Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Physiol., 13 May 2013
Sec. Exercise Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Investigating the human brain and muscle coupling during whole-body challenging exercise View all 8 articles

Fatigue is a pain—the use of novel neurophysiological techniques to understand the fatigue-pain relationship

  • Endurance Research Group, School of Sport and Exercise Sciences, University of Kent, Chatham, Kent, UK

Fatigue can be defined as any exercise-induced reduction in the ability to exert muscle force or power, regardless of whether or not the task can be sustained (Gandevia, 2001). There is no doubt that in sports performance where time to completion is the outcome measure, the management of fatigue is probably the determining factor of success. However, in exercise physiology, interventions and mechanisms have traditionally be measured using time to exhaustion tasks, where “the point of fatigue” occurs at task failure. The problem with this approach is that in sports performance, tasks are self-paced, and therefore in a successful performance, task failure (and thus the traditional concept of fatigue) never actually occurs. Therefore, which mechanisms govern fatigue in task failure, and which mechanisms govern work rate regulation (and thus the management of the process of fatigue) are very different constructs, which should not be used synonymously.

Whilst fatigue has previously been explained from a peripheral perspective (Kent-Braun, 1999), this standpoint is largely based on fatigue at task failure. More recently, greater focus has been placed on central mechanisms of fatigue (Gandevia, 2001; Noakes, 2012), so that now the role of the brain as a contributor to fatigue (at least in self-paced performance) is largely uncontested. This has led to the suggestion that perceived exertion is primarily derived from central command (Marcora, 2009), and conversely that somatosensory feedback from locomotor muscles is the major cause of fatigue (Amann and Secher, 2010). However, it is more likely that fatigue (in terms of endurance performance), is balanced through an interaction of these efferent and afferent systems (Perrey, 2010). By attributing a more “intelligent” role for the brain, Noakes' (2012) theory of central control suggests that afferent feedback from the periphery is collated and processed in conjunction with past experience and current knowledge of the external environment to produce a “sensation” of fatigue on which the level of motor output is based. The conjecture that fatigue is a sensation is probably not compatible with its role in task failure, and is therefore a highly disputed concept. However, that decision-making during (and before) exercise is based on the knowledge of the task and the self (i.e., pacing), and that this is a major factor in endurance performance (Mauger et al., 2009), is more widely accepted. Therefore, the variables which are used in this decision-making process (i.e., to up- or down-regulate work rate) are of considerable interest to the physiologist and psychologist, as the moderation of these has the potential to improve athletic performance.

There are numerous studies which have provided insight into which factors influence decision-making in exercise performance, including but not limited to; distance knowledge (Mauger et al., 2009), prior experience (Mauger et al., 2009), distance feedback (Albertus et al., 2005; Mauger et al., 2010b), performance feedback (Mauger et al., 2011), knowledge of ambient conditions (Castle et al., 2012), knowledge of the physiological state of the body (Tucker et al., 2004; Rauch et al., 2005; Noakes and Marino, 2007), external competition (Corbett et al., 2012) and motivation (Cabanac, 1986). Whilst these studies provide a useful insight into what information is required to successfully regulate work rate and which aspects may improve this process, many of these variables are either inherently present (or not) during competition, or are very difficult to manage. However, one variable which is frequently referred to by athletes, coaches and commentators, but has received peculiarly little focus in research, is exercise-induced pain. Indeed, pain during exercise is popularly referred to as a major inhibiting factor during exercise, and it is suggested that athletes who are better able to tolerate or overcome pain will be more successful. As exercise-induced pain is present in nearly all forms of time-dependent competition, the requirement for understanding its influence on performance and how athletes respond to its presence is imperative. The International Association for the Study of Pain define pain as an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or described in terms of such damage. This implies that pain is always subjective, has an emotional element and that this is not always directly related to the magnitude of the nociceptive signal. Therefore, the nature of the athlete's perception of the nociceptive signal is a key element to the experience of pain, which provides a promising basis for effective intervention and subsequent performance improvement.

During high intensity exercise, Type III and IV nociceptors, are stimulated by mechanical pressure, heat, cold, noxious pressure, and endogenous pain producing (algesic) substances. These contribute to the acute muscle pain associated with particular forms of exercise (O'Connor and Cook, 1999), which is ultimately interpreted in the brain and perceived as exercise-induced pain. Given that the conscious awareness of the self is an important facet in pacing based decisions in exercise, it is logical that exercise-induced pain may provide an individual with useful information regarding the relative “strain” on the body, and thus use this to inform a conscious decision to increase or decrease exercise intensity (Mauger et al., 2010a). If this is the case, then exercise-induced pain may be a likely contender contributing to “fatigue” (or the management of fatigue) in exercise performance.

Despite this apparent link, limited research has been conducted on the fatiguing effects of exercise-induced pain (Mauger et al., 2010a; Mauger and Hopker, 2012). Those studies which have investigated the role of pain on exercise have produced equivocal results —Khan et al. (2011) demonstrated that deep muscle pain, elicited by the injection of hypertonic saline, reduced maximal voluntary torque (~5%) but had no effect of voluntary activation or submaximal torque output. Conversely, Hollander et al. (2008) found that partial occlusion increased pain and rating of perceived exertion (RPE) for a given exercise intensity. Ray and Carter (2007) and Hudson et al. (2008) found that the analgesics codeine and aspirin did not effect performance of fixed intensity exercise, whilst Mauger et al. (2010a) found that acetaminophen significantly improved self-paced exercise performance. Additionally, the competitive opioid naloxone appears to increase RPE and reduce time to exhaustion (Sgherza et al., 2002) and chronic pain affects spatial perceptions in a similar manner to fatigue, so that target distances appear further away than they really are (Witt et al., 2009). Of these studies, only one (Mauger et al., 2010a) study provides insight into the role of pain in self-paced performance, where the decision to up- or down-regulate work rate is likely to be most strongly influenced by pain perception. Mauger et al. (2010a), found that 1.5 g of acetaminophen allowed well-trained cyclists to produce a higher power output for a given perceived pain and RPE, which resulted in a significantly faster time to complete a 16.1 km cycle time trial. The authors concluded that participants were willing to engage in a given level of pain, and that they regulated this by moderating their power output.

Studies investigating the influence of exercise-induced pain on performance can be approached by either increasing or decreasing the pain experienced during exercise. Perhaps part of the difficulty in designing and conducting these type of studies is finding interventions which accurately replicate the type of pain experienced during intense physical exercise, or that moderate pain without effecting other physiological functions which may also effect exercise capacity. Techniques to induce pain may range from cold or heat, direct electric current, tonically induced pain, mechanical pain and topically induced pain [for more detail on these methods, the Reader is referred to the excellent review by Staahl and Drewes (2004)]. Understanding the pain pathways involved with each of these techniques is important because as type III and IV afferents are not only involved in transmitting a nociceptive signal, but are also involved in moderating the exercise pressor reflex, it is difficult to attribute changes in performance to differences in pain perception or cardiovascular control—increased pain may stimulate heart rate and minute ventilation for example. Conversely, reducing pain during exercise creates its own inherent problems and confounding variables which may alter exercise capacity through mechanisms other than analgesia [e.g., epidural–motor output (Amann et al., 2009); fentanyl—exercise pressor response (Amann et al., 2008); acetaminophen—cortico-spinal excitability (Mauger and Hopker, 2013); aspirin—coagulation, inflammation (Hudson et al., 2008)]. These issues are further compounded by the fact that techniques used for brain imaging [functional magnetic resonance imaging, electroencephalography (EEG), positron emission tomography, functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS)] present several issues both with data acquisition during exercise, and differentiating differences in brain activity between exercise and the consequential pain. Accordingly, novel physiological techniques which are capable of inducing pain altering effects, and monitoring other indirect responses are of particular interest to the exercise scientist.

As the sensory and emotional aspects of pain ensure that it is ultimately a subjective experience (Rainville, 2002) that may be modulated via a number of factors outside of the internal transmission system, modulation of pain through brain based interventions are particularly attractive. Indeed, whilst the sensory aspects of pain are transmitted to the somatosensory (VPL, VPM) thalamic nuclei to cortical areas (S1, S2 and the posterior parietal cortex), and subsequently, from these areas to cortical limbic structures (insular cortex, anterior cingulate cortex), the emotional aspects of pain primarily involve the limbic system (Rainville, 2002). As such, the location of the areas where the brain processes the sensory and affective dimensions of pain are anatomically distinct, which means that the targeting of these areas to specifically moderate pain may provide an effective intervention.

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a non-invasive, transient type of neuro-stimulation which has successfully been used to treat stoke patients (Norris et al., 2010), depression (Boggio et al., 2007) and improve brain function (Marshall et al., 2004). This technique has also been shown to reduce both chronic and acute pain in healthy individuals and clinical populations (Boggio et al., 2008). Indeed, the use of tDCS to moderate pain appears to be particularly successful (Mylius et al., 2012b; Reidler et al., 2012), and can even be specifically targeted to brain regions so that modulation of emotional (Boggio et al., 2009) and sensory pain (Boggio et al., 2008) can be distinguished. The non-invasive, transient and target specific nature of a tDCS intervention provides a unique and exciting methodology by which the role of pain on exercise performance can be robustly tested. Some exercise physiology research groups have already started using this technique to investigate its potential to improve performance (Cogiamanian et al., 2007; Tanaka et al., 2009; Okano et al., 2013). Whilst improvements in exercise capacity or muscular force have been demonstrated, the limited experimental design of these studies has allowed little insight into the mechanisms underpinning the apparent effect, nor have the studies used a self-paced model of exercise performance. Whilst the effect of tDCS on specific neural mechanisms may be difficult to measure, relatively simple measures such as manipulation checks on pain response (i.e., pain threshold tests) motivation (motivation assessed questionnaires) and parasympathetic activity (resting heart rate, blood pressure and gas analysis) would significantly strengthen experimental design. Additionally, where laboratories and research groups have access to more specialized equipment, further mechanistic measures should also be sought to be acquired. As tDCS has been shown to increase brain blood flow (Zheng et al., 2011) and increase cortical excitability (Nitsche and Paulus, 2001), fNIRS and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) could be used to monitor these potential neurophysiological responses. Furthermore, whilst tDCS can be applied at rest, EEG may be used to identify whether activity has increased in the desired (or an undesired) region. In terms of using a tDCS intervention, researchers should be aware that there are several different tDCS set-ups (e.g., changing anodal or cathodal stimulation, brain area targeted and stimulation amplitude) which can be used to bring about different effects [for an excellent review the Reader is referred to Mylius et al. (2012a)]. Whilst anodal stimulation is generally accepted to increase excitability of the targeted region, and cathodal stimulation to decrease excitability (Nitsche and Paulus, 2001), the analgesic effects of these respective set-ups may be different depending on the brain region targeted. Stimulation (both anodal and cathodal) of the motor cortex (M1) is more consistent with increasing pain threshold (Boggio et al., 2008), whereas stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) appears to preferentially increase pain tolerance (Boggio et al., 2009). There is no agreed set-up for which to decrease exercise-induced pain, although it is likely that either (or both) anodal stimulation of the M1 and DLPFC will reduce the pain felt during intense exercise.

Thus, it appears that whilst no single technique may provide a “magic bullet” to examine the role of the brain (and exercise-induced pain) in exercise performance, a range of complimentary and novel techniques could be used to directly test a given mechanism (e.g., exercise-induced pain) in a reproducible and controlled manner. The challenge for research groups is now to forge collaborations of expertise, so that as many potential mechanistic explanations for an observed effect can be accounted for. With this in mind, perhaps well-known expressions such as “the man who can drive himself further once the effort gets painful is the man who will win,” can be accurately and robustly tested.

References

Albertus, Y., Tucker, R., St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, E. V., Hampson, D. B., and Noakes, T. D. (2005). Effect of distance feedback on pacing strategy and perceived exertion during cycling. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 37, 461–468.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Amann, M., Proctor, L. T., Sebranek, J. J., Eldridge, M. W., Pegelow, D. F., and Dempsey, J. A. (2008). Somatosensory feedback from the limbs exerts inhibitory influences on central neural drive during whole body endurance exercise. J. Appl. Physiol. 105, 1714–1724.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amann, M., Proctor, L. T., Sebranek, J. J., Pegelow, D. F., and Dempsey, J. A. (2009). Opioid-mediated muscle afferents inhibit central mo-tor drive and limit peripheral muscle fatigue development in humans. J. Physiol. 15, 271–283.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amann, M., and Secher, N. H. (2010). Point: afferent feedback from fatigued locomotor muscles is an important determinant of endurance exercise performance. J. Appl. Physiol. 108, 452–454.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boggio, P. S., Bermpohl, F., Vergara, A. O., Muniz, A. L., Nahas, F. H., Leme, P. B., et al. (2007). Go-no-go task performance improvement after anodal transcranial DC stimulation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 101, 91–98.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boggio, P. S., Zaghi, S., and Fregni, F. (2009). Modulation of emotions associated with images of human pain using anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuropsychologia 47, 212–217.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boggio, P. S., Zaghi, S., Lopes, M., and Fregni, F. (2008). Modulatory effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation on perception and pain thresholds in healthy volunteers. Eur. J. Neurol. 15, 1124–1130.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cabanac, M. (1986). Money versus pain: experimental study of a conflict in humans. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 46, 37–44.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Castle, P. C., Maxwell, N., Allchorn, A., Mauger, A. R., and White, D. K. (2012). Deception of ambient and body core temperature improves self paced cycling in hot, humid conditions. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 112, 377–385.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cogiamanian, F., Marceglia, S., Ardolino, G., Barbieri, S., and Priori, A. (2007). Improved isometric force endurance after transcranial direct current stimulation over the human motor cortical areas. Eur. J. Neurosci. 26, 242–249.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Corbett, J., Barwood, M. J., Ouzounoglou, A., Thelwell, R., and Dicks, M. (2012). Influence of competition on performance and pacing during cycling exercise. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 44, 509–515.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gandevia, S. C. (2001). Spinal and supraspinal factors in human muscle fatigue. Physiol. Rev. 81, 1725–1789.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hollander, D. B., Reeves, G. V., Clavier, J. D., Francois, M. R., Thomas, C., and Kraemer, R. R. (2008). Partial occlusion during resistance exercise alters effort sense and pain. J. Strength Cond. Res. 24, 235–243.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hudson, G. M., Green, J. M., Bishop, P. A., and Richardson, M. T. (2008). Effects of caffeine and aspirin on light resistance training performance, perceived exertion, and pain perception. J. Strength Cond. Res. 22, 1950–1957.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kent-Braun, J. A. (1999). Central and peripheral contributions to muscle fatigue in humans during sustained maximal effort. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 80, 57–63.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Khan, S. I., McNeil, C. J., Gandevia, S. C., and Taylor, J. L. (2011). Effect of experimental muscle pain on maximal voluntary activation of human biceps brachii muscle. J. Appl. Physiol. 111, 743–750.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marcora, S. (2009). Perception of effort during exercise is independent of afferent feedback from skeletal muscles, heart, and lungs. J. Appl. Physiol. 106, 2060–2062.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marshall, L., Mölle, M., Hallschmid, M., and Born, J. (2004). Transcranial direct current stimulation during sleep improves declarative memory. J. Neurosci. 3, 9985–9992.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mauger, A., and Hopker, J. (2012). The pain of exercise - insights in to the role of pain on exercise regulation and performance. J. Sports Med. Doping Stud. 2, 1–2.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Mauger, A. R., and Hopker, J. G. (2013). The effect of acetaminophen ingestion on cortico-spinal excitability. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 91, 187–189.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mauger, A. R., Jones, A. M., and Williams, C. A. (2009). Influence of feedback and prior experience on pacing during a 4-km cycle time trial. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 41, 451–458.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mauger, A. R., Jones, A. M., and Williams, C. A. (2010a). Influence of acetaminophen on performance during time trial cycling. J. Appl. Physiol. 108, 98–104.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mauger, A. R., Jones, A. M., and Williams, C. A. (2010b). Influence of exercise variation on the retention of a pacing strategy. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 108, 1015–1023.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mauger, A. R., Jones, A. M., and Williams, C. A. (2011). The effect of non-contingent and accurate performance feedback on pacing and time trial performance in 4-km track cycling. Br. J. Sports Med. 45, 225–229.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mylius, V., Borckardt, J. J., and Lefaucheur, J. P. (2012a). Noninvasive cortical modulation of experimental pain. Pain 153, 1350–1363.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mylius, V., Jung, M., Menzler, K., Haag, A., Khader, P. H., Oertel, W. H., et al. (2012b). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on pain perception and working memory. Eur. J. Pain 16, 974–982.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2001). Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology 57, 1899–1901.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Noakes, T. D. (2012). Fatigue is a brain-derived emotion that regulates the exercise behavior to ensure the protection of whole body homeostasis. Front. Physiol. 3:82. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2012.00082

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Noakes, T. D., and Marino, F. E. (2007). Arterial oxygenation, central motor output and exercise performance in humans. J. Physiol. 585(Pt 3), 919–921.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Norris, S., Degabriele, R., and Lagopoulos, J. (2010). Recommendations for the use of tDCS in clinical research. Acta Neuropsychiatrica 22, 197–198.

O'Connor, P. J., and Cook, D. B. (1999). Exercise and pain: the neurobiology, measurement, and laboratory study of pain in relation to exercise in humans. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 27, 119–166.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Okano, A. H., Fontes, E. B., Montenegro, R. A., Farinatti, P. D., Cyrino, E. S., Li, L. M., et al. (2013). Brain stimulation modulates the autonomic nervous system, rating of perceived exertion and performance during maximal exercise. Br. J. Sports Med. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2012-091658. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Perrey, S. (2010). Comments on point: counterpoint: afferent feedback from fatigued locomotor muscles is/is not an important determinant of endurance exercise performance: predominance of central motor command in the regulation of exercise. J. Appl. Physiol. 108, 458–468.

Rainville, P. (2002). Brain mechanisms of pain affect and pain modulation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12, 195–204.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ray, C. A., and Carter, J. R. (2007). Central modulation of exercise-induced muscle pain in humans. J. Physiol. 15, 287–294.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rauch, H. G., St Clair Gibson, A., Lambert, E. V., and Noakes, T. D. (2005). A signalling role for muscle glycogen in the regulation of pace during prolonged exercise. Br. J. Sports Med. 39, 34–38.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Reidler, J. S., Mendonca, M. E., Santana, M. B., Wang, X., Lenkinski, R., Motta, A. F., et al. (2012). Effects of motor cortex modulation and descending inhibitory systems on pain thresholds in healthy subjects. J. Pain. 13, 450–458.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sgherza, A. L., Axen, K., Fain, R., Hoffman, R. S., Dunbar, C. C., and Haas, F. (2002). Effect of naloxone on perceived exertion and exercise capacity during maximal cycle ergometry. J. Appl. Physiol. 93, 2023–2028.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Staahl, C., and Drewes, A. M. (2004). Experimental human pain models: a review of standardised methods for preclinical testing of analgesics. Basic Clin. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 95, 97–111.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tanaka, S., Hanakawa, T., Honda, M., and Watanabe, K. (2009). Enhancement of pinch force in the lower leg by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation. Exp. Brain Res. 196, 459–465.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tucker, R., Rauch, L., Harley, Y. X., and Noakes, T. D. (2004). Impaired exercise performance in the heat is associated with an anticipatory reduction in skeletal muscle recruitment. Pflugers Arch. 448, 422–430.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Witt, J. K., Linkenauger, S. A., Bakdash, J. Z., Augustyn, J. S., Cook, A., and Proffitt, D. R. (2009). The long road of pain: chronic pain increases perceived distance. Exp. Brain Res. 192, 145–148.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zheng, X., Alsop, D. C., and Schlaug, G. (2011). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on human regional cerebral blood flow. Neuroimage 58, 26–33.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Mauger AR (2013) Fatigue is a pain—the use of novel neurophysiological techniques to understand the fatigue-pain relationship. Front. Physiol. 4:104. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2013.00104

Received: 18 March 2013; Accepted: 24 April 2013;
Published online: 13 May 2013.

Edited by:

Stephane Perrey, Montpellier I University, France

Copyright © 2013 Mauger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: lex.mauger@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.