Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 October 2013
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Writing words: From brain to hand(s) View all 22 articles

How does graphotactic knowledge influence children's learning of new spellings?

  • 1Laboratoire Mémoire et Cognition, Psychology Department, Université Paris Descartes, Boulogne Billancourt, France
  • 2Institut Universitaire de France, Paris, France
  • 3Psychology Department, LAPSCO, Université Blaise Pascal, Clermont Ferrand Cedex1, France
  • 4Psychology Department, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA

Two experiments investigated whether and how the learning of spellings by French third graders is influenced by two graphotactic patterns: consonants cannot double in word-initial position (Experiment 1) and consonants cannot double after single consonants (Experiment 2). Children silently read meaningful texts that contained three types of novel spellings: no doublet (e.g., mupile, guprane), doublet in a legal position (e.g., muppile, gupprane), and doublet in an illegal position (e.g., mmupile, guprrane). Orthographic learning was assessed with a task of spelling to dictation. In both experiments, children recalled items without doublets better than items with doublets. In Experiment 1, children recalled spellings with a doublet in illegal word-initial position better than spellings with a doublet in legal word-medial position, and almost all misspellings involved the omission of the doublet. The fact that the graphotactic violation in an item like mmupile was in the salient initial position may explain why children often remembered both the presence and the position of the doublet. In Experiment 2, children recalled non-words with a doublet before a single consonant (legal, e.g., gupprane) better than those with a doublet after a single consonant (illegal, e.g., guprrane). Omission of the doublet was the most frequent error for both types of items. Children also made some transposition errors on items with a doublet after a single consonant, recalling for example gupprane instead of guprrane. These results suggest that, when a doublet is in the hard-to-remember medial position, children sometimes remember that an item contains a doublet but not which letter is doubled. Their knowledge that double consonants can occur before but not after single consonants leads to transposition errors on items like guprrane. These results shed new light on the conditions under which children use general knowledge about the graphotactic patterns of their writing system to reconstruct spellings.

Introduction

All alphabetic writing systems are based on the principle that graphemes represent phonemes, but they vary in orthographic depth, from consistent mappings between phonemes and graphemes in shallow writing systems to inconsistent mappings in deep writing systems (Sprenger-Charolles, 2003; Ziegler and Goswami, 2005). The fact that there is often more than one possible spelling for a given phoneme in deep writing systems like English and French is a major source of difficulty in learning to spell (Seymour et al., 2003). In some cases, spellers must choose between representing a sound with a single letter or a doublet, that is, a sequence of two identical letters. This case is especially frequent in French, where there is usually no phonological distinction between single and double consonants. For example, ule and ulle are similarly pronounced in bulle (“bubble”) and formule (“formula”), and acro and accro are similarly pronounced in acrobate (“acrobat”) and accrocher (“to hang up”). French children and adults sometimes omit a required doublet (e.g., bulle misspelled as bule) or double a letter when not required (e.g., formule misspelled as formulle; Lucci and Millet, 1994; Manesse et al., 2007).

It is often assumed that, when there is more than one possible spelling for a phoneme, spellers must memorize which one is correct. However, that may not always be true. French, like other writing systems, has statistical patterns that reduce or sometimes even eliminate the need for rote word-by-word memorization (e.g., Treiman et al., 2002; Deacon et al., 2008). Concerning consonant doubling, French spellers can rely on various graphotactic regularities, that is, statistical patterns concerning the arrangement of letters in words. These include the fact that some consonants double more often than others (e.g., l frequently doubles, d rarely doubles, and k never doubles) and that doublets may occur between two vowels (e.g., nappe “tablecloth”; apporter “to bring”) or between a vowel and a single consonant (e.g., apprendre “to learn”) but not at the beginnings of words or after a single consonant. Use of such graphotactic regularities cannot help spellers to avoid misspellings such as nape and aprendre, with omission of the doublet, or misspellings such as formulle, with l mistakenly doubled, but it can help them to avoid misspellings such as nnape, with an initial doublet, and aprrendre, with a doublet after a single consonant. However, reliance on graphotactic regularities in order to spell words that contain uncommon patterns should lead to the production of misspellings. These misspellings should not be random but should rather show a bias toward the most likely spellings. For example, French people who encounter a new word such as gumadde may subsequently spell it gummade if they remember that this word includes a doublet but not which letter is doubled and if they use their knowledge that m is more likely to double than d. In the present study, we tested these ideas in a study of French third graders. We asked whether and how children's learning of spellings is influenced by two graphotactic patterns that are not typically taught explicitly in French schools: consonants cannot double in word-initial position (Experiment 1) and after single consonants (Experiment 2).

Much of the evidence for children's knowledge of graphotactic patterns comes from studies in which they are asked to decide which of two non-words looks more like a word of their language. As early as first grade, for example, French children were more likely to choose non-words including doublets for pairs like imose and immose, where m is frequent in both single and double formats, than for pairs like idose and iddose, where d is frequent as a single letter but rarely doubles in French (Danjon and Pacton, 2009; see also Cassar and Treiman, 1997; Pacton et al., 2001). In judgment tasks such as these, graphotactic knowledge is the only source of information on which participants can rely. Here we went beyond previous studies by examining the influence of graphotactics in a more natural situation in which children can also use memory for word-specific spellings. We used, in addition to a judgment task, a task that models the everyday situation in which children encounter a new word during the course of reading for meaning and later try to spell it. Such a situation has been used in a number of other studies whose results suggest that, to a large extent, people teach themselves to spell through reading (Burt and Butterworth, 1996; Share, 1999, 2004; Burt and Furry, 2000; Nation et al., 2007).

Wright and Ehri (2007) reported an influence of graphotactics on memory for spellings in U.S. kindergartners and first graders. In their study, children were taught to pronounce isolated words. Children saw items that included only single consonants (e.g., rug), items that began with consonant doublets (e.g., rrug), and items that ended with consonant doublets (e.g., rugg). In English, consonant doubling virtually never occurs at the beginnings of words (llama is the chief exception, but this is not a word that young children would know). However, doubling sometimes occurs at the ends of words (e.g., ball, miss). The children studied by Wright and Ehri learned to read words with final doubled consonants as easily as words with only single consonants, but it took them longer to learn to read words with initial doublets. When children were later asked to spell the words, they remembered final doublets almost as well as final singletons. However, their memory for initial doublets was very poor. Many children misremembered words containing initial doublets without any doublets (e.g., rrug as rug), but a number of children doubled the final consonant instead of the initial one (e.g., rrug as rugg). Children rarely erroneously doubled final consonants when an item like tub had been taught, suggesting that a memory that some letter was doubled in rrug caused them to produce the error rugg. Children's knowledge of graphotactic patterns apparently led them to double the consonant in a legal rather than an illegal position.

Wright and Ehri's (2007) finding that kindergartners and first graders remembered final doubled letters better than initial doubled letters contrasts with the often reported finding that children pay special attention to initial letters in words (Ehri and Saltmarsh, 1995; Bowman and Treiman, 2002; Treiman et al., 2007) and remember them better than subsequent letters (e.g., Mendenhall, 1930; Jensen, 1962; Kooi et al., 1965; Stage and Wagner, 1992; Treiman et al., 1993). Wright and Ehri's finding also contrasts with the common finding that people recall distinctive items more easily than common items (e.g., Hunt and Worthen, 2006; Gounden and Nicolas, 2012). This facilitative effect has been shown in memory tasks using bizarre images and sentences describing bizarre events, as well as in memory tasks for words that contain atypical letter combinations. For example, Zechmeister (1972) showed that adults were better at remembering that they had seen words containing atypical letter combinations than words containing more typical letter combinations. Similarly, Grainger and Ziegler (2011) suggested that letter combinations that are encountered infrequently in other words are highly informative with respect to word identity.

In Experiment 1, we asked whether and how the learning of spellings by French third graders is influenced by their knowledge that consonants cannot double in word-initial position. Experiment 1 differed from the study of Wright and Ehri (2007) not only in the language under study and the age of the children but also in several other dimensions. One difference is that Wright and Ehri used non-words with doublets either at the beginning or at the end. Because French consonants may double in internal position but not in initial and final positions, we used spellings without doublets (e.g., mupile), spellings with an initial doublet (e.g., mmupile), and spellings with a medial doublet (e.g., muppile). The three types of non-word spellings all had the same pronunciations. Another difference is that, while Wright and Ehri (2007) presented children with spellings of isolated words and explicitly taught the children how to pronounce them, we included a task in which children encountered a new word during the course of reading for meaning and were later asked to spell it. In Experiment 1, children were exposed to the three types of non-word spellings just mentioned, with frequency of exposure equated, in the context of meaningful texts. Children were not explicitly asked to learn the new spellings. Memory for the spelling of non-words was tested after children had finished reading the stories in which the non-words were embedded and performing a filler task. We also included a judgment task like those of previous studies in which children saw pairs of non-words (e.g., nummar and nnumar) and judged which one looked more like a word of their language (Cassar and Treiman, 1997; Wright and Ehri, 2007; Danjon and Pacton, 2009).

If children show the same kind of pattern that Wright and Ehri (2007) reported, they should produce fewer correct spellings of non-words containing an initial doublet (e.g., mmupile) than of those containing a medial doublet (e.g., muppile) or those not containing a doublet (e.g., mupile). Both omission and transposition errors should be committed on items with an initial doublet, but omission errors should be largely restricted to items with a medial doublet. Thus, transposition of the doublet from one position to another should be more common for items with an initial doublet (as when mmupile is remembered as muppile) than for items with a medial doublet (as when muppile is remembered as mmupile). In contrast, if children's attention is captured by the presence of a doublet at the beginning of an item, and if they remember the initial part of the item well, they may often correctly recall non-words with an initial doublet. Children might even be more accurate on non-words with initial doublets than on non-words with medial doublets. The first graders studied by Wright and Ehri (2007) did not show this pattern, perhaps because their knowledge that initial consonants cannot double was relatively weak. Third graders may be more knowledgeable that items such as mmupile are orthographically odd. That orthographic distinctiveness might capture their attention and help them to remember not only the presence of the doublet but also its position.

Experiment 1

Methods

Participants

The participants were 24 native speakers of French from an elementary school in Poitiers, France. The children were tested at the end of their third grade year. Their characteristics in terms of age, gender, and spelling ability are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics (age and gender) and scores on the general spelling ability and judgment tasks (standard deviations in parentheses).

Stimuli

Non-word learning task. We constructed six bisyllabic non-words that were phonologically legal in French: /fonεs/, /mypil/, /nifyd/, /pamεv/, /ritod/, and /taryl/. Each non-word included two target consonants that are often spelled with a double letter in French: /f/, /m/, /n/, /p/, /r/, and /t/. Each target consonant was the onset of the first syllable in one non-word and the onset of the second syllable in another. We created no-doublet, initial-doublet, and medial-doublet spellings of each non-word, as in mupile (no doublet), mmupile (initial doublet), and muppile (medial doublet). The specific rendition of each non-word was counterbalanced across children so that (1) each child saw two no-doublet spellings, two initial-doublet spellings, and two medial-doublet spellings; (2) each child saw only one spelling of a given non-word (e.g., only the no-doublet spelling mupile for a child and only the medial-doublet muppile for another child); and (3) each spelling of a given non-word was presented to eight children (e.g., the no-doublet spelling mupile to eight children and the medial-doublet muppile to eight other children).

We created three stories, with an average length of 157 words (range 153–167), in which we embedded two non-words. The non-words served as nouns, for example the name of a type of fruit. Two non-words of the same type (i.e., no, initial, or medial doublet) were not included in the same story. A sample story appears in the Appendix. Each non-word occurred five times in each story. Four questions were prepared about each story. The first required participants to select an appropriate title for the story from a list of three. The next three questions were true/false questions about the content. The order of the stories and the non-words embedded in them were randomized across subjects.

Judgment task. This task included 24 pairs of non-words, which are shown in the Appendix. In the eight pairs used to assess children's knowledge that consonants cannot double in word-initial position, one non-word included a medial doublet and the other an initial doublet, as in the pair nummar and nnumar. The two doublets were formed with consonants that often double in French. In eight pairs used to assess children's knowledge that consonants cannot double before a consonant, one non-word included a doublet before a single consonant and the other a doublet after a single consonant, as in apprulir and aprrulir. The two doublets were formed with consonants that can double in French. In eight pairs used to assess children's knowledge that only some consonants can double, one non-word included a frequent doublet in word-medial position and the other a doublet formed with a consonant that never doubles in French, also in word-medial position, as in the pair onnave and ojjav. For each type of pair, legal non-words were on the right in half of the trials and on the left in the other half. They were arranged in a random order and stapled together in order to make a booklet. The booklet started with three practice pairs in which only one item could be a French word.

General spelling ability.General spelling ability was assessed with the Corbeau standardized spelling subtest (Chevrier-Muller et al., 1997). This test provides a global spelling score which reflects children's ability to produce spellings that are phonologically plausible, even though not necessarily orthographically correct, children's use of word-specific spelling knowledge, and children's correct use of grammatical markers. We used this test to ensure that the spelling ability of the participants was representative of the expected level of French third graders and that it was similar for the participants in Experiments 1 and 2.

Procedure

Children were tested as a group. The first day, children were told that they would receive booklets that included stories along with questions about each. Children were asked to read one story silently and move to the next page to answer questions about it, without rereading the story, then go to the next story, and so on. After this, children performed a letter cancellation task for 10 min. Then, the experimenter pronounced each of the six non-words and asked children to spell them as written in the texts they read. The experimenter explicitly informed children that they should not copy each other's spellings because they did not read the same texts. Furthermore, the presence of two adults in the classroom, the experimenter and children's teacher, allowed us to keep an eye on children.

The next day, the standardized spelling subtest was given by the experimenter. The judgment task was then given. Children were told that the experimenter had made up new words that no one had ever seen or heard before and that they would have to decide which made-up word is more like words they know. They were asked to look at the first practice pair and to circle that item on their booklet. This procedure was repeated with the remaining two practice items. Children received feedback for the practice items. They then went on to the test items, and here they were not told whether their responses were correct or incorrect.

Results

Non-word learning task

For each participant, we counted the number of spellings that contained only single consonants, the number of spellings that contained an initial doublet, and the number of spellings that contained a medial doublet. The mean values for the 24 participants, transformed into percentages, are shown in Table 2. Correct spellings, defined as those in which both target consonants (e.g., m and pp for the item muppile) were spelled as in the story, were more common on no-doublet items than on items presented with a doublet. Among the doublet items, correct spellings were more common for initial- than for medial-doublet items, as Table 2 shows. Analyses of variances (ANOVAs) on the number of correct spellings using subjects (F1) and items (F2) as random variables confirmed the main effect of item type [F1(2, 46) = 11.66, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.34; F2(2, 10) = 41.50, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.89]. Planned comparisons revealed significantly more correct spellings for no-doublet items (83.33%) than for the two types of doublet items [F1(1, 23) = 17.73, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.44; F2(1, 5) = 103.91, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.95] and significantly more correct spellings for initial- than for medial-doublet items [60.42 vs. 39.58%, F1(1, 23) = 5.37, p = 0.03, η2p = 0.19; F2(1, 5) = 14.81, p = 0.01, η2p = 0.75].

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Percentage of different types of spellings produced in Experiment 1.

We defined omission errors as those in which a doublet consonant was spelled with a singleton (e.g., mmupile or muppile misspelled as mupile). Transposition errors were defined as those that involved movement of the doubling feature to the wrong target consonant: the medial consonant instead of the initial consonant for the initial-doublet items (e.g., mmupile misspelled as muppile) and the initial consonant instead of the medial consonant for the medial-doublet items (e.g., muppile misspelled as mmupile). Misspellings of doublet items almost always involved omission of the doublet. There were only six transposition errors produced by three children, three on initial-doublet items and three on medial-doublet items.

Judgment task

Table 1 shows the scores in the test of graphotactic knowledge. Children showed very good knowledge that consonants cannot double in word initial position and that only some consonants can double. There was also evidence that children knew that double consonants can occur before but not after single consonants, although their knowledge about this property appeared to be less well-established. T-tests using subjects (t1) and items (t2) as random variables showed that the selection rate of legal items was significantly above chance (50%) for each of the three properties [t1s(23) > 6.35, ps < 0.001; t2s(7) > 9.51, ps < 0.001]. An ANOVA showed that the number of correct responses varied as a function of the type of graphotactic property [F1(2, 46) = 15.86, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.41; F2(2, 21) = 10.92, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.51]. Planned comparisons revealed lower scores for items assessing knowledge that double consonants can occur before but not after single consonants than for items assessing the two other properties [F1(1, 23) = 25.61, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.53; F2(1, 21) = 20.79, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.50], with no difference between items assessing which consonants can double and items assessing the fact that consonants cannot double in word-initial position (ps > 0.19).

Discussion

French third graders were better at recalling spellings that did not include doublets (e.g., mupile) than spellings that did. Among items with doublets, children performed better on those with an initial doublet (e.g., mmupile) than those with a medial doublet (e.g., muppile). These differences were found even though the children had seen the three types of spellings equally often in the texts they read. Transposition errors were rare (6 of 96 spellings produced), whether the doublet was positioned legally or illegally. Almost all of the misspellings involved omitting one consonant of a doublet.

The pattern of results in Experiment 1 is quite different from that found by Wright and Ehri (2007) with U.S. first graders. The children in that study produced fewer correct spellings for items with an initial doublet than for items with a final doublet, and they made transposition errors on initial-doublet items. However, our finding that items with a medial doublet were more often misspelled than items with an initial doublet fits well with other findings showing that misspellings are more common in the middles of words than at the beginnings or ends (e.g., Jensen, 1962; Stage and Wagner, 1992; Treiman et al., 1993).

The first graders tested by Wright and Ehri (2007) scored above chance, but not far above (67% correct), in choosing between a spelling with a doublet in the initial (illegal) position and a spelling with a doublet in the final (legal) position. The third graders of the present study chose spellings including a doublet in medial (legal) position far more often than spellings including a doublet in initial (illegal) position (91%). This confirms previous findings (e.g., Danjon and Pacton, 2009) that, in grade 3, French children's knowledge that doublets are illegal in word-initial position is well-established. Given this firmly established graphotactic knowledge, items like mmupile that include a graphotactic violation in initial position are orthographically distinctive for third graders. This orthographic distinctiveness may have captured third graders' attention. This helped them to remember both the presence and the position of the doublet, potentially explaining why they rarely transposed the doublet from the initial to the medial position.

Of course, there are several other reasons why the results of Experiment 1 may have differed from those reported by Wright and Ehri (2007). For example, the spellings were presented in isolation in Wright and Ehri's study but were embedded within stories in our study, the learning situation was intentional in Wright and Ehri's study but incidental in our study, and spellings were presented in uppercase letters in Wright and Ehri's study but in lowercase letters in our study. We return to this issue in the General Discussion.

Regardless of the potential role of these other variables, if the position of the graphotactic illegality, third graders' strong knowledge of this illegality, or both, capture children's attention, then children of the same age and spelling level should exhibit a pattern of performance more similar to that reported by Wright and Ehri (2007) for a graphotactic violation located in word-medial position, which is less likely to capture their attention. We investigated this issue in Experiment 2 by exploring whether French third graders' learning of spellings was influenced by their knowledge that consonants can double before, but not after, single consonants. Experiment 2 used the same procedure as Experiment 1, except that we modified the type of items embedded in the stories. Specifically, we used spellings without doublets (e.g., guprane), spellings with a doublet before a single consonant (e.g., gupprane), and spellings with a doublet after a single consonant (e.g., guprrane). The results of Experiment 1 and of Danjon and Pacton (2009) show that French third graders are sensitive to illegality of items like gupprane, but not as much as to the illegality of doublets in word-initial position. In Experiment 2, where the graphotactically illegal item is less distinctive and where all targets are located in the middles of the items, we expected that children would recall the legal spellings guprane and gupprane better than the illegal spelling guprrane and that they would sometimes spell the last as gupprane, transposing the doubling from an illegal to a legal position.

Experiment 2

Methods

Participants

The participants were 24 native speakers of French from an elementary school in Poitiers, France. The children were tested at the end of their third grade year. Table 1 provides information about their age, gender, and spelling ability.

Stimuli

Non-word learning task. We constructed six phonologically legal bisyllabic non-words: /dyflin/, /gypran/, /mifryes/, /nokril/, /toplir/, and /viklar/. Each non-word included a consonant cluster at the beginning of the second syllable. We created AB, AAB, and ABB spellings of each non-word, as in guprane (AB), gupprane (AAB), and guprrane (ABB). As in Experiment 1, the specific rendition of each non-word was counterbalanced across children so that (1) each child saw two AB spellings, two AAB spellings, and two ABB spellings; (2) each child saw only one spelling of a given non-word; and (3) each spelling of a given non-word was presented to eight children.

The non-words were embedded in the same stories as in Experiment 1, with the same constraints that two non-words were included in each story and that two non-words of the same type (i.e., AB, AAB, or ABB) were not included in the same story. Also as in Experiment 1, each non-word occurred five times in each story and the order of the stories and the non-words embedded in them were randomized across subjects. We used the same judgment task and general spelling test as in Experiments 1 and 2.

Procedure

The procedure was identical to that of Experiment 1.

Results

Non-word learning task

For each participant, we counted the number of spellings that contained only single consonants, the number of spellings that contained doublet before a single consonant, and the number of spellings that contained a doublet after a single consonant. Table 3 shows the mean values for the 24 participants, transformed into percentages. Correct spellings were more common for AB items than for items with a doublet, and among these items, correct spellings were more common for AAB (legal) than for ABB (illegal) items. ANOVAs on the number of correct spellings using subjects and items as random variables confirmed the main effect of item type [F1(2, 46) = 27.78, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.55; F2(2, 10) = 37.70, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.88]. Planned comparisons revealed significantly more correct spellings for AB items (87.5%) than for AAB and ABB items [F1(1, 23) = 39.66, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.63; F2(1, 5) = 148.00, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.97] and significantly more correct spellings for AAB than ABB items [52.1 vs. 14.6%, F1(1, 23) = 15.15, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.40; F2(1, 5) = 13.08, p = 0.015, η2p = 0.72].

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Percentages of AB, AAB, and ABB spellings produced in Experiment 2 as a function of type of spelling presented.

Omission errors were those in which a doublet consonant in AAB or ABB item was spelled with a singleton (e.g., gupprane or guprrane misspelled as guprane). Transposition errors involved movement of the doubling feature to the wrong target consonant: the first instead of the second consonant of the cluster for ABB items (e.g., guprrane misspelled as gupprane) and the second instead of the first consonant of the cluster for AAB items (e.g., gupprane misspelled as guprrane). Whereas omission errors were as frequent for AAB as for ABB items (41.7% and 50.0%, respectively), transposition errors were restricted to ABB items. Indeed, ten children made one (n = 8) or two (n = 2) transposition errors on ABB items, a rate of 25.0% transposition errors, whereas only one child made one transposition error on an AAB item (2.1%). While omission errors were equally common for AAB and ABB items [t1(23) = 0.81, p = 0.42; t2(5) = 0.21, p = 0.84], transposition errors were more common for ABB than AAB items [t1(23) = 3.11, p = 0.005; t2(5) = 4.00, p = 0.010].

Importantly, the larger number of transposition errors on ABB items than AAB items did not reflect a general trend to double the first consonant of the consonant cluster, irrespective of the type of item that was presented in the text. Indeed, children produced AAB spellings for ABB items both more often than for AB items and less often than for AAB items. An ANOVA on the number of AAB spellings revealed a main effect of item type (AB, AAB, and ABB) [F1(2, 46) = 14.71, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.39; F2(2, 10) = 14.11, p = 0.001, η2p = 0.74]. Planned comparisons showed that AAB spellings were more common for AAB items than for AB and ABB items [F1(1, 23) = 10.15, p = 0.004, η2p = 0.31; F2(1, 5) = 8.11, p = 0.036, η2p = 0.62] and that AAB spellings were more common for ABB items than for AB items [F1(1, 23) = 5.41, p = 0.029, η2p = 0.19; F2(1, 5) = 6.86, p = 0.047, η2p = 0.58].

Judgment task

Table 1 shows the scores in the judgment task used to assess children's graphotactic knowledge. The scores were very similar to those reported in Experiment 1. In particular, although children had knowledge about all three of the graphotactic properties assessed, their knowledge that consonants cannot double in word-initial position and that only some consonants can double appeared to be better established than their knowledge that consonants cannot double after single consonants. The selection rate of legal items was significantly above chance (50%) for each of the three properties [t1s(23) > 6.14, ps < 0.001; t2s(7) > 7.76, ps < 0.001]. However, the number of correct responses varied as a function of the type of graphotactic property [F1(2, 46) = 17.63, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.43; F2(2, 21) = 7.39, p = 0.004, η2p = 0.41]. Planned comparisons revealed lower scores for the items assessing knowledge that double consonants can occur before but not after single consonants than for the items assessing the two other properties [F1(1, 23) = 20.27, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.47; F2(1, 21) = 13.54, p = 0.001, η2p = 0.39]. The scores tended to be higher for items assessing the fact that consonants cannot double in word-initial position than for items assessing which consonants can be doubled, although this result was significant only by participants [F1(1, 23) = 7.27, p = 0.012, η2p = 0.24; F2(1, 21) = 1.24, p = 0.28, η2p = 0.06]. Finally, analyses comparing the scores of the participants in the two experiments for each of the three properties revealed no significant experiment effect (all ps > 0.13).

Discussion

French third graders were better at recalling spelling that did not contain doublets (e.g., guprane) than spellings that did. Among items with doublets, children showed better recall of those with a doublet before a single consonant (e.g., gupprane), which is legal in French, than those with a doublet after a single consonant, which is illegal in French (e.g., guprrane). Omission errors were the most frequent error for the two types of spellings containing doublets, and their frequency did not differ significantly according to whether the doublets were located in a legal (AAB items) or illegal position (ABB items). There were also some transposition errors, which were almost restricted to ABB items. The relatively frequent occurrence of transposition errors on ABB items, their uncommonness on AAB items, and the rare use of AAB spellings for AB items suggest that children sometimes remembered the presence of doubling but not the specific letter that was doubled. When this happened, children probably reconstructed a spelling based on their knowledge of which letters are most likely to double and in which positions. This reconstruction yielded correct spellings for AAB items but transposition errors for ABB items.

General Discussion

In two experiments, we investigated whether and how the learning of spellings by French third graders is influenced by two graphotactic patterns: consonants cannot double in word-initial position (Experiment 1) and consonants cannot double after single consonants (Experiment 2). To do this, we used an incidental orthographic learning task (Burt and Butterworth, 1996; Share, 1999, 2004; Burt and Furry, 2000; Nation et al., 2007). Children silently read meaningful texts that contained three types of novel spellings: those that did not contain any doublets (e.g., mupile, guprane), those with a doublet in a legal position (e.g., muppile, gupprane), and those with a doublet in an illegal position (e.g., mmupile, guprrane). Children were not instructed to remember the spellings, and their orthographic learning was subsequently assessed with a task of spelling to dictation.

In both experiments, children recalled items without doublets better than items with doublets. These results are consistent with previous findings about the difficulty of doublets for spellers of French (e.g., Manesse et al., 2007). This difficulty may arise, in part, because double consonants are less common than singletons. Children's difficulty in learning to spell items containing doublets may reflect the fact that spellings with common graphic patterns are easier to learn and remember than spellings with uncommon patterns.

Although the children in both experiments had more difficulty with doublets than singletons, other aspects of the results differed across the two experiments. In Experiment 1, children recalled spellings with an initial doublet (e.g., mmupile) better than spellings with a medial doublet (e.g., muppile). That is, they showed the surprising pattern of better performance on illegal than legal doublets. In Experiment 2, children recalled spellings with a doublet before a single consonant (e.g., gupprane) better than those with a doublet after a single consonant (e.g., guprrane). That is, they performed better on legal than illegal doublets. In both experiments, omission of the doublet was the most frequent error for both legal and illegal doublets. In Experiment 2, children also made some transposition errors on items with a doublet after a single consonant, recalling for example the legal gupprane instead of the illegal guprrane. Transpositions of a doublet from the illegal initial position to the legal medial position were uncommon in Experiment 1, however.

Why did the children in Experiment 1 perform better on illegal spellings like mmupile than on illegal spellings like muppile? The fact that illegality of an item like mmupile is in the salient initial position may be an important contributor. Children may have remembered the item with an illegal spelling pattern better than the item with a legal spelling pattern because the illegal pattern in the salient initial position captured their attention. It was only when we equated for position in Experiment 2, comparing legal and illegal spellings in the middles of the items, that we found better memory for legal spellings. This may help to explain why our results with initial doublets differed from those of Wright and Ehri (2007). The children in their study, who were kindergartners and first graders, had some knowledge about the illegality of items with initial doublets. However, this knowledge was not firmly established. The third graders in the present study had a strong knowledge that doublets cannot occur at the beginnings of words. This may have made the violation in items like mmupile very salient to them, helping them to remember both the presence and the position of the doublet.

Our third graders' relatively weak knowledge that consonants cannot double after single consonants may help to explain the transposition errors that we observed in Experiment 2. Although the children's graphotactic knowledge may have been sufficient in some cases to reconstruct the spelling of an item for which they remembered the presence of a doublet but not its position, it may have been insufficient to capture their attention when they read graphotactically illegal items like guprrane. Additional studies involving larger samples of participants who vary in literacy skill are needed to determine whether differences in the degree of graphotactic knowledge may explain why transposition errors are observed only for certain properties and/or only for certain individuals. Such studies should explore whether transposition errors are very rare among children who have a strong knowledge about a graphotactic property, as observed in Experiment 1, whereas biased transposition errors, in the direction of replacing illegal with legal spellings are made only by children who have an intermediate level of knowledge about a graphotactic property, as observed in Experiment 2. According to this view, children older than those involved in the present study or adults, who consistently express a preference for items such as gupprane over items such as guprrane in a judgment task would be expected to not commit transposition errors such as gupprane for guprrane. However, it is also possible that older children and adults would make such errors because, whereas the judgment task leads individuals to focus on the feature that distinguishes the non-words in a pair, namely the doublet either before or after the single consonant, reading may not require this level of analysis (e.g., Grainger and Whitney, 2004; Rayner et al., 2006).

We have suggested that the salience of the graphotactic violation in the initial position for third graders may help to explain why we found a different pattern of results in Experiment 1 than did Wright and Ehri (2007). However, there were a number of differences between our procedure and that of Wright and Ehri that could be important as well. One difference is that spellings were embedded in stories in our study but were presented in isolation in Wright and Ehri's study. We suspect that this is not a major reason why our Experiment 1 showed a different pattern of results than Wright and Ehri's experiment, however. Several studies have not found differences in orthographic learning according to whether spellings were seen in meaningful texts or in isolation (e.g., Cunningham, 2006; Nation et al., 2007; see also Wang et al., 2011, for the learning of items with regular spellings). Another difference is that the learning situation was incidental in our study but intentional in Wright and Ehri's study, and still another is that the items were presented in lowercase letters in our study but in uppercase letters in Wright and Ehri's study. Additional research is needed to determine whether these other factors are influential.

Although questions remain, our results show that learning to spell even in a deep orthography is not a matter of rote word-by-word memorization. By the end of third grade, French children show good knowledge of several important graphotactic patterns of their writing system. They appear to use these patterns to learn and remember the spellings of new words that they encounter while reading. Thus, the degree to which a word conforms to the patterns is an important determinant of spelling performance, above and beyond the amount of exposure to the word. Our results also suggest that the position of a letter or letter group plays an important role in memory for words' spellings. Thus, French third graders who encounter a novel item with a doublet in the hard-to-remember medial position sometimes remember that an item contained a doublet but not which letter was doubled. In such cases, they seem to use their knowledge of a graphotactic pattern—that double consonants can occur before but not after single consonants—and this sometimes leads them to transposition errors such as misspelling guprrane as gupprane. When a doublet is in the salient initial position, in contrast, third graders are better at remembering that the word contained a doublet and where the doublet occurred. Our results confirm that people use their general knowledge about the graphotactic patterns of their writing system, even when other sources of information would logically suffice to produce correct spellings (e.g., Kemp and Bryant, 2003; Pacton et al., 2005; Pacton and Deacon, 2008), and they shed new light on the conditions under which children do this.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part by ANR grant APPORTHO and NICHD Grant HD051610.

References

Bowman, M., and Treiman, R. (2002). Relating print and speech: the effects of letter names and word position on reading and spelling performance. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 82, 305–340. doi: 10.1016/S0022-0965(02)00101-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burt, J. S., and Butterworth, P. (1996). Spellings in adults: orthographic transparency, learning new letter strings, and reading accuracy. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 8, 3–43. doi: 10.1080/095414496383194

CrossRef Full Text

Burt, J. S., and Furry, M. B. (2000). Spelling in adults: the role of reading skills and experience. Read. Writ. Interdiscipl. J. 13, 1–30. doi: 10.1023/A:1008071802996

CrossRef Full Text

Cassar, M., and Treiman, R. (1997). The beginnings of orthographic knowledge: children's knowledge of double letters in words. J. Educ. Psychol. 89, 631–644. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.89.4.631

CrossRef Full Text

Chevrier-Muller, C., Simon, A. M., and Fournier, S. (1997). Batterie « Langage Oral, Langage Ecrit, Mémoire, Attention » L2MA (Manuel, Matériel, Cahier d'enregistrement) [Oral and Written Language Battery. Memory. Attention. L2MA (Manual, Material, Logbook)]. Paris: Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.

Cunningham, A. E. (2006). Accounting for children's orthographic learning while reading text: Do children self-teach? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 95, 56–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.03.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Danjon, J., and Pacton, S. (2009). “Children's learning about properties of double letters: the case of French,” in Presented at the 16th European Society for Cognitive Psychology Conference (Kraków: ESCOP).

Deacon, S. H., Conrad, N., and Pacton, S. (2008). A statistical learning perspective on children's learning about graphotactic and morphological regularities in spelling. Can. Psychol. 49, 118–124. doi: 10.1037/0708-5591.49.2.118

CrossRef Full Text

Ehri, L. C., and Saltmarsh, J. (1995). Beginning readers outperform older disabled readers in learning to read words by sight. Read. Writ. Interdiscipl. J. 7, 295–326. doi: 10.1007/BF03162082

CrossRef Full Text

Gounden, Y., and Nicolas, S. (2012). The impact of processing time on the bizarreness and orthographic distinctiveness effects. Scand. J. Psychol. 53, 287–294. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2012.00945.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grainger, J., and Whitney, C. (2004). Does the huamn mnid raed wrods as a whole? Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 58–59. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grainger, J., and Ziegler, J. C. (2011). A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Front. Psychol. 2:54. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00054

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hunt, R. R., and Worthen, J. B. (2006). Distinctiveness and Memory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195169669.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text

Jensen, A. R. (1962). Spelling errors and the serial-position effect. J. Educ. Psychol. 53, 105–109. doi: 10.1037/h0042141

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kemp, N., and Bryant, P. (2003). Do beez buzz? Rule-based and frequency-based knowledge in learning to spell plural -s. Child Dev. 74, 63–74. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00521

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kooi, B. Y., Schutz, R. E., and Baker, R. L. (1965). Spelling errors and the serial-position effect. J. Educ. Psychol. 56, 334–336. doi: 10.1037/h0022648

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lucci, V., and Millet, A. (1994). L'orthographe De tous les jours. Enquête sur les Pratiques Orthographiques des Français. [Everyday Spelling. Survey of Orthographic Practices of the French]. Paris: Champion.

Manesse, D., Chervel, A., and Cogis, D. (2007). Orthographe: à qui la Faute? [Spelling: Whose fault is it?]. Paris: ESF.

Mendenhall, J. E. (1930). The characteristics of spelling errors. J. Educ. Psychol. 21, 648–656. doi: 10.1037/h0073356

CrossRef Full Text

Nation, K., Angell, P., and Castles, A. (2007). Orthographic learning via self-teaching in children learning to read English: effects of exposure, durability, and context. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 96, 71–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.06.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pacton, S., and Deacon, S. H. (2008). The timing and mechanisms of children's use of morphological information in spelling: a review of evidence from French and English. Cogn. Dev. 23, 339–359. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.09.004

CrossRef Full Text

Pacton, S., Fayol, M., and Perruchet, P. (2005). Children's implicit learning of graphotactic and morphological regularities. Child Dev. 76, 324–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00848_a.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M., and Cleeremans, A. (2001). Implicit learning in real world context: the case of orthographic regularities. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130, 401–426. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.401

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rayner, K., White, S. J., Johnson, R. L., and Liversedge, S. P. (2006). Raeding wrods with jubmled lettres: there is a cost. Psychol. Sci. 17, 192–193. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01684.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Seymour, P., Aro, M., and Erskine, J. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. Br. J. Psychol. 94, 143–174. doi: 10.1348/000712603321661859

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: a direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 72, 95–129. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1998.2481

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Share, D. L. (2004). Orthographic learning at a glance: on the time course and developmental onset of self-teaching. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 87, 267–298. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.01.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2003). “Linguistic processes in reading and spelling. The case of alphabetic writing systems: English, French, German and Spanish,” in Handbook of Children's Literacy, eds T. Nunes and P. Bryant (Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers), 43–65.

Stage, S. A., and Wagner, R. K. (1992). Development of young children's phonological and orthographic knowledge as revealed by their spellings. Dev. Psychol. 28, 287–296. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.2.287

CrossRef Full Text

Treiman, R., Berch, D., and Weatherston, S. (1993). Children's use of phoneme-grapheme correspondences in spelling: roles of position and stress. J. Educ. Psychol. 85, 466–477. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.85.3.466

CrossRef Full Text

Treiman, R., Cohen, J., Mulqueeny, K., Kessler, B., and Schechtman, S. (2007). Young children's knowledge about printed names. Child Dev. 5, 1458–1471. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01077.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Treiman, R., Kessler, B., and Bick, S. (2002). Context sensitivity in the spelling of English vowels. J. Mem. Lang. 47, 448–468. doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00010-4

CrossRef Full Text

Wang, H. C., Castles, A., Nickels, L., and Nation, K. (2011). Context effects on orthographic learning of regular and irregular words. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 109, 39–57. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.11.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wright, D. M., and Ehri, L. C. (2007). Beginners remember orthography when they learn to read words: the case of doubled letters. Appl. Psycholinguistics 28, 115–133. doi: 10.1017/S0142716406070068

CrossRef Full Text

Zechmeister, E. B. (1972). Orthographic distinctiveness as a variable in word recognition. Am. J. Psychol. 85, 425–430. doi: 10.2307/1420843

CrossRef Full Text

Ziegler, J., and Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychol. Bull. 131, 3–29. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Appendix

Stimuli for Judgment Task

Practice pairs: vilo-xtyu, rsmt-baso, natu-gnhd.

Consonants cannot double in word-initial position: fannous-ffanous, furrois-ffurois, nnifor-niffor, nummar-nnumar, rrafout-raffout, rramin-rammin, ttamir-tammir, ttinot-tinnot.

Consonants cannot double after a single consonant: acclomir-acllomir, accriver-acrriver, affrunir-afrrunir, apllover-applover, apprulir-aprrulir, gabiffler-gabifller, mouffrive-moufrrive, rupllave-rupplave.

Only some consonants can double: akkoge-attoge, bekkul-befful, boxxit-bottit, onnave-ojjave, oxxile-ommile, tinnas-tihhas, tummet-tukket, ullate-ujjate.

Sample Story Used in Experiment 1

Autrefois, les habitants de la campagne se retrouvaient pour le muppile, la fête du village. C'était l'occasion de chanter, de danser et de déguster les spécialités de la région. Au muppile, la coutume était de s'amuser toute la nuit et d'attendre que le jour se lève pour partager un énorme gâteau au nifude. Le nifude est un fruit délicieux que l'on trouvait dans les forêts. Lors d'unmuppile, une drôle d'histoire est arrivée à Loura, une jeune villageoise curieuse et intrépide qui aimait faire des blagues. La fillette a voulu goûter le gâteau avant la fin du muppile parce qu'elle adorait le goût du nifude. Malheureusement, le gâteau se renversa. La pauvre fillette passa le reste de la nuit à chercher la plante qui donne le nifude, pour que sa mère prépare un autre gâteau. Finalement, personne ne se rendit compte de rien et le muppile se termina comme prévu. Le nifude fit encore beaucoup d'heureux.

(In the past, the people of the country met for a muppile, the party of the village. It was an opportunity to sing, dance, and savor the specialties of the area. At the muppile, people would play all night and wait for dawn in order to share a big cake made with nifude. Nifude is a delicious fruit that is found in forests. During a muppile, something funny happened to Loura, a young, curious, and intrepid villager who loved playing jokes. This young girl wanted to taste the cake before the end of the muppile because she loved the taste of nifude. Unfortunately, the cake fell over. The poor girl spent the rest of the night looking for the plant that provides the nifude so that her mother could make a new cake. In the end, nobody noticed anything and the muppile finished as anticipated. The nifude made a lot of people happy).

Keywords: spelling, graphotacics, implicit learning, statistical learning, orthographic learning

Citation: Pacton S, Sobaco A, Fayol M and Treiman R (2013) How does graphotactic knowledge influence children's learning of new spellings? Front. Psychol. 4:701. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00701

Received: 30 June 2013; Accepted: 14 September 2013;
Published online: 04 October 2013.

Edited by:

Sonia Kandel, Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition - CNRS UMR 5105, France

Reviewed by:

Alexandra Bendixen, Carl von Ossietzky University of Oldenburg, Germany
Sufen Chen, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, USA

Copyright © 2013 Pacton, Sobaco, Fayol and Treiman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sébastien Pacton, Laboratoire Mémoire et Cognition, Université Paris Descartes, 71 Avenue Edouard Vaillant, 92100 Boulogne Billancourt, France e-mail: sebastien.pacton@parisdescartes.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.