Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 01 August 2013
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Intrinsic motivations and open-ended development in animals, humans, and robots View all 19 articles

Novelty, attention, and challenges for developmental psychology

  • Department of Psychology, University of Hull, Hull, UK

Introduction

In this brief essay, I seek to demonstrate the significance of exploratory behavior for understanding cognitive development. Historically, organisms were thought to act solely in the service of achieving biologically significant goals, such as satisfying thirst, hunger, and reproductive drives. However, it became apparent that both animals and humans engage in behavior where the adaptive goal is unclear (see Hunt, 1963, 1965). With no obvious external target, this activity is best described as being intrinsically motivated, and often directed toward the unknown and the unexpected (Kagan, 2002). Hence novelty, the discrepancy between what is known and what is discovered, can elicit activity and exploration of the environment.

What is the relevance to developmental process? Attention to novelty plays a seemingly simple role in learning and development, directing the senses toward what is as yet unknown. Yet, research shows that patterns of attention to novelty are not straightforward, particularly during infancy. There is considerable evidence that attention is sometimes biased toward familiarity, rather than novelty. Unlike our understanding of novelty preference, we struggle to understand when and why familiarity preferences occur. Below I briefly review this area of research and illustrate how this basic aspect of learning continues to puzzle developmental psychologists.

From Animals to Infants?

The habituation mechanism, which directs attention to novelty, has been widely-studied across the animal kingdom (Sokolov, 1963; Thompson and Spencer, 1966). Thorpe (1963) defined habituation as “the relatively permanent waning of a response as a result of repeated stimulation … ” (p. 61). An important feature of habituation is that an organism's responding will recover to the presentation of a different stimulus—an effect known as dishabituation (Thompson and Spencer, 1966). Hence, habituation is stimulus–specific and attention will recover to novel stimuli. A seminal study by Fantz (1964) demonstrated that infants' visual attention to a familiar, repeated image will decrease relative to their attention to a novel image. Other early studies of infant habituation also reveal infants' interest in novel stimuli (see Cohen and Gelber, 1975, for a review). There is evidence that even newborns will habituate and direct their attention to novelty (Friedman, 1972; Slater et al., 1982, 1984). Many infancy researchers are interested in the use of novelty preferences as a methodological tool. Habituation–dishabituation procedures are used to demonstrate infants' discrimination of stimuli, and seemingly precocious cognitive abilities (e.g., Baillargeon, 1987; Spelke et al., 1992; but see Schilling, 2000, for a counterargument).

Infants' interest in novelty is consistent with theories of habituation accounting for both human and animal responding. However, there is substantial evidence that infants do not always prefer a novel stimulus—sometimes they prefer to attend to a familiar stimulus. In Rose et al. (1982) groups of 3.5- and 6.5-month-olds were exposed to a visual stimulus for different durations. This familiar stimulus was then paired with a novel stimulus. Infants at both ages displayed familiarity preferences after shorter exposures, and novelty preferences after longer exposures. Similarly, Hunter et al. (1983) presented 8- and 12-month-olds with a set of toys, and tested their preference for the familiar vs. novel toys after differing amounts of familiarization. The infants preferred the familiar toys after a shorter familiarization period, and the novel toys after a longer familiarization period. In these studies, familiarization time was manipulated between groups of infants. Roder et al. (2000) provide a within-infants demonstration that 4.5-month-olds preferences shift from familiarity to novelty as a function of familiarization time.

While familiarity and novelty preferences have largely been investigated for the visual modality, there is also evidence that infants display these preferences for auditory stimuli (e.g., Colombo and Bundy, 1983; Spence, 1996). More recent research has found that infants' preferences will also “reverse” as their memory for a familiarized stimulus decays over time. In Bahrick and Pickens (1995), 3-month-olds displayed a novelty preference after a 1 min retention interval, and a familiarity preference after a 1 month interval (see also Spence, 1996; Bahrick et al., 1997; Courage and Howe, 2001). Familiarity preferences do not just occur to repetitions of a specific stimulus. Infants who have categorized a set of stimuli will sometimes attend more to a novel stimulus from the same category, rather than a novel stimulus drawn from a novel category (e.g., Gomez and Gerken, 1999; Fiser and Aslin, 2002; Gómez and Maye, 2005; Mather and Plunkett, 2011). Hunter and Ames (1988) provide a descriptive model of infants' familiarity and novelty preferences. The main factor is familiarization time—with briefer exposures, the infant attends more to a familiar stimulus, but with longer exposures, their attention turns to novelty. How quickly an infant makes this “familiarity-to-novelty shift” will depend on their processing speed and the complexity of the stimuli. Hence, familiarity preferences are more likely for younger infants (slower processors), and for more complex stimuli.

Familiarity preferences are not consistent with the habituation process, where attention simply declines with repeated exposure to a stimulus (Thompson and Spencer, 1966). Some computational models of infant attention have tentatively linked familiarity preferences with the process of sensitization (Sirois and Mareschal, 2004; Schoner and Thelen, 2006). Sensitization occurs when the presentation of a stimulus leads to heightened behavioral responding. Importantly, if a stimulus is repeated, it can have the effect of sensitizing itself. Under dual-process theory (Groves and Thompson, 1970), habituation and sensitization are separate, opposing processes which interact to determine responding. Sensitization is related to stimuli intensity, and decays quickly. If sensitization is initially stronger than habituation, there will be an early increase in responding to a repeated stimulus, followed by a decrease. This pattern of response to a repeated stimulus is similar to the familiarity–novelty shift sometimes evidenced by infants. However, in contrast to habituation, sensitization will generalize to a wide range of stimuli (see Domjan, 1998, for examples). Hence, while sensitization can occur to a repeated stimulus, it would also generalize to other stimuli if they were also present. This means that sensitization cannot account for the stimulus specificity of familiarity and novelty preferences (see also Turk-Browne et al., 2008, for a related argument).

The Old or the New?

An alternative theoretical perspective could account for the existence of familiarity preferences. Since the 1950's, a variety of arguments have been made that both adults and infants prefer stimuli which provide an optimal level of novelty or information (Dember and Earl, 1957; Berlyne, 1960; McCall and McGhee, 1977). The optimum is defined by a “moderate” discrepancy between a stimulus and an observer's representation of that stimulus. Hence, the more discrepant a stimulus is from the observer's state of knowledge, the more novel it is to the observer. Relatedly, stimulus complexity influences the amount of learning required to reduce this discrepancy. Any stimulus which is more or less discrepant than the optimum is of less interest to the observer. The familiarity-to-novelty shift displayed by infants is consistent with optimal-level theory. It is possible for a familiar stimulus to be favored over a novel stimulus, because the familiar stimulus could initially be closer to the optimum. Further processing of the familiar stimulus will result in a shift away from the optimum, and a novel stimulus will be preferred (see Hunter and Ames, 1988, for an elaboration).

A problem with obtaining evidence of the familiarity-to-novelty shift is that there is a temporally limited window for observing a familiarity preference. At a certain point, attention will shift toward novelty, thus a successful experimental design must be sensitive to this shift. Different infants will also process information at different rates, meaning that individual preferences can be obscured by group data (see Roder et al., 2000). Unfortunately, optimal-level theory does not provide a remedy for these methodological issues. The key variables involved—stimulus complexity, processing speed, and the optimal level of novelty—are usually unknown quantities. This makes it difficult to predict the occurrence of familiarity preferences, and to test the assumptions of the theory (see Thomas, 1971). A lack of familiarity preference could be due to the stimuli not being sufficiently complex, or an infant rapidly processing the stimuli. Therefore, while the existence of familiarity preferences is consistent with optimal-level theory, the theory itself perhaps does little more than assert that we seek out moderately novel stimuli.

One approach to dealing with the shortcomings of optimal-level theory has been to develop more computationally explicit models of familiarity and novelty preferences (Sirois and Mareschal, 2004; Schoner and Thelen, 2006; see also Perone et al., 2011) and to mathematically formalize the information content of a stimulus (e.g., Kidd et al., 2012). These recent advances offer an improved level of theoretical precision over past formulations of optimal-level theory. Nonetheless, these models incorporate some of the basic assumptions of optimal-level theory, and may also retain the difficulties of predicting the familiarity-to-novelty shift. Our ability to understand exactly when infants will seek out familiarity or novelty is likely to require a deeper understanding of why there is an optimum in the first place. Development requires a balance of familiarization with regularities in the environment (Gibson, 1969) and shifting attention to what is new and unknown so as to create new cognitive structures (Piaget, 1936/1952). Therefore, rather than just focusing on preferences for individual stimuli, one useful approach might be to explore the more global consequences for the abstraction and development of knowledge.

Order and Timing: The Cycle of Cognitive Development

The familiarity-to-novelty shift causes infants to process stimuli in a particular sequence. That is, with all other factors held constant, infants' will explore different stimuli in a systematic fashion, based on their prior experience and learning. Beyond the laboratory, how do these preferences shape patterns of learning across the vast multitude of items and events in the real world, across multiple timescales? Computational models and experimental data demonstrate how the pattern of input can influence the trajectory and success of learning. In Elman (1993), recurrent neural networks were more successful at acquiring grammatical categories if they began by only learning about a subset of the total sentences available, rather than learning about all sentences together (see also Plunkett and Marchman, 1991, 1993). Other research suggests that order effects may also occur in infant categorization (Sandhofer and Doumas, 2008; Mather and Plunkett, 2011). What is particularly intriguing is that in some cases, exposure to an initially restricted stimulus set supports learning (Elman, 1993), whereas in other cases, reduced variability hinders learning (see Mather and Plunkett, 2011). These findings hint at a global effect of optimal preferences on successful learning.

Currently, we understand little about the role that familiarity and novelty preferences might play in driving successful patterns of learning. However, if we can better understand the effects of these preferences on cognitive development, then we might make sense of the underlying cause of optimal preferences. Conversely, our explanations of cognitive development would also benefit from understand the impact of exploratory behavior on learning. Much current developmental research is concerned with specifying the mechanisms of learning, without considering how and why attention prioritizes certain stimuli for learning. Cognitive development needs to be understood as a cyclical process, where attention influences learning, and learning guides attention. If, as Piaget (1936/1952) argued, the child is actively engaged in the construction of their own knowledge, then exploratory behavior needs to be placed at the heart of cognitive development.

Conclusions

A hallmark of human behavior is that we seek to explore and understand our environments, even in the absence of biological or externally specified goals. Our interest in what is new and unknown is evident from birth. However, we do not yet have a clear understanding of the mechanisms which determine whether a child attends to familiarity or novelty. The function of optimal preferences may need to be interpreted in the context of broader developmental changes. Both the processes of cognitive growth and exploratory behavior can be better understood by considering their interdependence.

References

Bahrick, L. E., Hernandez-Reif, M., and Pickens, J. N. (1997). The effect of retrieval cues on visual preferences and memory in infancy: evidence for a four-phase attention function. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 67, 1–20. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1997.2399

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bahrick, L. E., and Pickens, J. N. (1995). Infant memory for object motion across a period of three months: implications for a four-phase attention function. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 59, 343–371. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1995.1017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanance in 3.5- and 4.5-month-old infants. Dev. Psychol. 23, 655–664. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.23.5.655

CrossRef Full Text

Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. doi: 10.1037/11164-000

Cohen, L. B., and Gelber, E. R. (1975). “Infant visual memory,” in Infant Perception: From Sensation to Cognition, Vol. 1, eds L. B. Cohen and P. Salapatek (London: Academic Press), 347–403.

Colombo, J., and Bundy, R. S. (1983). Infant response to auditory familiarity and novelty. Infant Behav. Dev. 6, 305–311. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(83)80039-3

CrossRef Full Text

Courage, M. L., and Howe, M. L. (2001). Long-term retention in 3.5-month-olds: familiarization time and individual differences in attentional style. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 79, 271–293. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2606

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dember, W. N., and Earl, R. W. (1957). Analysis of exploratory, manipulatory, and curiosity behaviors. Psychol. Rev. 64, 91–96. doi: 10.1037/h0046861

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Domjan, M. (1998). The Principles of Learning and Behaviour. 4th Edn. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Elman, J. L. (1993). Learning and development in neural networks: the importance of starting small. Cognition 48, 71–99. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90058-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fantz, R. L. (1964). Visual experience in infants: decreased attention to familiar patterns relative to novel ones. Science 146, 668–670. doi: 10.1126/science.146.3644.668

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fiser, J., and Aslin, R. N. (2002). Statistical learning of new visual feature combinations by infants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 15822–15826. doi: 10.1073/pnas.232472899

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Friedman, S. (1972). Habituation and recovery of visual response in the alert human newborn. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 13, 339–349. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(72)90095-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gibson, E. (1969). Principles of Perceptual Learning and Development. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Gomez, R. L., and Gerken, L. (1999). Artificial grammar learning by 1-year-olds leads to specific and abstract knowledge. Cognition 70, 109–135. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00003-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gómez, R. L., and Maye, J. (2005). The developmental trajectory of nonadjacent dependency learning. Infancy 7, 183–206. doi: 10.1207/s15327078in0702_4

CrossRef Full Text

Groves, P. M., and Thompson, R. F. (1970). Habituation: a dual-process theory. Psychol. Rev. 77, 419–450. doi: 10.1037/h0029810

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hunt, J. McV. (1963). “Motivation inherent in information processing and action,” in Motivation and Social Interaction: Cognitive Determinants, ed O. J. Harvey (New York, NY: Ronald Press), 35–94.

Hunt, J. McV. (1965). “Intrinsic motivation and its role in psychological development,” in Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Vol. 13, ed D. Levine (Lincoln, NB: University of Nebraska), 189–282.

Hunter, M. A., and Ames, E. W. (1988). “A multifactor model of infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli,” in Advances in Infancy Research, Vol. 5, eds C. Rovee-Collier and L. P. Lipsitt (Stamford, CT: Ablex), 69–95.

Hunter, M. A., Ames, E. W., and Koopman, R. (1983). Effects of stimulus complexity and familarisation time on infant preferences for novel and familiar stimuli. Dev. Psychol. 19, 338–352. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.19.3.338

CrossRef Full Text

Kagan, J. (2002). Surprise, Uncertainty, and Mental Structures. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Kidd, C., Piantadosi, S. T., and Aslin, R. N. (2012). The Goldilocks effect: human infants allocate attention to visual sequences that are neither too simple nor too complex. PLoS ONE 7:e36399. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036399

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mather, E., and Plunkett, K. (2011). Same items, different order: effects of temporal variability on infant categorization. Cognition 119, 438–447. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.02.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McCall, R. B., and McGhee, P. E. (1977). “The discrepancy hypothesis of attention and affect in infants,” in The Structuring of Experience, eds I. C. Uzgiris and F. Weizmann (New York, NY: Plenum), 179–210.

Perone, S., Simmering, V. R., and Spencer, J. P. (2011). Stronger neural dynamics capture changes in infants' visual working memory capacity over development. Dev. Sci. 14, 1379–1392. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01083.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Piaget, J. (1936/1952). The Origins of Intelligence in Children. New York, NY: International Universities Press.

Plunkett, K., and Marchman, V. (1991). U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perceptron: implications for child language acquisition. Cognition 38, 43–102. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90022-V

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plunkett, K., and Marchman, V. (1993). From rote learning to system building: acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets. Cognition 48, 21–69. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(93)90057-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Roder, B. J., Bushnell, E. W., and Sasseville, A. M. (2000). Infants' preferences for familiarity and novelty during the course of visual processing. Infancy 1, 491–507. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0104_9

CrossRef Full Text

Rose, S. A., Melloy-Carminar, P., Gottfried, A. W., and Bridger, W. H. (1982). Familiarity and novelty preferences in infant recognition memory: implications for information processing. Dev. Psychol. 18, 704–713. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.18.5.704

CrossRef Full Text

Sandhofer, C. M., and Doumas, L. A. A. (2008). Order of presentation effects in learning color categories. J. Cogn. Dev. 9, 194–221. doi: 10.1080/15248370802022639

CrossRef Full Text

Schilling, T. H. (2000). Infants' looking at possible and impossible screen rotations: the role of familiarization. Infancy 1, 389–402. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0104_2

CrossRef Full Text

Schoner, G., and Thelen, E. (2006). Using dynamic field theory to rethink infant habituation. Psychol. Rev. 113, 273–299. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.113.2.273

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sirois, S., and Mareschal, D. (2004). An interacting systems model of infant habituation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1352–1362. doi: 10.1162/0898929042304778

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Slater, A., Morison, V., and Rose, D. (1982). Visual memory at birth. Br. J. Psychol. 73, 519–525. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1982.tb01834.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Slater, A., Morison, V., and Rose, D. (1984). Habituation in the newborn. Infant Behav. Dev. 7, 183–200. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80057-0

CrossRef Full Text

Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the Conditioned Reflex. Oxford: Pergamon Press.

Spelke, E. S., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, J., and Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychol. Rev. 99, 605–632. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.99.4.605

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Spence, M. J. (1996). Young infants' long-term auditory memory: evidence for changes in preference as a function of delay. Dev. Psychobiol. 29, 685–695.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Thomas, H. (1971). Discrepancy hypotheses: methodological and theoretical considerations. Psychol. Rev. 78, 249–259. doi: 10.1037/h0030795

CrossRef Full Text

Thompson, R. F., and Spencer, W. A. (1966). Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychol. Rev. 73, 16–43. doi: 10.1037/h0022681

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thorpe, W. H. (1963). Learning and Instinct in Animals, 2nd Edn. London: Methuen.

Turk-Browne, N. B., Scholl, B. J., and Chun, M. M. (2008). Babies and brains: habituation in infant cognition and functional neuroimaging. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2:16. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.016.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Citation: Mather E (2013) Novelty, attention, and challenges for developmental psychology. Front. Psychol. 4:491. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00491

Received: 20 May 2013; Accepted: 13 July 2013;
Published online: 01 August 2013.

Edited by:

Marco Mirolli, Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione, Italy

Copyright © 2013 Mather. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: emily.mather@hull.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.