Skip to main content

SPECIALTY GRAND CHALLENGE article

Front. Psychol., 25 April 2013
Sec. Eating Behavior

The Psychology of Eating

  • 1Department of Psychology I, University of Würzburg, Würzburg, Germany
  • 2Research Group “Self-Regulation and Health,” Research Unit INSIDE, Université du Luxembourg, Walferdange, Luxembourg
  • 3Research Group on Health Psychology, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

People engage in eating behavior as a matter of survival, normally every day. That is, one has to make choices about what to eat, when, and how much. In contrast to our ancestors, however, whose primary task was to seek out any food that would provide energy and nutrients, those choices have become more difficult nowadays. In western or westernized societies in particular, food is abundant, cheap, and available in a great variety. Moreover, eating is a fundamentally rewarding behavior, and is thus intrinsically linked to mood and emotions (Vögele and Gibson, 2010).

Because of this, we felt that the creation of a new specialty section about determinants and consequences of eating behavior and mechanisms of its modulation is warranted. The aim of Frontiers in Eating Behavior is to build knowledge for the understanding of eating behavior by bringing together academics with different expertise, e.g., researchers investigating basic processes related to eating behavior, clinical researchers examining the psychological, physiological, and nutritional aspects of eating disorders and practitioners, such as clinical psychologists, physicians, and other health care professionals. As yet, this new specialty section is the first and only periodical in open access publishing with a breadth of scope on eating behavior embracing various methodologies and study populations.

When humans are energy deficient, a complex interplay of physiological processes signals the brain that food should be consumed, i.e., an individual feels hungry. When enough food has been consumed, these processes signal that consumption should be terminated, i.e., an individual feels satiated or sated (Benelam, 2009). Nevertheless, this homeostatic regulation of eating is steadily challenged and overridden by the omnipresence of food and food-related cues. That is, eating can be triggered even in the absence of hunger or extended beyond satiation (Lowe and Butryn, 2007). Numerous factors are known that determine or guide eating behavior in an automatic and implicit fashion (Cohen and Farley, 2008). For instance, eating may be initiated or prolonged by the presence of others, i.e., is influenced by social factors (Herman and Polivy, 2004). Food choices and consumption are also strongly influenced by environmental factors, e.g., advertising, packaging, portion sizes, lighting, and many more (Stroebele and De Castro, 2004; Cohen and Babey, 2012). As a consequence, constant monitoring and self-regulation of eating is necessary in order to eat healthily, i.e., to provide the body both qualitatively and quantitatively with the right nutrients. At the same time, eating healthily also means to be able to enjoy the rewarding aspects of food without falling prey to a loss of control over eating.

Many individuals are able to do this successfully, yet some exhibit over-regulation of eating behavior resulting in underweight and malnutrition. Cases of anorexia nervosa have been known for a long time (Bemporad, 1996). On the other side of the extreme, permanent failures of self-regulation may result in overweight and obesity. As with anorexia, obesity is an age-old health condition (Haslam, 2011), but its prevalence has dramatically increased in the second half of the twentieth century (Stroebe, 2008). While prevalence rates in western countries appear to stabilize, rates of severe obesity continue to grow (Bessesen, 2008; Yanovski and Yanovski, 2011) and newly industrialized countries seem to catch up (Finucane et al., 2011).

In most cases, obesity is the result of poor dietary habits – rather than compulsive eating binges – which contribute to a modest average daily excess of energy intake over energy expenditure (Rogers, 2011). Some individuals, however, show regular binge eating which is defined as consuming large amounts of food over a discrete period of time with a sense of lack of control over eating, and which is associated with marked distress (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Prevalence of binge eating disorder (BED) is increased in obese individuals, but not all patients with BED are necessarily obese. Furthermore, there are some individuals who engage in regular binge eating but use compensatory behaviors like vomiting to prevent weight gain. Hence, patients with bulimia nervosa (BN) are mostly normal-weight (Thompson, 2003).

Unlike anorexia and obesity, BED, and BN have been first described in the twentieth century but their research history yet encompasses several decades (Stunkard, 1959; Russell, 1979). Both eating disorders and obesity involve medical complications and are marked by psychological distress and co-morbid mental disorders (Thompson, 2003), of which mood and anxiety disorders are the most prevalent (Vögele and Gibson, 2010). Therefore, it is of no surprise that there are numerous scientific journals, which are devoted to publishing research on the etiology and treatment of those disorders.

In addition to eating disorders and obesity, there are a vast number of eating behaviors that deserve scientific scrutiny and discussion. For instance, there are some problematic eating behaviors that are not included in the current diagnostic manuals, but are continuously debated in terms of their clinical relevance (cf. Corsica and Pelchat, 2010; Vandereycken, 2011), e.g., night eating (Stunkard et al., 1955), orthorexia (Bratman and Knight, 2001), or food addiction (Randolph, 1956). Furthermore, there is an array of eating behaviors that do not reflect disordered eating per se, but nonetheless seem to be associated with occasional overeating and moderate overweight, e.g., restrained or emotional eating (Herman and Mack, 1975; Macht and Simons, 2011). Accordingly, it has been recognized that some eating behaviors can be mapped onto a continuum ranging from normal to disordered eating (e.g., Lowe et al., 1996).

Thus, it would appear to be inappropriate to focus on eating disorders and obesity as separate entities. We understand our mission for Frontiers in Eating Behavior to create a view on eating and its disorders on a continuum from healthy eating practices to disordered eating behaviors. Therefore, findings from basic research on eating behavior are important to understand disordered eating behavior. These findings may include research on how food and food-cues are processed in the brain, mechanisms underlying successful and unsuccessful self-regulation of eating, or social and environmental determinants of and individual differences in food choice and consumption. In addition, there is increasing evidence that eating behaviors, e.g., food preferences, are shaped by gene-environment interactions in early childhood. Nevertheless, the role of experience, or learning (e.g., classic conditioning, observational learning), is critical in the development of young children’s eating behavior, which may well carry over into adulthood (Havermans, in press). Thus, a better understanding of the developmental aspects of eating behavior is essential to understand eating behavior in adulthood. On the other hand, results from clinical studies may advance our knowledge concerning non-clinical issues, which are relevant for most humans. For example, research on triggers of and treatment approaches for reducing binge eating may also be useful to increase dieting success or inducing healthier food choices in overweight individuals without eating disorders.

Thus, we encourage researchers to break away from isolated, categorical views on normal vs. disordered eating, homeostatic vs. hedonic concepts, or physiological vs. psychological mechanisms. We hope that Frontiers in Eating Behavior will increase the awareness of a more comprehensive view on eating behavior and advance our understanding of a behavior that is essential for our survival as individuals and as a species.

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Bemporad, J. R. (1996). Self-starvation through the ages: reflections on the pre-history of anorexia nervosa. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 19, 217–237.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Benelam, B. (2009). Satiation, satiety and their effects on eating behaviour. Nutr. Bull. 34, 126–173.

CrossRef Full Text

Bessesen, D. H. (2008). Update on obesity. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 93, 2027–2034.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bratman, S., and Knight, D. (2001). Health Food Junkies – Orthorexia Nervosa: Overcoming the Obsession with Healthful Eating. New York: Broadway.

Cohen, D. A., and Babey, S. H. (2012). Contextual influences on eating behaviours: heuristic processing and dietary choices. Obes. Rev. 13, 766–779.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohen, D. A., and Farley, T. A. (2008). Eating as an automatic behavior. Prev. Chronic Dis. 5, 1–7.

Corsica, J. A., and Pelchat, M. L. (2010). Food addiction: true or false? Curr. Opin. Gastroenterol. 26, 165–169.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Finucane, M. M., Stevens, G. A., Cowan, M. J., Danaei, G., Lin, J. K., Paciorek, C. J., et al. (2011). National, regional, and global trends in body-mass index since 1980: systematic analysis of health examination surveys and epidemiological studies with 960 country-years and 9.1 million participants. Lancet 377, 557–567.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Haslam, D. (2011). The history of obesity. Clin. Obes. 1, 189–197.

CrossRef Full Text

Havermans, R. C. (in press). Pavlovian craving and overeating: a conditioned incentive model. Curr. Obes. Rep.

Herman, C. P., and Mack, D. (1975). Restrained and unrestrained eating. J. Pers. 43, 647–660.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Herman, C. P., and Polivy, J. (2004). “The self-regulation of eating: theoretical and practical problems,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications, eds R. F. Baumeister, and K. D. Vohs (New York: The Guilford Press), 492–508.

Lowe, M. R., and Butryn, M. L. (2007). Hedonic hunger: a new dimension of appetite? Physiol. Behav. 91, 432–439.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lowe, M. R., Gleaves, D. H., Disimone-Weiss, R. T., Furgueson, C., Gayda, C. A., Kolsky, P. A., et al. (1996). Restraint, dieting, and the continuum model of bulimia nervosa. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 105, 508–517.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Macht, M., and Simons, G. (2011). “Emotional eating,” in Emotion Regulation and Well-Being, eds I. Nyklícek, A. Vingerhoets, and M. Zeelenberg (New York: Springer), 281–295.

Randolph, T. G. (1956). The descriptive features of food addiction: addictive eating and drinking. Q. J. Stud. Alcohol. 17, 198–224.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rogers, P. J. (2011). Obesity – is food addiction to blame? Addiction 106, 1213–1214.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Russell, G. (1979). Bulimia nervosa: an ominous variant of anorexia nervosa. Psychol. Med. 9, 429–448.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stroebe, W. (2008). “Environmental causes of the increase in overweight and obesity,” in Dieting, Overweight, and Obesity – Self-Regulation in a Food-Rich Environment, ed. W. Stroebe (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 59–92.

Stroebele, N., and De Castro, J. M. (2004). Effect of ambience on food intake and food choice. Nutrition 20, 821–838.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stunkard, A. J. (1959). Eating patterns and obesity. Psychiatr. Q. 33, 284–295.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stunkard, A. J., Grace, W. J., and Wolff, H. G. (1955). The night-eating syndrome – a pattern of food intake among certain obese patients. Am. J. Med. 19, 78–86.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thompson, J. K. (2003). Handbook of Eating Disorders and Obesity. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Vandereycken, W. (2011). Media hype, diagnostic fad or genuine disorder? Professionals’ opinions about night eating syndrome, orthorexia, muscle dysmorphia, and emetophobia. Eat. Disord. 19, 145–155.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vögele, C., and Gibson, L. (2010). “Mood, emotions and eating disorders,” in Oxford Handbook of Eating Disorders. Series: Oxford Library of Psychology, ed. W. S. Agras (New York: Oxford University Press), 180–205.

Yanovski, S. Z., and Yanovski, J. A. (2011). Obesity prevalence in the United States – up, down, or sideways? N. Engl. J. Med. 364, 987–989.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Meule A and Vögele C (2013) The psychology of eating. Front. Psychol. 4:215. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00215

Received: 27 February 2013; Accepted: 08 April 2013;
Published online: 25 April 2013.

Edited by:

Axel Cleeremans, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Axel Cleeremans, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Belgium

Copyright: © 2013 Meule and Vögele. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: adrian.meule@uni-wuerzburg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.