Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 18 December 2012
Sec. Social and Evolutionary Neuroscience

Is psychopathy a disorder or an adaptation?


Liane J. Leedom* Liane J. Leedom1*Linda Hartoonian Almas Linda Hartoonian Almas2
  • 1University of Bridgeport, Bridgeport, CT, USA
  • 2Aftermath: Surviving Psychopathy Foundation, Yorkville, IL, USA

In a recent article in Frontiers in Evolutionary Psychology, Krupp et al. (2012) propose that (1) psychopathy is associated with nepotism and therefore (2) psychopathy is not a disorder but an evolutionarily based life strategy. In this commentary, we will address these two points, and outline an alternative evolutionary theory.

Is Psychopathy Associated with Nepotism?

Krupp et al. (2012) state, “individuals executing well-designed strategies, a necessary feature of psychological adaptations, should tend to be nepotistic—providing aid to close genealogical kin and/or sparing them from harm (p. 2).” They then present data linking psychopathy to index offenses involving violence toward non-relatives as opposed to relatives. Total Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) scores were positively correlated with victimization of non-relatives. These findings agree with prior studies linking psychopathy to violent victimization (Coid and Yang, 2011). The idea that psychopathic individuals might not aggress against kin has also been advanced by Coyne and Thomas (2008), “According to evolutionary theory … psychopaths would be more likely to use such (aggressive) behavior against strangers, associates, or colleagues, as compared with romantic relationships or family members (p. 1113).” We assert: (1) the finding that violent psychopathic offenders are more frequently imprisoned for stranger victimization does not necessarily imply they do not also victimize kin in a way that would “impair fitness,” (2) most psychopathic individuals are not violent and so non-violent victimization is perhaps more important in this group, and (3) even if present, lack of violent harm does not equate to nepotistic help.

We applaud these authors for raising the issue of the treatment of relatives by psychopathic individuals as it is commonly asserted that offenders do better when they have supportive family ties (Andrews and Bonta, 2003). This assertion has caused mental health and criminal justice professionals to encourage family ties (Rotgers and Maniacci, 2005). There is little data regarding the impact of offenders on the family members who are asked to act as supports; if psychopathy is associated with harm to family members, it may not be ethical to encourage family ties. It is difficult to study the impact of psychopathy on the family because of ethical constraints on research involving prisoners and lack of access to psychopathic individuals in the community (Widom, 1977). A review of Cleckley's cases (1964), surveys of people who claim relationships with psychopathic individuals (Leedom and Andersen, 2011), and qualitative analyses of memoirs written by the wives and adult sons and daughters of highly psychopathic individuals (Leedom et al., 2012, submitted) indicate that family members report considerable harm including psychological, emotional, financial and physical abuse, and exploitation. Sons and daughters also report being encouraged to engage in antisocial behaviors. These studies do not however provide data about the rate of harm because the subjects are self-selected for the occurrence of harm. Controlled studies comparing psychopathic individuals in the community to matched controls are needed to prove hypotheses regarding harm or aid to family members. Interestingly, the memoire studies do suggest that psychopathic individuals will aid family members if they perceive a benefit to themselves for doing so (Leedom et al., 2012, submitted). Also reported was nepotistic assistance to highly psychopathic individuals by less psychopathic kin. Psychopathy may persist in human populations in part because of kin support to (not from) psychopathic individuals.

Is Psychopathy a Mental Disorder?

The paper cites Wakefield (1992),

I propose a hybrid account of disorder as harmful dysfunction, wherein dysfunction is a scientific and factual term based in evolutionary biology that refers to the failure of an internal mechanism to perform a natural function for which it was designed, and harmful is a value term referring to the consequences that occur to the person because of the dysfunction and are deemed negative by sociocultural standards (p. 374).

According to one prevailing theory of psychopathy, the integrated emotions systems (IES) model (Blair et al., 2005), psychopathic individuals are impaired in their processing of interpersonal cues associated with fear and distress. Consequently, psychopathic individuals fail to heed cues that would otherwise lead them to inhibit aggressive behavior. This theory points to the failure of an internal mechanism to perform a natural function for which it was designed. That psychopathy is associated with disastrous consequences for individuals and society was not disputed by the authors. Any “benefit” of psychopathy according to the paper is in perpetuation of genes only. However, that psychopathic individuals might contribute to the gene pool, has no bearing on the definition of harm as conceptualized by Wakefield.

The authors also suggest “psychopathy is neither co-morbid nor associated with the neurodevelopmental perturbations characteristic of other serious mental illnesses, such as psychosis (p. 1).” We disagree, as there is significant co-morbidity between paranoid personality disorder and psychopathy (Blackburn and Maybury, 1985; Blackburn, 1998; Fullam and Dolan, 2006; McGregor et al., 2012). We point to Eysenck's concept of psychoticism, which links psychopathy to a tendency to become psychotic (Eysenck and Eysenck, 1977; Corr, 2010). Genetic studies have even identified potential genes linking psychoticism to schizophrenia (Suchankova et al., 2012). In the aforementioned memoire study, psychoticism was a ubiquitous finding in husbands and parents with high PCL-R scores. DSM 5 includes a measure of psychoticism in personality disorder assessments (American Psychiatric Association, 2012). Hence future research will clarify the relationship between psychoticism and psychopathy. We conclude that the contention that psychopathy does not meet Wakefield's definition of disorder was not proven by the author's arguments.

Behavioral Systems and Psychopathy

According to the behavioral systems perspective (BSP) (Johnson et al., 2012; Leedom, submitted) there are four social behavioral systems that have been subjected to adaptive selection in primate evolution: the attachment, caregiving, dominance, and sexual systems. In the BSP, psychopathy is associated with excessive sexual responses, lack of caregiving, and aberrant dominance responses. The lack of caregiving termed lack of “love” in the paper could be another failed internal mechanism in psychopathy. The BSP hypothesizes that violent behavior and the interpersonal symptoms of psychopathy are a consequence of aberrant development of the dominance system (Johnson et al., 2012; Leedom, submitted).

Rather than being “an adaptation” psychopathy may represent a spandrel—a syndrome that arises as a consequence of other features (Buss et al., 1998; Cuzzillo, 1999). We hypothesize that psychopathy may arise in part due to selection for social dominance (and possibly mating effort) and assert that it is not psychopathy but dominance (combined with mating effort) that confers adaptive advantage. Viewed as a “spandrel,” psychopathy is no more an “adaptation” than are anxiety disorders and depression, which represent diminished rather than enhanced dominance system function (Johnson et al., 2012). Recent research by Hawley (2002) on the development of the dominance system supports the BSP of psychopathy, and also suggests that “caregiving” or “prosocial” responses may be dominance behavior as these are used instrumentally by individuals of all ages to gain power. Hence, “nepotism” and other “prosocial behavior” displayed by psychopathic individuals is likely a function of the dominance as opposed to the caregiving system. New assessment instruments are needed to determine the presence of specific motives and the function of behavioral systems in psychopathic individuals as related to prosocial and antisocial behavior (Leedom et al., 2012).

Conclusion

We strongly dispute the assertion that psychopathy is not a disorder. Psychopathy is a mental disorder according to both the Wakefield definition cited in this study and American Psychiatric Association criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). More studies of the harm done to family members by psychopathic individuals are needed. Psychopathy may persist because it represents a dominance-related spandrel and because of nepotistic help to, not from, psychopathic individuals.

Acknowledgments

The authors appreciate the feedback received from David Oberleitner, Ph.D. in the preparation of this opinion.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

American Psychiatric Association. (2012). Personality Disorders APA DSM-5. Available online at: http://www.dsm5.org/proposedrevision/Pages/PersonalityDisorders.aspx [Accessed 26 June 2012].

Andrews, D. A., and Bonta, J. (2003). The Psychology of Criminal Conduct, 3rd Edn. Cincinnati, OH: Anderson Publishing Co.

Blackburn, R. (1998). “Psychopathy and the contribution of personality to violence,” in Psychopathy: Antisocial, Criminal, and Violent Behavior, eds T. Millon, E. Simonsen, M. Birket-Smith, and R. D. Davis (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 50–68.

Blackburn, R., and Maybury, C. (1985). Identifying the psychopath: the relation of Cleckley's criteria to the interpersonal domain. Pers. Individ. Dif. 6, 375–386.

Blair, J., Mitchell, D., and Blair, K. (2005). The Psychopath: Emotion and the Brain. Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Buss, D. M., Haselton, M. G., Shackelford, T. K., Bleske, A. L., and Wakefield, J. C. (1998). Adaptations, exaptations, and spandrels. Am. Psychol. 53, 533–548.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Cleckley, H. M. (1964). The Mask of Sanity: an Attempt to Clarify Some Issues About the So-Called Psychopathic Personality. St. Louis, MO: C. V. Mosby Co.

Coid, J., and Yang, M. (2011). The impact of psychopathy on violence among the household population of Great Britain. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 46, 473–480.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Corr, P. J. (2010). The psychoticism–psychopathy continuum: a neuropsychological model of core deficits. Pers. Individ. Dif. 48, 695–703.

Coyne, S. M., and Thomas, T. J. (2008). Psychopathy, aggression, and cheating behavior: a test of the Cheater-Hawk hypothesis. Pers. Individ. Dif. 44, 1105–1115.

Cuzzillo, S. L. W. (1999). Spandrels in the consulting room. Am. Psychol. 54, 441–442.

Eysenck, H. J., and Eysenck, S. B. (1977). Psychoticism as a Dimension of Personality. Oxford England: Carne and Russak.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fullam, R., and Dolan, M. (2006). The criminal and personality profile of patients with schizophrenia and comorbid psychopathic traits. Pers. Individ. Dif. 40, 1591–1602.

Hawley, P. H. (2002). Social dominance and prosocial and coercive strategies of resource control in preschoolers. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 26, 167–176.

Johnson, S. L., Leedom, L. J., and Muhtadie, L. (2012). The dominance behavioral system and psychopathology: evidence from self-report, observational, and biological studies. Psychol. Bull. 138, 692–743.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Krupp, D. B., Sewall, L. A., Lalumière, M. L., Sheriff, C., and Harris, G. T. (2012). Nepotistic patterns of violent psychopathy: evidence for adaptation? Front. Psychol. 3:305. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00305

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leedom, L. J., and Andersen, D. (2011). “Antisocial/psychopathic personality: what do family members, romantic partners and friends report?” in Presented at the Society for the Scientific Study of Psychopathy (Montreal, QC, Canada).

Leedom, L. J., Geislin, E., and Hartoonian Almas, L. (2012). “Did he ever love me?” A qualitative study of life with a psychopathic husband. Fam. Interpers. Violence Q. (in press)

McGregor, K., Castle, D., and Dolan, M. (2012). Schizophrenia spectrum disorders, substance misuse, and the four-facet model of psychopathy: the relationship to violence. Schizophr. Res. 136, 116–121.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rotgers, F., and Maniacci, M. (2005). Antisocial Personality Disorder: a Practitioner's Guide to Comparative Treatments. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company.

Suchankova, P., Klang, J., Cavanna, C., Holm, G., Nilsson, S., Jönsson, E. G., et al. (2012). Is the Gly82Ser polymorphism in the RAGE gene relevant to schizophrenia and the personality trait psychoticism? J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 37, 122–128.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wakefield, J. C. (1992). The concept of mental disorder: on the boundary between biological facts and social values. Am. Psychol. 47, 373–388.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Widom, C. S. (1977). A methodology for studying noninstitutionalized psychopaths. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 45, 674–683.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Citation: Leedom LJ and Hartoonian Almas L (2012) Is psychopathy a disorder or an adaptation? Front. Psychology 3:549. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00549

Received: 19 September 2012; Accepted: 22 November 2012;
Published online: 18 December 2012.

Edited by:

Ed Hagen, Washington State University, USA

Reviewed by:

Ed Hagen, Washington State University, USA

Copyright © 2012 Leedom and Hartoonian Almas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: ljleedom@aol.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.