was also a significant effect of group on print-concept knowledge [F(1, 16) = 11.9, p < 0.05; ωp2=0.38]; however, improvements in uppercase letter recognition did not differ significantly between groups [F(1, 15) = 0.048, p = 0.83; ωp2<0.001]. Post hoc analyses reveal that there was a significant difference in the improvement of object manipulation skills in the experimental group, compared to the control group; improvements in scores on the stationary and locomotor domains were not significantly different between groups (see Table 5). The results of the secondary analyses indicate non-significant differences between the experimental and control group on their level of engagement in both movement and preliteracy activities (see Table 6). Figures 3A–H show the change over time in the experimental and control group for each primary and secondary outcomes. Figure 4 illustrates the change over time in the gross motor scores of the individual participants in the experimental group. There were no reported adverse effects of the intervention.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Scores for each assessment in the experimental and control groups for gross motor and preliteracy skills, and parent engagement scores.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Analysis of covariance results for primary outcomes of motor and preliteracy skills.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Post hoc analysis of covariance results.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Analysis of covariance results for secondary outcomes.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean scores at time 1 and time 2 for both groups; error bars represent the 95% confidence intervals. (A) Gross motor skills; (B) print-concept knowledge; (C) locomotor skills; (D) stationary skills; (E) object manipulation skills; (F) alphabet knowledge; (G) parental preliteracy engagement scores; and (H) parental movement engagement scores.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Individual change over time in gross motor skills in the experimental group.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate the impact of a program designed to improve preschool-aged children’s movement and preliteracy skills and the results show large significant effects between the experimental and control group in the improvement in both skill domains. This study is timely as evidence continues to accumulate supporting the critical importance of movement and preliteracy skills to several health and academic outcomes in preschool-aged children (43, 44). This alongside the growing number of children who are physically inactive (6), overweight or obese (7), and not ready for school (5) signals that targeted multicomponent interventions are necessary. An opportune time to intervene exists during the early years because children typically find enjoyment in learning new movement skills, such as jumping and throwing, and reading stories; thus, finding movement- and reading-based programs fun (24).

It is important to consider the results of this intervention within the context of the program structure. The positive improvements in gross motor skills suggest that programs consisting of direct movement skill instruction have the capacity to improve the movement skills of children with typical development. This aligns with the few intervention studies conducted in this age group showing positive gains in children participating in direct movement skill instruction (27). Furthermore, the significant gains in both gross motor and preliteracy skills despite the relatively low dose of the intervention (i.e., one time per week for 1 h) highlight the importance of both the combined learning environment and the parental component. As demonstrated by Callcott et al. (23), pairing motor and preliteracy learning in a single program has the capacity to produce synergistic gains in both domains. Although this synergistic hypothesis was not directly tested in this study, our results suggest that important gains in movement and preliteracy skills can be achieved in a dual program that capitalizes on this relationship. Additionally, parents were actively involved in the program and were provided with handouts outlining the weekly activities and encouraged to practice the activities at home. The rate of at-home practice for both the movement and reading activities ranged from 45 to 52%, indicating that activities were practiced on approximately half the days not spent in the program. The involvement of the parents was likely critical to expand their child’s learning beyond the weekly session. One recommendation from Veldman et al. (27) suggests that programs should run at least twice a week; however, our results suggest that running one session a week and involving parents actively throughout the program may confer similar skill gains to a higher-dose program.

The results of the post hoc analyses on our primary motor outcomes indicate that the program seems to selectively improve object manipulation skills, not stationary or locomotor skills. However, there was a medium effect size observed for locomotor skills suggesting that non-significant findings for this domain may have been a power issue. The non-significant findings with respect to stationary and locomotor skills suggest that the movement skill component of the program should be modified to enhance skill development in these domains. We also found selective improvements in print-concept knowledge and not in alphabet knowledge. This may be explained through the organization of the program: print concepts were introduced during the first half of the intervention and alphabet concepts were introduced in the latter half. Thus, children were exposed to print concepts for a longer duration over the intervention period, compared to alphabet concepts, lending to the observed results. Finally, we also found non-significant findings with respect to levels of parental engagement of motor and preliteracy activities at home, and this may explained in consideration of the parental component of the intervention and measurement issues. The effect size for changes in parental engagement in motor and preliteracy activities at home are moderate and large, respectively; therefore, the analyses may be limited in their power to detect a statistical difference even when these fairly substantial increases exist. Furthermore, the measurement of engagement was through a self-reported survey, which may not be sufficiently sensitive to detect significant differences. Finally, the survey items asked about only the frequency of engagement, not the quality of practice. Therefore, while we could not detect changes in frequency of practice of activities at home, it may be that parents engaged in infrequent, but high-quality practice at home.

There are limitations that should be noted in conjunction with the results of the study. First, participants were not randomly assigned to the intervention and control group, thus there are some imbalances in baseline characteristics. The imbalances in baseline motor and preliteracy skills were non-significant and were accounted for in the analyses; the effect of the intervention remained significant after controlling for these differences in time 1 scores. However, it remains possible that our results reflect greater opportunity for improvement in the experimental group given their lower baseline scores. Thus, future research should ensure that baseline movement and preliteracy scores are equivalent in both groups. The imbalance in parental education levels may have impacted the results through differences in parental motivations (i.e., reasons underlying their decision to participate in the study), quality of activity practice at home (i.e., how closely home practice resembled the intervention activities), and the general quality of the child’s surrounding environment (i.e., access to physical equipment and social support). However, as the imbalance in parental education was not accompanied by differences in household income levels, or baseline levels of parental engagement, it is difficult to determine the true effect of this statistical imbalance. Nonetheless, it should be considered a limitation of our sample. Second, our sample was small and relatively homogenous; therefore, the generalizability of the results is limited. Furthermore, the power of our analyses is limited by the small sample; the analyses of the effect of the intervention on the subdomains of motor skills and parental engagement levels were substantially underpowered, and this may explain their non-significance.

Conclusion

The results of this study show that participation in a direct instruction, community-based, parent-oriented movement and preliteracy program can significantly improve movement and preliteracy skill levels of preschool children with typical development. These novel results highlight the feasibility and importance of intervening during the preschool years and involving the parents throughout the intervention to maximize gains. Future research should begin to investigate the generalizability of the program in different community settings with larger more diverse samples.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of Hamilton Integrated Research Ethics Board with written informed consent from all parents of participating children. All parents of participating children gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

CB designed the study, coordinated recruitment and data collection, designed the preliteracy component of the intervention and assisted with weekly implementation of the intervention, carried on the data analyses, and drafted the initial manuscript. EB assisted with study design, recruitment and data collection, design of the motor component of the intervention and weekly implementation of the intervention, and revised and approved the final manuscript as submitted. WC assisted with design of the intervention, selection and design of outcome measurements, and reviewed and approved the final manuscript as submitted. JC supervised the design and execution of all phases of the study and revised and approved the final manuscript as submitted. All the authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank our valuable volunteers for helping us run the program. We would also like to thank our participants and their families for contributing their time and energy into this study.

Funding

JC was supported by an endowed research chair through the Department of Family Medicine, McMaster University. The funding source did not have any involvement in the study design, data collection, analysis and interpretation of data, or writing of the manuscript.

Abbreviations

PWPA, preschool word and print awareness; PALS-PK, phonological awareness literacy screening-preschool; PDMS-2, peabody developmental motor scales-2nd edition; ANCOVA, analysis of covariance.

References

1. Case A, Fertig A, Paxson C. The lasting impact of childhood health and circumstance. J Health Econ (2005) 24(2):365–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2004.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Rutter M. Pathways from childhood to adult life. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1989) 30(1):23–51. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00768.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Must A, Strauss RS. Risks and consequences of childhood and adolescent obesity. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord (1999) 23(S2):2–11. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0800852

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Guo SS, Chumlea WC. Tracking of body mass index in children in relation to overweight in adulthood. Am J Clin Nutr (1999) 70(1):145s–8s.

Google Scholar

5. Ministry of Health and Long-Term Care, Public Health Division. Maintaining the Gains, Moving the Yardstick: Ontario Health Status Report, 2011. Toronto, ON (2011). Available from: http://www.health.gov.on.ca/en/common/ministry/publications/reports/cmoh_13/cmoh_13.pdf

Google Scholar

6. Barnes JD, Cameron C, Carson V, Chaput JP, Faulkner GE, Janson K, et al. Results from Canada’s 2016 ParticipACTION report card on physical activity for children and youth. J Phys Act Health (2016) 13(11 Suppl 2):S110–6. doi:10.1123/jpah.2016-0300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Shields M. Overweight and obesity among children and youth. Health Rep (2006) 17(3):27–42.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

8. Lubans DR, Morgan PJ, Cliff DP, Barnett LM, Okely AD. Fundamental movement skills in children and adolescents. Sports Med (2010) 40(12):1019–35. doi:10.2165/11536850-000000000-00000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Hart KI, Fujiki M, Brinton B, Hart CH. The relationship between social behavior and severity of language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res (2004) 47(3):647–62. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2004/050)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Chapman JW, Tunmer WE, Prochnow JE. Early reading-related skills and performance, reading self-concept, and the development of academic self-concept: a longitudinal study. J Educ Psychol (2000) 92(4):703. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.92.4.703

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Claessens A, Duncan G, Engel M. Kindergarten skills and fifth-grade achievement: evidence from the ECLS-K. Econ Educ Rev (2009) 28(4):415–27. doi:10.1016/j.econedurev.2008.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Clark JE. From the beginning: a developmental perspective on movement and mobility. Quest (2005) 57(1):37–45. doi:10.1080/00336297.2005.10491841

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Hendrix CG, Prins MR, Dekkers H. Developmental coordination disorder and overweight and obesity in children: a systematic review. Obes Rev (2014) 15(5):408–23. doi:10.1111/obr.12137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev (2000) 71(1):44–56. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Diamond A. Effects of physical exercise on executive functions: going beyond simply moving to moving with thought. Ann Sports Med Res (2015) 2(1):1011.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

16. Iverson JM. Developing language in a developing body: the relationship between motor development and language development. J Child Lang (2010) 37(02):229–61. doi:10.1017/S0305000909990432

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. van der Fels IM, Te Wierike SC, Hartman E, Elferink-Gemser MT, Smith J, Visscher C. The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4-16 year old typically developing children: a systematic review. J Sci Med Sport (2015) 18(6):697–703. doi:10.1016/j.jsams.2014.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Fujiki M, Brinton B, Clarke D. Emotion regulation in children with specific language impairment. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch (2002) 33(2):102–11. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2002/008)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Smith L, Gasser M. The development of embodied cognition: six lessons from babies. Artif Life (2005) 11(1–2):13–29. doi:10.1162/1064546053278973

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Hofsten CV. Action, the foundation for cognitive development. Scand J Psychol (2009) 50(6):617–23. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00780.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Adolph KE, Joh AS. Motor Development: How Infants Get Into the Act. Introduction to Infant Development. (2006). Available from: http://dukespace.lib.duke.edu/dspace/handle/10161/7334

Google Scholar

23. Callcott D, Hammond L, Hill S. The synergistic effect of teaching a combined explicit movement and phonological awareness program to preschool aged students. Early Child Educ J (2015) 43(3):201–11. doi:10.1007/s10643-014-0652-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Pellegrini AD, Smith PK. Physical activity play: the nature and function of a neglected aspect of play. Child Dev (1998) 69(3):577–98. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06226.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Zevenbergen AA, Whitehurst GJ. Dialogic reading: a shared picture book reading intervention for preschoolers. In: Van Kleeck A, Stahl SA, Bauer EB, editors. On Reading Books to Children: Parents and Teachers. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates (2003). p. 177–200.

Google Scholar

26. Chambers M, Sugden D. Early Years Movement Skills: Description, Diagnosis and Intervention. London: Wiley (2006). 182 p.

Google Scholar

27. Veldman SL, Jones RA, Okely AD. Efficacy of gross motor skill interventions in young children: an updated systematic review. BMJ Open Sport Exerc Med (2016) 2(1):e000067. doi:10.1136/bmjsem-2015-000067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kaiser AP, Hancock TB. Teaching parents new skills to support their young children’s development. Infants Young Child (2003) 16(1):9–21. doi:10.1097/00001163-200301000-00003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Hart B, Risley TR. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes Publishing (1995).

Google Scholar

30. Riethmuller AM, Jones RA, Okely AD. Efficacy of interventions to improve motor development in young children: a systematic review. Pediatrics (2009) 124(4):e782–92. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-0333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Bremer E, Balogh R, Lloyd M. Effectiveness of a fundamental motor skill intervention for 4-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder: a pilot study. Autism (2015) 19(8):980–91. doi:10.1177/1362361314557548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Bremer E, Lloyd M. School-based fundamental-motor-skill intervention for children with autism-like characteristics: an exploratory study. Adapt Phys Activ Q (2016) 33(1):66–88. doi:10.1123/APAQ.2015-0009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Whitehurst GJ, Falco FL, Lonigan CJ, Fischel JE, DeBaryshe BD, Valdez-Menchaca MC, et al. Accelerating language development through picture book reading. Dev Psychol (1988) 24(4):552–9.

Google Scholar

34. Justice LM, McGinty AS. Read It Again Pre-K. School of Teaching and Learning Preschool Language and Literacy Lab (2009). Available from: https://earlychildhood.ehe.osu.edu/files/2016/04/Pre-K-Introduction.pdf

Google Scholar

35. Justice LM, Ezell HK. Use of storybook reading to increase print awareness in at-risk children. Am J Speech Lang Pathol (2002) 11(1):17–29. doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2002/003)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Invernizzi M, Sullivan A, Meier J, Swank L. Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening for Preschool. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia (2004).

Google Scholar

37. Justice LM, Bowles RP, Skibbe LE. Measuring preschool attainment of print-concept knowledge: a study of typical and at-risk 3- to 5-year-old children using item response theory. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch (2006) 37(3):224–35. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2006/024)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Folio MR, Fewell RR. Peabody Developmental Motor Scales: Examiner’s Manual. Austin, TX: PRO-ED (2000).

Google Scholar

39. Darrah J, Magill-Evans J, Volden J, Hodge M, Kembhavi G. Scores of typically developing children on the peabody developmental motor scales – infancy to preschool. Phys Occup Ther Pediatr (2007) 27(3):5–19. doi:10.1080/J006v27n03_02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Burton AW, Miller DE. Movement Skill Assessment. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics (1998).

Google Scholar

41. Olejnik S, Algina J. Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs. Psychol Methods (2003) 8(4):434–47. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.8.4.434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc Publishers (1988).

Google Scholar

43. Palmer KK, Miller MW, Robinson L. Acute exercise enhances preschoolers ability to sustain attention. J Sport Exerc Psychol (2013) 35(4):433–7. doi:10.1123/jsep.35.4.433

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Bremer E, Cairney J. Fundamental movement skills and health-related outcomes a narrative review of longitudinal and intervention studies targeting typically developing children. Am J Lifestyle Med (2016). doi:10.1177/1559827616640196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: early childhood, fundamental movement skills, preliteracy skills, early intervention, child development

Citation: Bedard C, Bremer E, Campbell W and Cairney J (2017) A Quasi-Experimental Study of a Movement and Preliteracy Program for 3- and 4-Year-Old Children. Front. Pediatr. 5:94. doi: 10.3389/fped.2017.00094

Received: 12 January 2017; Accepted: 12 April 2017;
Published: 01 May 2017

Edited by:

Frederick Robert Carrick, Bedfordshire Centre for Mental Health Research in Association with University of Cambridge, UK

Reviewed by:

Cihad Dundar, Ondokuz Mayis University, Turkey
Angela Jocelyn Fawcett, Swansea University, UK

Copyright: © 2017 Bedard, Bremer, Campbell and Cairney. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chloe Bedard, bedardct@mcmaster.ca

Download