Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pharmacol., 26 September 2017
Sec. Neuropharmacology
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuropharmacological, Neurobiological and Behavioral Mechanisms of Learning and Memory View all 38 articles

Negative Facial Expressions – But Not Visual Scenes – Enhance Human Working Memory in Younger and Older Participants

  • 1Laboratory of Neurosciences and Behavior, Department of Physiological Sciences, University of Brasilia, Brasilia, Brazil
  • 2Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 3Faculty of Ceilandia, University of Brasilia, Brasilia, Brazil
  • 4Euro-American University Center (UNIEURO), Brasilia, Brazil
  • 5Neuroscience Research Program, CEUMA University, São Luís, Brazil

Many studies have investigated the influence of emotion on memory processes across the human lifespan. Some results have shown older adults (OA) performing better with positive stimuli, some with negative items, whereas some found no impact of emotional valence. Here we tested, in two independent studies, how younger adults (YA) and OA would perform in a visuospatial working memory (VSWM) task with positive, negative, and neutral images. The task consisted of identifying the new location of a stimulus in a crescent set of identical stimuli presented in different locations in a touch-screen monitor. In other words, participants should memorize the locations previously occupied to identify the new location. For each trial, the number of occupied locations increased until 8 or until a mistake was made. In study 1, 56 YA and 38 OA completed the task using images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS). Results showed that, although YA outperformed OA, no effects of emotion were found. In study 2, 26 YA and 25 OA were tested using facial expressions as stimuli. Data from this study showed that negative faces facilitated performance and this effect did not differ between age groups. No differences were found between men and women. Taken together, our findings suggest that YA and OA’s VSWM can be influenced by the emotional valence of the information, though this effect was present only for facial stimuli. Presumably, this may have happened due to the social and biological importance of such stimuli, which are more effective in transmitting emotions than IAPS images. Critically, our results also indicate that the mixed findings in the literature about the influence of aging on the interactions between memory and emotion may be caused by the use of different stimuli and methods. This possibility should be kept in mind in future studies about memory and emotion across the lifespan.

Introduction

Visuospatial working memory (VSWM), one of the storage subsystems of working memory (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974, 1994), has been found to be impacted by cognitive aging (Fabiani et al., 2015; Cabeza et al., 2016). Hale and colleagues, for instance, conducted a series of studies comparing age-related differences between visuospatial and verbal working memory (Jenkins et al., 2000; Myerson et al., 2003; Hale et al., 2011) and found stronger differences between younger (YA) and older adults (OA) in the visuospatial domain, suggesting that the latter is severely affected by aging (see also Bopp and Verhaeghen, 2007). Nevertheless, the effects of emotional processing in the age-related decline in VSWM have yet to be understood. In other words, although it is known that the modulatory effect of emotion on memory changes across the lifespan (Scheibe and Carstensen, 2010), it is not clear how these alterations occur in VSWM.

A common idea in the literature about how aging affects memory and emotion interactions is the Positivity Effect, which refers to how OA’s memory is biased toward positive events or stimuli (Carstensen and Mikels, 2005; Mikels et al., 2005; Petrican et al., 2008). A proposal by Mammarella et al. (2016a) offers a biological explanation for this effect, based on the modulatory influences of noradrenaline on emotional memory (Tully and Bolshakov, 2010). According to Mammarella et al. (2016a), recent evidence points to noradrenaline being linked to behavioral changes related to motivation, reward, and stimuli salience (Bouret and Richmond, 2015; Mather et al., 2015). Another line of evidence suggests an increase in activity of the noradrenergic system in aging (Seals and Esler, 2000), which would increase the effects of this component on OA’s emotional memory. Additionally, if aging brings a shift in a person’s goals, motivation, and interests, distinct stimuli may become more or less salient and rewarding. That is, if OA are more focused on emotionally meaningful and positive experiences, these will possibly be differently affected by noradrenaline compared to YA, leading to the Positivity Effect. In fact, such age-related change in goals, motivations, and interests is compatible with another proposal, named the Socioemotional Selectivity Theory (Carstensen et al., 1999). This theory states that one’s goals depend on their temporal context. Whereas YA perceive their remaining lifetime as long, OA tend to focus their cognitive resources on the pursuit of emotionally meaningful experiences. Joining Mammarella et al. (2016a)’s and Carstensen et al. (1999)’s proposals, OA’s focus on positive events may make these events more salient and differentially modulated by noradrenaline, enhancing their memorization.

Recent studies have supported the presence of a Positivity Bias in OA using different types of stimuli, including images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) (Lang et al., 1997;Mammarella et al., 2016b; Kan et al., 2017) and facial stimuli (Castel et al., 2016; Sava et al., 2017). Nevertheless, it is necessary to point out that some studies did not find a Positivity Effect. Those studies found a Negative Bias instead, with both YA and OA having better memory performances for negative items (Gruhn et al., 2005; Thomas and Hasher, 2006; Satler and Tomaz, 2011; Belham et al., 2013; Foster et al., 2013). This may happen because negative stimuli require prompter responses from an evolutionary point of view (Rozin and Royzman, 2001; Galli et al., 2011). Additionally, some studies simply have found no effect of emotion on memory whatsoever (Denburg et al., 2003; Garcia et al., 2011).

Regarding working memory specifically, recent studies have found mixed effects of emotion on OA’s performance. For example, Ziaei et al. (2015, 2017) found no effects of emotional valence when younger and older participants had to indicate if an IAPS image had been presented during the past three trials. Bermudez and Souza (2016), however, asked participants to indicate the six positions previously occupied by IAPS images in an array of 16 images. Valence did not affect YA’s behavior, but OA performed significantly worse with negative images. Mammarella et al. (2013) used a working memory task with emotional words and found that positive valence had a beneficial effect in OA but not in YA. Using the same task, Borella et al. (2014) found that both age groups performed better with negative words, although this result seemed to be influenced by individual differences. In a different study also using words, Truong and Yang (2014) found that negative and positive valence equally facilitated performance and the effects did not between the two age groups.

These mixed findings, together with the lack of knowledge about how OA’s VSWM is influenced by emotions, lead to the necessity of more studies. Here we aimed to investigate how YA and OA would perform in a VSWM test with negative, positive, and neutral stimuli. Younger and older participants responded the Spatial Delayed Recognition Span Task (SDRST) (Satler et al., 2015), in which identical stimuli are presented in different locations in a crescent set of up to eight locations. This task was chosen because it requires that participants keep the initial spatial locations in mind to be able to identify the new location within each trial. The locations are changed in each new trial and participants must update the information in their VSWM. This task has previously been used with YA and OA (Satler et al., 2015). We predicted that emotional valence would facilitate performance in both age groups. However, as detailed in the previous paragraph, the working memory literature shows mixed results in terms of how the age groups’ performance is modulated by the emotional valence of the stimuli. Thus, we did not have a strong prediction as to which valence (negative or positive) would have a larger influence in the current study. We report two independent studies using the same SDRST. Study 1 used negative, positive, and neutral images from the IAPS. In study 2, the stimuli were composed of angry, happy, and neutral facial expressions. We were interested in how accuracy in this VSWM task would be affected by the different valences displayed by the stimuli on the screen.

Study 1

Materials and Methods

Participants and Stimuli

Study 1 included 56 YA recruited from the university’s undergraduate programs (30 women; mean age 21.38 ± 2.90; at least 13 years of formal education) and 39 healthy OA (24 women; mean age 71.10 ± 6.72; at least 13 years of formal education) recruited from the Geriatric Medical Center, University Hospital of Brasilia. All were right-handed volunteers with no history of neurological or psychiatric episodes and no recent use of psychotropic medication, as evaluated by a detailed anamnesis. The eligibility criteria also included no consumption of alcohol or drugs in the 24 h prior to testing. Participants were vaguely informed about the aims of the study, and signed a written informed consent in accordance with the ethical guidelines for research with human subjects (196/96 CNS/MS Resolution). The study was approved by the Human Subjects Ethics Committee of the Health Sciences Faculty of the University of Brasilia (CEP-FS160/08 and CEP-FM064/2007). All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and hearing. OA scored at least 24 on the Mini-mental State Examination (Folstein et al., 1975) and less than 9 on the Geriatric Depression Scale (Yesavage and Sheikh, 1986).

The stimuli material used in study 1 were composed of emotional pictures selected from the IAPS (Lang et al., 1997) and could depict objects and scenes. Three positive images (slides number: 5750, 5030, 1660; valence: 6.60, 6.51, 6.49; and arousal: 3.14, 2.74, 4.57, respectively), four negative images (slides number: 3000, 3120, 3130, 3030; valence: 1.41, 1.56, 1.58, 1.91; and arousal: 7.26, 6.84, 6.97, 6.76, respectively), and two neutral images (slides number: 5510, 7010; valence: 5.15, 4.94; and arousal: 2.82, 1.76, respectively) were selected. No facial images were selected for study 1. Each participant responded to nine trials, one for each image, presented in a pseudo-randomized order. Computer software registered correct and incorrect responses for each given answer. The time of execution of the task varied according to each participant’s response time, but the full procedure did not last more than 2 h.

The Spatial Delayed Recognition Span Task (SDRST)

The task was a computer-based version (Delphi language, computational program TREA) of the SDRST, which measures participants’ working memory (Lacreuse et al., 2005). The task is presented to YA and OA on a touch-screen monitor (LG Studio Works 440, Microtouch, 17′) positioned within arm’s reach. The computer-based SDRST has been successfully used with different populations and stimuli by our group (Satler et al., 2015). In this task, participants must discriminate a novel location of a stimulus among an increasing array of identical stimuli presented sequentially in various locations within the same trial. At the beginning of each trial, one stimulus is presented at random in 1 of the 16 possible locations on the screen. Participants must touch it. After a pre-determined delay, that stimulus re-appears in the same position and another identical stimulus appears in a new position. Participants must touch the stimulus presented in the new location. Every time a correct response is made, a new stimulus is added to the array. This goes on until the maximum of eight stimuli or until a mistake is made. In both cases, a new trial with a different stimulus begins. Correct answers led to the emission of an acute auditory feedback signal; wrong answers led to a bass auditory signal. Stimuli within a single trial were identical and did not repeat in two consecutive trials.

The stimuli were presented for a period of up to 5 s (1 s for YA, 5 s for OA) (Figure 1). Before the beginning of the task, every participant received written and oral instructions and completed a practice session. The practice session used geometrical shapes as stimuli and was conducted in the same fashion as the main task. The practice session was concluded when participants correctly answered two consecutive complete trials (eight stimuli per trial) or after 20 trials.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Experimental design for the Spatial Delayed Recognition Span Task (SDRST). Participants had to memorize the occupied locations and identify the new location within each trial up to a maximum of eight locations. The task was conducted on a touch-screen monitor.

Statistical Analyses

Accuracy was calculated as the mean of correct choices before a mistake for all the trials of each emotional valence. A mixed-design ANOVA was run (SPSS v. 18.00; SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, United States, 2009) with age (YA or OA) as a between-subjects factor, and emotional valence (neutral, positive, and negative) as a within-subjects factor. Significance was defined as a p-value < 0.05.

Results

Mean accuracy for YA and OA can be seen in Table 1. No differences were found between men and women (p > 0.205). Accuracy in this task was not influenced by the emotional valence of the IAPS image (F(2,186) = 0.293, p = 0.706, ηp2 = 0.004). However, YA outperformed OA (F(1,93) = 65.217, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.412). There was no significant interaction between the factors (F(2,186) = 0.723, p = 0.487, ηp2 = 0.008) (Figure 2).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Accuracy during the Spatial Delayed Recognition Span Task (SDRST) for Younger adults and Older adults with positive, neutral, and negative IAPS images.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Mean accuracy for younger (YA) and older adults (OA) responding to the SDRST with neutral, negative, and positive IAPS images. Error bars represent confidence intervals (95%). YA outperformed OA (p < 0.001). There was no significant influence of emotional valence (p = 0.706).

Discussion

The age-related difference in performance found in study 1 is in line with previous research, suggesting that healthy aging is related to deficits in various cognitive domains, including VSWM. For instance, Jenkins et al. (2000) tested YA and OA in visuospatial speed, memory, and learning tasks and discovered an age-related decline on all three types of processing.

The absence of a valence effect on memory performance was surprising and unexpected. As mentioned before, several studies support the existence of a Negativity Bias in YA and a Positivity Effect in OA. Nevertheless, the current results are in line with other investigations reporting no valence effects on memory performance, such as Denburg et al. (2003) and Garcia et al. (2011). The present results are also in line with at least other two studies who found no effects of emotion on YA and OA’s WM for IAPS images (Ziaei et al., 2015, 2017).

One possibility is that the non-significant effect of valence in the current study was caused by the fact that the IAPS images are not a straightforward way of displaying emotions, being sometimes too complex or rich in visual information (Britton et al., 2008). By contrast, facial photographs with different emotional expressions are considered by many studies as one of the most important and direct ways of externalizing emotions (Hess et al., 1997; Nahm et al., 1997). For example, Isaacowitz et al. (2007) investigated age-related differences in the recognition of emotions from lexical stimuli (sentences describing emotional situations) and facial expressions. They found an interaction between age group and task type, indicating that facial stimuli elicited significantly less age differences than did the lexical stimuli. Some researchers, on the other hand, propose that aging brings deficits in the recognition of negative facial expressions, but not of positive ones (Orgeta and Phillips, 2007; Kellough and Knight, 2012). Moreover, Altamura et al. (2016) found that OA are quicker in identifying a positive facial expression compared to a negative one. Thus, we decided to conduct a second study using photographs of facial expressions as stimuli. Due to the social and biological relevance of facial stimuli, we predicted that study 2 would lead to different results from study 1, with emotional valence influencing memory performance of the two age groups.

Study 2

Materials and Methods

Inclusion and exclusion criteria were the same as used in Study 1. Twenty-six YA recruited from the university’s undergraduate programs (13 women; mean age 21.31 ± 2.05 years; at least 14 years of formal education) and 25 healthy OA (11 women; mean age 69.92 ± 6.41 years; at least 13 years of formal education) recruited from the Geriatric Medical Center, University Hospital of Brasilia took part in this study. This study was approved by the Human Subjects Ethics Committee of the Health Sciences Faculty of the University of Brasilia (CEP-FS 097/11). For this study, all participants scored more than 24 on the Mini-mental State Examination (Folstein et al., 1975) and less than 9 on the Geriatric Depression Scale (Yesavage and Sheikh, 1986).

The same SDRST was conducted (Figure 1) using colored photographs (4 cm × 4 cm) of adult models displaying different facial expressions, manipulated to only show the face, with no interference from hair or other body parts. The facial data set was provided to our group by Dr. Hisao Nishijo from the University of Toyama, Japan, and has been used in previous studies from that lab (Hori et al., 2005). Seven images of happiness (positive valence), two images of anger (negative valence), and two neutral expressions were chosen from the original data set based on a pilot study where 30 YA and OA (who did not participate in the main study) identified the emotional expression of several faces displayed on the screen. Only images that elicited a correct classification rate of over 90% were chosen. Each participant responded to one block of each emotional valence. Each block contained 10 trials and two identical faces were never presented in two consecutive trials. Images were presented for 3 s.

Results

Mean accuracy for YA and OA can be seen in Table 2. No differences were found between men and women (p > 0.107). Accuracy was influenced by the valence of the facial expression (F(2,98) = 6.024, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.109). Pairwise comparisons adjusted with the Bonferroni correction showed that negative items elicited much better performance than positive items (p = 0.002) and a marginally better performance than neutral items (p = 0.084). Neutral items did not differ from positive items (p = 0.894). YA performed better than OA (F(1,49) = 40.198, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.451). There was no significant interaction between the factors (F(2,98) = 0.351, p = 0.705, ηp2 = 0.007) (Figure 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Accuracy during the SDRST for Younger adults and Older adults with positive, neutral, and negative facial stimuli.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Mean accuracy for YA and OA responding to the SDRST with neutral, negative, and positive facial photographs. Error bars represent confidence intervals (95%). YA outperformed OA (p < 0.001). Performance with negative images was higher than with positive images (p = 0.002).

Discussion

In study 2, YA again performed better than OA, as expected. However, this time valence influenced memory accuracy, with negative faces eliciting a better performance than positive faces and neutral faces (marginally). This is called the Negativity Bias. It states that, due to their larger influence on the adaptive value of an individual, negative events are more efficiently remembered (Rozin and Royzman, 2001). Negative stimuli also attract more attention to their location and generate a more prompt behavioral response because they indicate places to be avoided, possibility of contamination, and imminent threats from others (reviewed by Palermo and Rhodes, 2007). Being in a negative emotional state has also been shown to improve cognition (Gray et al., 2002). Importantly, study 2 revealed no interaction between age and emotion, suggesting that negative emotion can benefit memory performance of both YA and OA, as found in previous studies (Gruhn et al., 2005; Thomas and Hasher, 2006; Foster et al., 2013). In other words, the results of the current study suggest that YA and OA did not differ in how their VSWM is influenced by the emotional valence displayed in facial stimuli.

The absence of the Positivity Effect in OA may have been due to the difficulty of the task. Two studies (Mather and Knight, 2005; Knight et al., 2007) presented participants with either full-attention tasks or divided-attention tasks and showed that, when the task is more cognitively demanding, the tendency of OA to favor positive over negative images is eliminated. It is also possible that the Positivity Effect is not as universal as originally thought, since the current study and others studies mentioned throughout this paper have failed to find it.

On a different note, Lewinski (2015) argues that humans only rate neutral facial expressions as neutral in 60% of the cases. This result does not suggest that the neutral faces used in the current study were inappropriate. Rather, it raises the possibility that the 30 pilot participants failed to recognize other faces as neutral. Future research may take advantage of automated facial coding software to aid in stimuli selection.

General Discussion: Iaps × Faces

The present research explored the responses of YA and OA performing a VSWM task that required the processing of emotional stimuli. The main goal was to investigate age-related differences in the emotional modulation of VSWM. Study 1 used positive, negative, and neutral IAPS pictures, whereas study 2 presented participants with happy, angry, and neutral facial expressions. As expected, in both studies YA showed higher accuracy than OA. Interestingly, however, only in study 2 emotion influenced memory, with negative faces leading to a better performance. The results were not influenced by whether participants were male or female, suggesting no sex-related differences in the relationship between VSWM and emotion.

A few possible explanations for the different results between the two studies can be raised, based on how faces differ from other visual stimuli. First, faces are one of the most important and straightforward ways of externalizing emotions (Hess et al., 1997). When directly compared with IAPS images, faces are less ambiguous and more familiar, which enhances the efficiency of their processing (Öhman et al., 2001; Britton et al., 2008; Ekman and Cordaro, 2011). Second, studies have demonstrated that faces are more quickly detected than other types of stimuli (Palermo and Rhodes, 2007), partially due to the Fusiform Face Area (Kanwisher et al., 1997). Liu et al. (2002) showed that a stimulus is categorized as a face in extricate areas of the brain no longer than 100 ms after the stimulus presentation, which does not happen for other images (e.g., houses). The fact that facial stimuli are more quickly identified and do not require additional cognitive load (Britton et al., 2008) may have allowed for a faster processing of the emotional information in those stimuli and, in consequence, strengthened their influence on VSWM when compared to IAPS images. Finally, IAPS images are more complex and more distinct between each other than faces. These characteristics induce a slower habituation to novelty and demand more sustained attention during the task (Knight et al., 2007; Britton et al., 2008). Previous research has shown that the effects of emotion on memory are reduced when participants are instructed to pay extra attention to stimuli (Talmi et al., 2008). Thus, the additional attentional resources required by the IAPS images may have reduced the influence of their emotional content on memory, when compared to emotional faces. All this evidence supports the conclusion that faces are more efficient in transmitting emotional valence and, thus, influencing working memory, than other types of stimuli such as contextual pictures.

Some limitations of the current study should be addressed. Our experiments were not designed to directly test the influence of the type of stimuli on memory and emotion interaction in OA. Study 1 was designed to investigate VSWM for emotional images and study 2 was developed in consequence of the results from the first study. Also, in study 1, the stimuli presentation times differed between the two age groups due to OA being slower in their movements and less familiar with the use of the computer. In study 2 we used the same stimulus duration for both age groups. Because of this, a direct comparison between IAPS and facial stimuli in the same study should be conducted to strengthen our conclusions. The facial stimuli used in our studies showed pictures of adult models, but not of OA. This means that our older participants were responding to out-group faces, which could lead to different cognitive processing (Samanez-Larkin and Carstensen, 2011). However, previous research has demonstrated that both YA and OA are faster and more accurate in identifying emotional expressions in younger than in older faces (Ebner et al., 2012). This finding, and the fact that we found no interactions between emotion and aging, supports the conclusion that the facial stimuli used here were adequate, though future research could directly repeat our experiments using older faces. It is important to highlight that the use of only anger as the negative emotional expression was dictated by the pilot study described in the “Materials and Methods” section of study 2. However, we are aware that the processing of negative valence may differ due to the specific emotion being tested (Phan et al., 2002). Thus, it is important that future studies repeat our experiments with a larger variety of facial expressions (e.g., sadness and disgust) within the same emotional valence.

Conclusion

Our findings suggest that the emotional modulation of VSWM is influenced by the type of information to be remembered. This effect seems to be present for YA and for OA. Taken together, our results contribute to the understanding of information processing in YA and OA and to the characterization of cognition across the human lifespan. This knowledge may explain the inconsistent literature findings about the interplay between memory, emotion, and aging, and lead to the development of better methodological approaches when studying these topics.

Author Contributions

FB, MT, CS, AG, and CT: designed study. FB, CS, and AG: collected data. FB, MT, CS, AG, and CT: analyzed data. FB, MT, CS, AG, RR, SC, and CT: interpreted data. FB, MT, CS, AG, RR, SC, and CT: wrote paper.

Funding

This work was partially supported by a FAPEMA grant to CT. FB and CS were recipients of Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq) scholarships when the studies were conducted. FB is now a recipient of a scholarship by the Capes Foundation within the Ministry of Education, Brazil (grant no. BEX 99999.006087/2013-02). MT is a fellowship recipient of CNPq (PQ 2, 2015, 311582/2015). SC is a recipient of a scholarship by CNPq.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

We thank Prof. Dr. Concepta Margareth MacManus Pimentel for the English revision of this manuscript.

References

Altamura, M., Padalino, F. A., Stella, E., Balzotti, A., Bellomo, A., Palumbo, R., et al. (2016). Facial emotion recognition in bipolar disorder and healthy aging. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 204, 188–193. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D., and Hitch, G. J. (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology 8, 485–493. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.485

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A.D., and Hitch, G. (1974). “Working memory,” in The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, ed. G. Bower (New York, NY: Academic Press).

Google Scholar

Belham, F. S., Satler, C., Garcia, A., Tomaz, C., Gasbarri, A., Rego, A., et al. (2013). Age-related differences in cortical activity during a visuo-spatial working memory task with facial stimuli. PLOS ONE 8:e75778. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bermudez, T., and Souza, A. S. (2016). Can emotional content reduce the age gap in visual working memory? Evidence from two tasks. Cogn. Emot. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1240066 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bopp, K. L., and Verhaeghen, P. (2007). Age-related differences in control processes in verbal and visuospatial working memory: storage, transformation, supervision, and coordination. J. Gerontol. 62, 239–246. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.5.P239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borella, E., Carretti, B., Grassi, M., Nucci, M., and Sciore, R. (2014). Are age-related differences between young and older adults in an affective working memory test sensitive to the music effects? Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:298. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouret, S., and Richmond, B. J. (2015). Sensitivity of locus ceruleus neurons to reward value for goal-directed actions. J. Neurosci. 35, 4005–4014. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4553-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Britton, J. C., Taylor, S. F., Sudheimer, K. D., and Liberzon, I. (2008). Facial expressions and complex IAPS pictures: common and differential networks. Neuroimage 31, 906–919. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., and Park, D. C. (2016). Cognitive Neuroscience of Aging: Linking Cognitive and Cerebral Aging. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., and Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: a theory of socioemotional selectivity. Am. Psychol. 54, 165–181. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carstensen, L. L., and Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the intersection of emotion and cognition: aging and the positivity effect. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 14, 117–121. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00348.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castel, A. D., Friedman, M. C., Mcgillivray, S., Flores, C. C., Murayama, K., Kerr, T., et al. (2016). I owe you: age-related similarities and differences in associative memory for gains and losses. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 23, 549–565. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2015.1130214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Denburg, N. L., Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., and Adolphs, R. (2003). Evidence for preserved emotional memory in normal older persons. Emotion 3, 239–253. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.3.239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebner, N., Johnson, M., and Fischer, H. (2012). Neural mechanisms of reading facial emotions in young and older adults. Front. Psychol. 3:223. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., and Cordaro, D. (2011). What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emot. Rev. 3, 364–370. doi: 10.1177/1754073911410740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fabiani, M., Zimmerman, B., and Gratton, G. (2015). Working memory and aging: a review. Mechanisms of Sensory Working Memory: Attention and Perfomance XXV, eds P. Jolicoeur, C. Lefebvre, and J. Marinez-Trujillo (ıAmsterdam: Elsevier), 131.

Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and Mchugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, S. M., Davis, H. P., and Kisley, M. A. (2013). Brain responses to emotional images related to cognitive ability in older adults. Psychol. Aging 28, 179–190. doi: 10.1037/a0030928

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galli, G., Wolpe, N., and Otten, L. J. (2011). Sex differences in the use of anticipatory brain activity to encode emotional events. J. Neurosci. 31, 12364–12370. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1619-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia, A., Uribe, C., Tavares, M. C. H., and Tomaz, C. (2011). EEG and autonomic responses during performance of matching and non-matching to sample working memory tasks with emotional content. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 5:82. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, J. R., Braver, T. S., and Raichle, M. E. (2002). Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 4115–4120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.062381899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruhn, D., Smith, J., and Baltes, P. B. (2005). No aging bias favoring memory for positive material: evidence from a heterogeneity-homogeneity list paradigm using emotionally toned words. Psychol. Aging 20, 579–588. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hale, S., Rose, N. S., Myerson, J., Strube, M. J., Sommers, M., Tye-Murray, N., et al. (2011). The structure of working memory abilities across the adult life span. Psychol. Aging 26, 92–110. doi: 10.1037/a0021483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hess, U., Blairy, S., and Kleck, R. E. (1997). The intensity of emotional facial expressions and decoding accuracy. J. Nonverbal Behav. 21, 241–257. doi: 10.1023/A:1024952730333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hori, E., Tazumi, T., Umeno, K., Kamachi, M., Kobayashi, T., Ono, T., et al. (2005). Effects of facial expression on shared attention mechanisms. Physiol. Behav. 84, 397–405. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaacowitz, D. M., Lockenhoff, C. E., Lane, R. D., Wright, R., Sechrest, L., Riedel, R., et al. (2007). Age differences in recognition of emotion in lexical stimuli and facial expressions. Psychol. Aging 22, 147–159. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.1.147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenkins, L., Myerson, J., Joerding, J. A., and Hale, S. (2000). Converging evidence that visuospatial cognition is more age-sensitive than verbal cognition. Psychol. Aging 15, 157–175. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.15.1.157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kan, I. P., Garrison, S. L., Drummey, A. B., Emmert, B. E. Jr., and Rogers, L. L. (2017). The roles of chronological age and time perspective in memory positivity. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2017.1356262 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanwisher, N., Mcdermott, J., and Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J. Neurosci. 17, 4302–4311.

Google Scholar

Kellough, J. L., and Knight, R. T. (2012). Positivity effects in older adults’ perception of facial emotion: the role of future time perspective. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 67, 150–158. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knight, M., Seymour, T., Gaunt, J., Baker, C., Nesmith, K., and Mather, M. (2007). Aging and goal-directed emotional attention: distraction reverses emotional biases. Emotion 7, 705–714. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacreuse, A., Kim, C. B., Rosene, D. L., Killiany, R. J., Moss, M. B., Moore, T. L., et al. (2005). Sex, age, and training modulate spatial memory in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta). Behav. Neurosci. 119, 118–126. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., and Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction Manual. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida.

Google Scholar

Lewinski, P. (2015). Automated facial coding software outperforms people in recognizing neutral faces as neutral from standardized datasets. Front. Psychol. 6:1386. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J., Harris, A., and Kanwisher, N. (2002). Stages of processing in face perception: an MEG study. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 910–916. doi: 10.1038/nn909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mammarella, N., Borella, E., Carretti, B., Leonardi, G., and Fairfield, B. (2013). Examining an emotion enhancement effect in working memory: evidence from age-related differences. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 23, 416–438. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2013.775065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mammarella, N., Di Domenico, A., Palumbo, R., and Fairfield, B. (2016a). Noradrenergic modulation of emotional memory in aging. Ageing Res. Rev. 27, 61–66. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2016.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mammarella, N., Di Domenico, A., Palumbo, R., and Fairfield, B. (2016b). When green is positive and red is negative: aging and the influence of color on emotional memories. Psychol. Aging 31, 914–926. doi: 10.1037/pag0000122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M., Clewett, D., Sakaki, M., and Harley, C. W. (2015). Norepinephrine ignites local hot spots of neuronal excitation: how arousal amplifies selectivity in perception and memory. Behav. Brain Sci. 39:e200. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X15000667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M., and Knight, M. (2005). Goal-directed memory: the role of cognitive control in older adults‘ emotional memory. Psychol. Aging 20, 554–570. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikels, J. A., Larkin, G. R., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., and Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Divergent trajectories in the aging mind, changes in working memory for affective versus visual information with age. Psychol. Aging 20, 542–553. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.4.542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myerson, J., Emery, L., White, D. A., and Hale, S. (2003). Effects of age, domain, and processing demands on memory span: evidence for differential decline. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 10, 20–27. doi: 10.1076/anec.10.1.20.13454

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nahm, F. K., Perret, A., Amaral, D. G., and Albright, T. D. (1997). How do monkeys look at faces? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 611–623. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Öhman, A., Lundqvist, D., and Esteves, F. (2001). The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80, 381–396. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orgeta, V., and Phillips, L. H. (2007). Effects of age and emotional intensity on the recognition of facial emotion. Exp. Aging Res. 34, 63–79. doi: 10.1080/03610730701762047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palermo, R., and Rhodes, G. (2007). Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact. Neuropsychologia 45, 75–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrican, R., Moscovitch, M., and Schimmack, U. (2008). Cognitive resources, valence, and memory retrieval of emotional events in older adults. Psychol. Aging 23, 585–594. doi: 10.1037/a0013176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phan, K. L., Wager, T., Taylor, S. F., and Liberzon, I. (2002). Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage 16, 331–348. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozin, P., and Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 5, 296–320. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samanez-Larkin, G.R., and Carstensen, L.L. (2011). “Socioemotional functioning and the aging brain,” in The Handbook of Social Neuroscience, eds J. Decety and J. T. Cacioppo (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 507–523.

Google Scholar

Satler, C., Belham, F. S., Garcia, A., Tomaz, C., and Tavares, M. C. H. (2015). Computerized spatial delayed recognition span task: a specific tool to assess visuospatial working memory. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:53. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satler, C., and Tomaz, C. (2011). Emotional working memory in Alzheimer’s Disease patients. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Dis. Extra 1, 124–138. doi: 10.1159/000329155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sava, A. -A., Krolak-Salmon, P., Delphin-Combe, F., Cloarec, M., and Chainay, H. (2017). Memory for faces with emotional expressions in Alzheimer’s disease and healthy older participants: positivity effect is not only due to familiarity. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 24, 1–28. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2016.1143444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheibe, S., and Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Emotional aging: recent findings and future trends. J. Gerontol. 65B, 135–144. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seals, D. R., and Esler, M. D. (2000). Human ageing and the sympathoadrenal system. J. Physiol. 528, 407–417. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.00407.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talmi, D., Anderson, A. K., Riggs, L., Caplan, J. B., and Moscovitch, M. (2008). Immediate memory consequences of the effect of emotion on attention to pictures. Learn. Mem. 15, 172–182. doi: 10.1101/lm.722908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, R. C., and Hasher, L. (2006). The influence of emotional valence on age differences in early processing and memory. Psychol. Aging 21, 821–825. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.4.821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Truong, L., and Yang, L. (2014). Friend or foe? Decoding the facilitative and disruptive effects of emotion on working memory in younger and older adults. Front. Psychol. 5:94. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tully, K., and Bolshakov, V. Y. (2010). Emotional enhancement of memory: how norepinephrine enables synaptic plasticity. Mol. Brain 3:15. doi: 10.1186/1756-6606-3-15

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yesavage, J. A., and Sheikh, J. I. (1986). Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS). Clin. Gerontol. 5, 1–2. doi: 10.1300/J018v05n01-09

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziaei, M., Salami, A., and Persson, J. (2017). Age-related alterations in functional connectivity patterns during working memory encoding of emotional items. Neuropsychologia 94, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziaei, M., Von Hippel, W., Henry, J. D., and Becker, S. I. (2015). Are age effects in positivity influenced by the valence of distractors? PLOS ONE 10:e0137604. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: aging, visuospatial working memory, emotion, facial stimuli, IAPS

Citation: Belham FS, Tavares MCH, Satler C, Garcia A, Rodrigues RC, Canabarro SLS and Tomaz C (2017) Negative Facial Expressions – But Not Visual Scenes – Enhance Human Working Memory in Younger and Older Participants. Front. Pharmacol. 8:668. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2017.00668

Received: 26 June 2017; Accepted: 07 September 2017;
Published: 26 September 2017.

Edited by:

Alfredo Meneses, Center for Research and Advanced Studies of the National Polytechnic Institute (CINVESTAV), Mexico

Reviewed by:

Rocco Palumbo, Harvard Medical School, United States
Assunta Pompili, University of L’Aquila, Italy
Peter Lewinski, Kozminski University, Poland

Copyright © 2017 Belham, Tavares, Satler, Garcia, Rodrigues, Canabarro and Tomaz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carlos Tomaz, ctomaz@ceuma.br

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.