Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Physiol., 31 October 2013
Sec. Exercise Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Investigating the human brain and muscle coupling during whole-body challenging exercise View all 8 articles

The evolutionary significance of fatigue

  • 1Departamento de Educação Física, Universidade Católica de Brasília, Taguatinga, Brazil
  • 2Departamento de Educação Física, Universidade Estadual de Londrina, Londrina, Brazil

The concept of fatigue has inspired a wide body of research since the earliest studies in muscular and exercise physiology (Noakes, 2012). During the last decades, intense debates (Amann and Secher, 2010) have been conducted among sport and exercise physiologists in an attempt to clarify the mechanisms that regulate fatigue. It is interesting to note that various models have been proposed for its understanding along with the vast number of studies generated in this area (Abbiss and Laursen, 2005; Amann and Dempsey, 2008; Millet, 2011). Given this important lack of scientific consensus, it is therefore pertinent to clarify if we are thinking about the complex phenomenon of fatigue in the right way. From a historical perspective, the whole picture of fatigue research would evoke the old Indian tale of the blind men and the elephant. Briefly, this parable described several blind men touching an elephant to figure out what it looked like with a subsequent discussion on the nature of the elephant from their own perspectives. Every man touched only a single part of the elephant's body, leading each of them to guess differently about the object's features. Similarly, fatigue researchers have strongly defended different theories without assuming that they could only be aware of a limited piece of the whole fatigue phenomenon. Thus, the absence of agreement between fatigue specialists could have its origin with the technological dependence of fatigue research. That is, the epistemological approximation to fatigue has been more inductive than deductive as laboratory facilities with restricted findings (e.g., maximum oxygen uptake; VO2max) dictated the elaboration of further explanatory theories (e.g., Anaerobic/Cardiovascular Model). In this regard, some caution should be considered with the new technological advances in brain and muscle imaging as these could be performed under the same erroneous process. This consideration is very important given that new technologies are also capable of possessing important methodological limitations under exercise conditions that may limit our understanding of fatigue. Although it seems that we will see a new era in fatigue research, the risk of elaborating on biased knowledge because of technology limitations should not be ignored.

A new revolution in this area could start if the scientific process was more deductive. For example, a recent article has presented an evolutionary explanation for the greater effectiveness of different training and nutritional strategies (Boullosa et al., 2013). This line of reasoning has been inspired by previous works from evolutionary medicine based on the assumption that nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution (Dobzhansky, 1973). That is, all biological phenomena are the consequences of millions of years of evolution through the action of natural selection (Lieberman, 2012). Therefore, as a biological phenomenon, fatigue should not be excluded from the influence of natural selection. The questions that arise include: how did fatigue evolve across evolutionary processes and what its evolutionary significance is? Consequently, it could be hypothesized that the protective role that fatigue exerts, developed to avoid a catastrophic failure of the organism. This assumption is one of the key points of the Central Governor Model (CGM) (Noakes, 2012). However, individuals are able to go beyond their body limits, despite fatigue symptoms, finally achieving the catastrophic failure (i.e., death) (Noakes, 2007). This apparent paradox could be overcome with the consideration of the selfish gene hypothesis (Dawkins, 1976). This hypothesis states that active replicators (i.e., genes), which are programmed to make copies of themselves, modulate the behavior of their carrier organism. The evolutionary significance of fatigue could be understood when assuming that its role on exercise performance (i.e., organism behavior) should be linked to the genetic success of the carrier organism (e.g., athlete). This assumption is critical and has important implications for designing appropriate studies in which the athlete is highly motivated during exercise under real conditions to better analyze the influence of fatigue on performance. However, it is well known that a more ecological approach limits the applicability of sophisticated and technological instruments. Moreover, it should be pointed out that true maximal efforts during exhaustive exercises are more suitable under real than under laboratory conditions as inferred from recorded maximum heart rate values in both conditions (Semin et al., 2008). Furthermore, an incremental test until exhaustion, which is a common protocol in fatigue evaluations, may have no equivalent in the evolutionary process of our species. These considerations raise important concerns regarding the validity of the common protocols in fatigue evaluation. This problem could be overcome in future studies with sufficient ecological validity while allowing the recording of valid data with new imaging technologies that should be interpreted at a translational level of physiology (Heck et al., 2011).

There are several findings that could reinforce the guiding role of this evolutionary approach in future fatigue research. For instance, it has been proposed that our ancestors predominantly performed physical activities at low intensities, mainly below lactate threshold (LT) (Boullosa et al., 2013). This concept supports the validity of polarized training that is widely utilized by modern endurance athletes and is characterized by keeping ’easy training easy, and hard training hard' (Muñoz et al., 2013). Within this concept, the main factor regulating the level of physical activity of our ancestors is proposed to be perception of effort (Smirmaul, 2012). Perception of effort increases in response to increased motor commands and cognitive effort related to maintenance of muscle contractions (Nakamura et al., 2008; Marcora et al., 2009). Locomotor muscle fatigue also increases perception of effort and reduces exercise tolerance (Marcora et al., 2008). As increased perception of effort is often accompanied by muscle contraction-induced discomfort and pain, it can inhibit engagement in purposeless behaviors which could undesirably lead to an increase in energy expenditure at a time when periods of food shortage were common. In other words, increased perception of effort during inefficient physical activities would be a favored phenotypic trait that evolved under scarce caloric sources, as energy efficiency is thought to be an important evolutionary pressure (Niven and Laughlin, 2008). It has to be pointed out that this view does not indicate that perception of effort has an anticipatory or template role that regulates exercise duration by means of subconscious control of motor commands (Swart et al., 2012). There is evidence supporting the more biologically plausible model that exercise intolerance is a cognitive event related to task disengagement due to attainment of maximal levels of perception of effort in highly motivated people (Marcora and Staiano, 2010). Subsequently, it is reasonable to imply that the modern athlete chooses to engage in activities below the LT most of the time as rating of perceived exertion (RPE) is maintained at tolerable and relatively comfortable values at this exercise level in an efficient manner in preparation for any following intense sessions. It is costly from a perceptual perspective to keep going for long periods of time on a daily basis above LT (Seiler and Tønnessen, 2009). Therefore, fatigue is a relevant issue that does change perception of effort and subsequent behavior in accordance with natural selection. This evidence could help to better understand the role of evolutionary concepts incorporated within this new era of fatigue research. More specifically, it would be pertinent to examine peripheral and central adaptations of athletes enrolled in long-lasting training regimes that consider this phylogenetic template (Boullosa et al., 2013). Greater adaptations and more efficient responses during maximal and submaximal exercises may become evident with the use of new imaging techniques in athletes when training within this concept.

Acknowledgments

We thank Laurinda Abreu for her suggestions and Dr. Anthony Leicht for the English revision.

References

Abbiss, C. R., and Laursen, P. B. (2005). Models to explain fatigue during prolonged endurance cycling. Sports Med. 35, 865–898. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200535100-00004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amann, M., and Dempsey, J. A. (2008). Locomotor muscle fatigue modifies central motor drive in healthy humans and imposes a limitation to exercise performance. J. Physiol. 586, 161–173. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.141838

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Amann, M., and Secher, N. H. (2010). Point: afferent feedback from fatigued locomotor muscles is an important determinant of endurance exercise performance. J. Appl. Physiol. 108, 452–454. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00976.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boullosa, D. A., Abreu, L., Varela-Sanz, A., and Mujika, I. (2013). Do olympic athletes train as in the paleolithic era. Sports Med. 43, 909–917. doi: 10.1007/s40279-013-0086-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Dobzhansky, T. (1973). Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution. Am. Biol. Teach. 35, 125–129. doi: 10.2307/4444260

CrossRef Full Text

Heck, T. G., Scholer, C. M., and de Bittencourt, P. I. (2011). HSP70 expression: does it a novel fatigue signalling factor from immune system to the brain. Cell Bichem. Funct. 29, 215–226. doi: 10.1002/cbf.1739

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lieberman, D. E. (2012). What we can learn about running from barefoot running: an evolutionary medical perspective. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 40, 63–72. doi: 10.1097/JES.0b013e31824ab210

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marcora, S. M., Bosio, A., and de Morree, H. M. (2008). Locomotor muscle fatigue increases cardiorespiratory responses and reduces performance during intense cycling exercise independently from metabolic stress. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 294, R874–R883. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00678.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marcora, S. M., and Staiano, W. (2010). The limit to exercise tolerance in humans: mind over muscle. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 109, 763–770. doi: 10.1007/s00421-010-1418-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marcora, S. M., Staiano, W., and Manning, V. (2009). Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. J. Appl. Physiol. 106, 857–864. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.91324.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Millet, G. Y. (2011). Can neuromuscular fatigue explain running strategies and performance in ultra-marathons?: the flush model. Sports Med. 41, 489–506. doi: 10.2165/11588760-000000000-00000

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Muñoz, I., Seiler, S., Bautista, J., España, J., Larumbe, E., and Esteve-Lanao, J. (2013). Does polarized training improve performance in recreational runners. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Nakamura, F. Y., Okuno, N. M., Perandini, L. A., Soares-Caldeira, L. F., Simões, H. G., Cardoso, J. R., et al. (2008). Critical power can be estimated from nonexhaustive tests based on rating of perceived exertion. J. Strength Cond. Res. 22, 937–943. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0b013e31816a41fa

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Niven, J. E., and Laughlin, S. B. (2008). Energy limitation as a selective pressure on the evolution of sensory systems. J. Exp. Biol. 211(Pt 11), 1792–1804. doi: 10.1242/jeb.017574

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Noakes, T. D. (2007). The limits of human endurance: what is the greatest endurance performance of all time. Which factors regulate performance at extreme altitude? Adv. Exp. Med. Biol. 618, 255–276. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-75434-5_20

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Noakes, T. D. (2012). Fatigue is a brain-derived emotion that regulates the exercise behavior to ensure the protection of whole body homeostasis. Front. Physiol. 3:82. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2012.00082

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Seiler, S., and Tønnessen, E. (2009). Intervals, thresholds, and long slow distance: the role of intensity and duration in endurance training. Sportsci. 13, 32–53. Available online at: http://www.sportsci.org/2009/ss.htm

Semin, K., Stahlnecker, A. C., Heelan, K., Brown, G. A., Shaw, B. S., and Shaw, I. (2008). Discrepancy between training, competition and laboratory measures of maximum heart rate in NCAA division 2 distance runners. J. Sports Sci. Med. 7, 455–460. Available online at: http://www.jssm.org/vol7/n4/5/v7n4-5text.php

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Smirmaul, Bde P. (2012). Sense of effort and other unpleasant sensations during exercise: clarifying concepts and mechanisms. Br. J. Sports Med. 46, 308–311. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2010.071407

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Swart, J., Lindsay, T. R., Lambert, M. I., Brown, J. C., and Noakes, T. D. (2012). Perceptual cues in the regulation of exercise performance—physical sensantions of exercise and awareness of effort interact as separate cues. Br. J. Sports Med. 46, 42–48. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2011-090337

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: perceived exertion, evolutionary biology, exercise physiology, human performance, Philosophy of Science

Citation: Boullosa DA and Nakamura FY (2013) The evolutionary significance of fatigue. Front. Physiol. 4:309. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2013.00309

Received: 30 September 2013; Accepted: 08 October 2013;
Published online: 31 October 2013.

Edited by:

Stephane Perrey, Montpellier I University, France

Reviewed by:

Stephane Perrey, Montpellier I University, France

Copyright © 2013 Boullosa and Nakamura. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: d_boullosa@yahoo.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.