Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 19 December 2013
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Writing words: From brain to hand(s) View all 22 articles

The lognormal handwriter: learning, performing, and declining

  • 1Laboratoire Scribens, Département de Génie Électrique, École Polytechnique de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
  • 2Département de psychiatrie, Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
  • 3Département de Mathématiques et Informatique, LAMIA, Université des Antilles et de la Guyanne, Campus de Fouillole, Pointe-à-Pitre, Guadeloupe, France

The generation of handwriting is a complex neuromotor skill requiring the interaction of many cognitive processes. It aims at producing a message to be imprinted as an ink trace left on a writing medium. The generated trajectory of the pen tip is made up of strokes superimposed over time. The Kinematic Theory of rapid human movements and its family of lognormal models provide analytical representations of these strokes, often considered as the basic unit of handwriting. This paradigm has not only been experimentally confirmed in numerous predictive and physiologically significant tests but it has also been shown to be the ideal mathematical description for the impulse response of a neuromuscular system. This latter demonstration suggests that the lognormality of the velocity patterns can be interpreted as reflecting the behavior of subjects who are in perfect control of their movements. To illustrate this interpretation, we present a short overview of the main concepts behind the Kinematic Theory and briefly describe how its models can be exploited, using various software tools, to investigate these ideal lognormal behaviors. We emphasize that the parameters extracted during various tasks can be used to analyze some underlying processes associated with their realization. To investigate the operational convergence hypothesis, we report on two original studies. First, we focus on the early steps of the motor learning process as seen as a converging behavior toward the production of more precise lognormal patterns as young children practicing handwriting start to become more fluent writers. Second, we illustrate how aging affects handwriting by pointing out the increasing departure from the ideal lognormal behavior as the control of the fine motricity begins to decline. Overall, the paper highlights this developmental process of merging toward a lognormal behavior with learning, mastering this behavior to succeed in performing a given task, and then gradually deviating from it with aging.

Introduction

The generation of handwriting is a very complex neuromotor skill requiring the interaction of many cognitive processes. It aims at imprinting on a writing medium a message encoded in the trajectory of a pen tip, a trajectory ultimately made up of strokes superimposed over time (Thomassen et al., 1983; Kao et al., 1986; Plamondon et al., 1989; Van Galen, 1991). Taking a simplified sequential process approach (Plamondon et al., 1999), the production of a message can be seen as requiring the realization of numerous and diverse cognitive tasks. Starting from communication intents where the content of a message is elaborated, some semantic or cognitive networks must define and delimit the context of this message. Then various syntactic processes must activate the proper set of linguistic rules and interact with the specific lexicon of the selected language. The handwritten versions of the words in these lexicons are considered as being made up of allographs or character models. These models can be seen as ideal action plans that have been learned over the years. They can be instantiated to activate specific neuromuscular networks, producing a series of basic strokes superimposed over time– the fundamental units of handwriting movements—making up the intended pen tip trajectory.

The challenging and interesting element of handwriting studies is that they can be tracked from a broad spectrum of perspectives, from investigating high-level neurocognitive capabilities associated with language production down to the purely mechanical understanding of motor control gestures (Kandel et al., 2006; Bosga-Stork et al., 2011). Numerous researchers from various disciplines1 have been involved in studying some of these processes: neuroscience, experimental psychology, computer science and engineering, physics, education and developmental sciences, robotics, forensic science, paleontology and more. These studies were conducted using bottom-up or top-down approaches and exploit more or less sophisticated technologies, from complex fMRI investigations (Katanoda et al., 2001; Seitz, 2009; Richards et al., 2011; Shah et al., 2013) to simple non-invasive digitizer or instrumented pen signal analysis (Maarse, 1987; Baron and Plamondon, 1989; Teulings, 1996). Several studies deal with the fundamental understanding of the underlying neurocognitive and neuromotor processes (Thomassen and Teulings, 1983; Van Galen and Teulings, 1983; Van Galen, 1991) while others are primarily concerned with practical applications like on-line or off-line handwriting recognition (Nouboud and Plamondon, 1990; Plamondon et al., 1999; Plamondon and Srihari, 2000; Koerich et al., 2003; Lorigo and Govindaraju, 2006; Srihari et al., 2007; Tagougui et al., 2011), signature verification (Plamondon and Lorette, 1989; Leclerc and Plamondon, 1994; Pirlo and Impedovo, 2008; Impedovo et al., 2012; Plamondon et al., 2013a, 2014a), writer identification (Schomaker, 2007; Sreeraj and Idicula, 2011; Awaida and Mahmoud, 2012), and document analysis and processing (Doermann and Tombre, 2013). Many of these experiments rely in one way or another on a few basic models, and this is particularly true for investigations dealing with the motor control aspects of handwriting production. Indeed, many models have been proposed to study human movement control in general and handwriting in particular: models relying on neural networks (Bullock and Grossberg, 1988; Schomaker et al., 1989; Schomaker, 1991; Kalveram, 1998; Gangadhar et al., 2007), equilibrium point models (Feldman, 1966; Bizzi et al., 1978, 1992; Feldman and Latash, 2005), behavioral models (Thomassen et al., 1983; Van Galen and Teulings, 1983; Schmidt and Lee, 1999), coupled oscillator models (Hollerbach, 1981; Kelso, 1995; Zanone et al., 2005), emergent and convergent models (Plamondon, 1995a,b; Plamondon and Djioua, 2006), and models exploiting minimization principles (Wada and Kawato, 1995; Engelbrecht, 2001) including minimization of the acceleration (Neilson, 1993; Neilson and Neilson, 2005), of the energy (Nelson, 1983), of the time (Hermes and LaSalle, 1969; Enderle and Wolfe, 1987; Tanaka et al., 2006), of the jerk (Hogan, 1984; Flash and Hogan, 1985), of the snap (Edelman and Flash, 1987), of the torque changes (Uno et al., 1989) and of the sensory-motor noise (Harris and Wolpert, 1998). At the stroke level, many of these models exploit the properties of various analytic functions to reproduce and reconstruct human movements: exponentials (Plamondon and Lamarche, 1986), second order systems (Denier van der Gon and Thuring, 1965; Dooijes, 1983), Gaussians (Leclerc et al., 1992), beta functions (Alimi, 2003), splines (Morasso et al., 1983), and trigonometric functions (Hollerbach, 1981; Maarse, 1987).

Among the models providing analytical representations of the trajectories, the family of lognormal models predicted by the Kinematic Theory of rapid human movements (Plamondon, 1995a,b, 1998; Plamondon et al., 2003; Plamondon and Djioua, 2006) has been used to explain most of the basic phenomena reported in classic studies on human motor control (Plamondon and Alimi, 1997) and to study several factors involved in fine motricity (Djioua and Plamondon, 2009a; O'Reilly and Plamondon, 2011; Woch et al., 2011). Apart from these fundamental studies, these models have also been used, directly or indirectly, in many practical applications like the design of a signature verification system (Plamondon, 1994), the development of tools to help children learn handwriting (Carrières and Plamondon, 1994; Djeziri et al., 2002), the generation of synthetic signatures and gestures databases for algorithm testing or classifier learning (Almaksour et al., 2011; Galbally et al., 2012a,b) as well as the design of biomedical set-ups to detect fine motor control problems associated with brain stroke risk factors (O'Reilly and Plamondon, 2011, 2012a,b).

In this paper, we push one step further the lognormality concept to point out how it can be used to provide a global estimate of the performance of a handwriter. We begin by presenting a brief overview of the Kinematic Theory of rapid human movements to particularly illustrate how its family of lognormal models can be seen as describing human beings when they are in perfect control of their movements. We then give a list of software tools that we have developed over the years to automatically extract the model parameters from handwriting. Typical examples of original and reconstructed trajectory patterns are presented. References are also given to specific studies dealing with how the theory can be used to reveal the conditions for a lognormal handwriter to successfully execute a required trajectory. Continuing along this paradigm, we look at motor learning as a shift toward a lognormal behavior, a conduct that we then master and exploit for a large part of our life and that we slowly see decrease as we get older. To further investigate this interpretation of the lognormality, we look at some aspects of the move toward lognormality by analyzing the handwriting of young kindergarten children, 3 to 5 years old, to emphasize how they improve the control of their fine motricity as they perform typical learning lessons. Using a similar presentation scheme, we also study the move away from lognormality by analyzing aging effects on handwriting. Typical changes observed in the model-based descriptions are reported, illustrating the deviation from lognormal behavior. The paper concludes by briefly summarizing the results and exploring some possible extensions of this approach to study health problems (Parkinson disease, Alzheimer's disease, brain stroke) and rehabilitation therapy.

Materials and Methods

Defining and Taking Advantage of Lognormality

The Kinematic Theory describes a neuromuscular network involved in handwriting production as a linear system that controls the velocity of the pen tip. Assuming that such a system is made up of numerous coupled subsystems and that this coupling can be expressed with proportionality relationships between the cumulative time delays associated with the activation of these subsystems, the theory predicts, using the Central Limit Theorem, that the magnitude of the velocity profiles produced by the global system will tend toward a lognormal pattern (Plamondon, 1995a,b, 1998; Plamondon et al., 2003):

| vi(t;Pi) |=DiΛi(t0i;μi,σi2)             = Diσi2π(tt0i)exp [ [ ln(tt0i)μi ]22σi2 ](1)

where the set of parameters Pi = [Di, t0 i, μi, σi] describing a lognormal pulse refers to:

Di: the input command, which is the intended distance to be covered with the pulse;

t0 i: the time occurrence of that command, as instantiated in the central nervous system (CNS);

μi: the log time delay (the time delay on a logarithmic time scale);

σi: the log response time (the response time on a logarithmic time scale).

The production of a given stroke can thus, be seen as the process of recruiting a sufficient number of time-coupled neuromuscular units to produce the most perfect lognormal profile. These lognormal functions are the basic primitives, the elementary strokes that can be used to produce any complex pen tip trajectory. Under this paradigm, handwriting generation starts with the instantiation of an action plan made up of virtual targets linked together by circular arcs. This planning space, which can be modeled with a grid of leaky integrators simulating learning neurons (Plamondon and Privitera, 1995), acts as a command generator, each command producing a lognormal stroke. A sequence of commands results in a vector summation process where the velocities of individual strokes are superimposed over time to produce a given pattern (a letter, a word, a signature, a gesture, and so forth) (Plamondon and Guerfali, 1998). The magnitude and direction of the velocity of a given trajectory are thus, described by a Sigma-Lognormal equation (Plamondon and Djioua, 2006):

v(t)=i=1Nvi(t;t0i,μi,σi2)=i=1NDi[ cos(θi(t))sin(θi(t)) ]Λi((t;t0i,μi,σi2);N2(2)
θi(t)=θsi+(θeiθsi)2[ 1+erf(ln(tt0i)μiσi2) ](3)

where θsi and θei stand, respectively, for the starting and ending angular direction of each discontinuous stroke, as ideally represented in the action plan. When only straight line movements are produced, Equation 2 reduces to what is called a Delta-Lognormal equation:

v(t)=D1Λ(t;t0,μ1,σ12)D2Λ(t;t0,μ2,σ22)(4)

where the subscripts 1 and 2 refer, respectively, to the agonist and antagonist neuromuscular networks.

From a validation point of view, apart from its outstanding performances in reproducing handwriting under various conditions (Plamondon et al., 1993, 2014a; Alimi and Plamondon, 1994; Plamondon and Alimi, 1997; Feng et al., 2002), as well as making clear predictions regarding the conditions under which the various speed-accuracy trade-offs emerge (Plamondon and Alimi, 1997)2, two decisive experiments have provided functional and biomedical supports to the theory and confirmed its underlying hypotheses and its physiological significance. On the one hand, to get a lognormal convergence, the subsystems constituting a neuromuscular network must be synchronized in such a way that the cumulative time delays of the command propagating along a given network must obey proportionality relationships. These proportional effects have been clearly observed between various pairs of upper arm muscles involved in the production of rapid movements using electromyography (EMG) (Plamondon et al., 2013b). On the other hand, when a lognormal is observed, the theory presumes that it is the result of a command that has been activated in the CNS at a given time t0 (see Equation 1). This prediction has also been confirmed using electroencephalography (EEG), where it has been shown that a specific evoked response potential (ERP) was produced at t0 (O'Reilly et al., 2013).

Several software packages have been developed over the years to extract the lognormal parameters from various velocity curves under different experimental data acquisition conditions and set-ups (Guerfali and Plamondon, 1998; Djioua et al., 2007; Plamondon et al., 2007; Djioua and Plamondon, 2009b; O'Reilly and Plamondon, 2009a, 2010, 2012c), and most up-to-date algorithms have been implemented in ScriptStudio research software. These different tools allow a researcher to study handwriting through the parametric lognormal description of the neuromuscular networks involved in a given task (Plamondon et al., 2009; Plamondon, 2013). Starting with the pen tip position [x(t), y(t)] as sampled by a digitizer, these software tools compute the velocity vector and, using different optimization algorithms, extract the lognormal parameters of the strokes that best describe the sampled trajectory. Figure 1 presents three typical examples of velocity reconstruction. In 1A, a single stroke is reconstructed using a Delta-Lognormal equation (Equation 4), while in 1B a complex graphic trace has been reconstructed using a Sigma-Lognormal equation (Equations 2 and 3) resulting in the speed profile shown in 1C. In 1D and 1E, similar patterns are shown for the handwritten character “a.” In each case, the parameter extraction algorithm provides the values of the lognormal parameters that have been found to automatically reconstruct a profile, the number of lognormals (nbLog) required to reconstruct the signal as well as a measure of the quality of the reconstruction as evaluated by computing the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) between the original vo(t) and the reconstructed vr(t) profiles:

SNR=10log (tste[ vox2(t)+voy2(t) ]dttste[ (vox(t)vrx(t))2+(voy(t)vry(t))2 ]dt)(5)

where the global effect of the distortions is computed using the velocity components vx (t) and vy (t) from the beginning ts to the end te of these Cartesian signals.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Example of the speed profile of a delta-lognormal modeling for a typical fast reaching movement. (B,C) Example of the trace (B) and the speed profile (C) of the sigma-lognormal modeling of a typical triangular movement. (D,E) Example of the trace (D) and the speed profile (E) of the sigma-lognormal modeling of a typical handwritten letter “a.”

In other words, with these optimization tools, for each handwriting trace produced by a subject, the Kinematic Theory provides a new parametric representation space to study its motor control behavior. This offers a new window into studying human movements, where some specific strategies for succeeding at a given task can be pointed out, these strategies relying on how the lognormality is mastered and exploited.

For example, it has been demonstrated that strong coupling of agonist and antagonist neuromuscular networks were necessary to produce a single fast stroke with a direction reversal without a pause at the breaking point (Woch and Plamondon, 2010). This resulted in very high correlations between the agonist and antagonist lognormal parameters extracted from each individual trajectory (Woch et al., 2011). Similarly, analyzing a Fitts' task under the paradigm of the Kinematic Theory, it has been shown that the subjects had to correlate more tightly the impulse commands sent to the agonist and antagonist neuromuscular systems in order to achieve good performances as the difficulty of the task increases whereas the correlation in the timing of the neuromuscular action co-varied with the size of the trajectory's geometrical properties (O'Reilly and Plamondon, 2013).

Overall, the theory has not only been experimentally supported in numerous predictive and physiologically significant tests but it has also been shown to be the ideal mathematical model to describe the impulse response of a neuromuscular system (Djioua and Plamondon, 2010), which results in what is known as “asymmetric bell-shaped velocity profiles.” This mathematical demonstration suggests that the asymptotic convergence toward lognormal impulse responses and velocity patterns can be interpreted as reflecting the behavior of subjects who are in total control of their movements. In this context, if we specifically focus on the basic mathematical convergence toward lognormality, handwriting learning, on the one hand, can be interpreted as a migration toward an ideal control of perfectly mastered movements. In other words, we can account for the lognormality of the underlying neuromotor processes as a convergence toward the complete mastering of a given task. On the other hand, aging should reveal a progressive decrease of the fine motor control, as reflected by a departure from lognormality.

Additionally, from a mathematical point of view, the Kinematic Theory is a theory of convergence toward smoothness. The lognormal function is an optimal descriptor of the velocity profiles: the smoothest velocity being reached when the energy associated with the convergence error toward lognormality is minimized (Djioua and Plamondon, 2010). As such, the Kinematic Theory can be considered as an ultimate minimization theory. We strengthen this statement in the following sections by investigating two phases of this process: characterizing handwriting learning as a move toward lognormality and characterizing aging as a move away from lognormality. To do so, we utilize the Kinematic Theory to reconstruct various handwriting samples and analyze them using three performance criteria: the SNR of the reconstructed pattern as defined in Equation 5, the nbLog used to make that reconstruction, and the SNR/nbLog ratio which can be seen as a global indicator of a given writer's motor control skills.

Experiment 1: Learning

Moving toward lognormality

The mastering of fast writing and drawing movements is the norm in healthy adults with full control of their neuromuscular system (Zesiger, 1995; Karldottir, 1996; Senatore and Marcelli, 2012), but this is not the case for young children. According to many studies, the automation of handwriting skill by children is the usual outcome of a non-monotonous learning phase which takes around 10 years (Meulenbroek and Van Galen, 1988; Vinter and Mounoud, 1991; Albaret and Santamaria, 1996; Zesiger et al., 2000; Frélicot et al., 2002; Chartrel and Vinter, 2004). In the context of the Kinematic Theory, it is after this period that lognormality should be well established and should begin to be fully exploited. Indeed, it has been shown that rapid movements produced by young adults can be almost perfectly modeled as sums of lognormal vectors (Guerfali and Plamondon, 1998; O'Reilly and Plamondon, 2009a,b). When children are still in the early steps of their learning process, is it expected that a lognormal behavior should be already discernible.

The goal of the present study was to determine if the SNR of the reconstructed pattern, the nbLog used to make a reconstruction and the SNR/nbLog ratio could be good indicators of the progress of children's performances in early phases of scolarship (PS, MS, GS).

Participants

The subjects that have been chosen for this preliminary study had reached the best scriptural behavior according to three early grade criteria of kindergarten, and as such were considered as good writers according to their grade. Such choice is necessary to try to limit a priori the effects of some factors that can influence a child's handwriting skills like handwriting learning difficulties. To select such samples of children for each school level we proceeded as follows: during the 2011–2012 school year, a preliminary group of 66 pupils was randomly selected within the population of a Guadeloupe kindergarten to take part in preliminary data acquisition. These children were from three school grades: the PS level was grouping 3- and 4-year-old participants; the MS level, the 4- and 5-year-olds and the GS level, the 5- and 6-year-olds. The members of the PS, MS, and GS grades had 6, 18, and 30 months of handwriting lessons, respectively. This preliminary study was necessary for screening, from a large pool of possible tasks, those that had the higher discrimination potential. However, data collected at this stage were not adequate for statistical analysis since only a few samples, generally three to five, could be collected for each kind of movement, to avoid exposing young children to large number of trials. Given the high variability of human motor productions, such a small number of repetitions per children and per movement types was not sufficient to provide stable averages of children neuromotor characteristics.

Thereafter, for each of the three grades, five children were selected from this first group as participants in the present study. This selection was performed according to three inclusion criteria. First, the child's teacher had pointed out that in normal lessons the child was interested in handwriting and drawing, and had demonstrated satisfying performances in these activities. Second, the teacher had certified that the child had completed the two drawing and the two writing activities proposed to all the children in the group during normal classroom hours. These first two criteria confirmed that each child's performance was in compliance with the French kindergarten program expectations for their specific group level. The third criterion dealt with the results of the analysis of the child's performance at the first data acquisition phase. The teacher and the experimenter had found that the child was motivated to take part in the experiment and that the child had produced all the required trajectories (about 40). The different trajectories that the child had produced for the first data acquisition had been reconstructed on average with a SNR >25 dB and with an optimal nbLog for a given pattern consistent with the ideal sigma-lognormal model. This second experimental stage was necessary for producing data that are adequate for statistical modeling (i.e., a large number of repetitions per children for a small subset of highly discriminative tasks). For ethical reasons and given the repetitive (and possibly boring) nature of this experiment performed by very young children, only the most interested children had to be selected, resulting in a reduction from the initial pool of 66 children to a subset of 15 highly interested children for the second stage.

For each of these three groups (GS, MS, and PS), the five selected participants were thus, a priori considered as being able to successfully execute the required tasks and as having the most stable level of performance with respect to their own level of learning and expertise.

Procedure and apparatus

During the first acquisition phase, selected children were asked to produce their movements on a Wacom Intuos3 tablet and the kinematic of the motion was digitized at 200 Hz. Eleven patterns (Duval, 2012) had to be performed: an oblique trace, five pseudo-letters, and three cursive letters (l, p, and r). It must be noted that each child prior to participating to the study had been given the chance to familiarize himself or herself with the equipment, practicing the required patterns under the supervision of a teacher.

A conclusion of the first acquisition phase was that among the 11 tested patterns, the most successful ones were the oblique traces (/) and the bridge movements (∩) (Duval et al., 2013). Thus, these two tasks were selected for the next stage that aimed to investigate a potential migration toward lognormality. For this second acquisition phase, the requirement was to rapidly produce an oblique trajectory, starting from a common origin and reaching a given fixed target, and a bridge trace, starting from a common origin and reaching a given fixed target after passing over an intermediate target. Each pair of trajectories was repeated 30 times by every participant, except for a few very young PS participants who had difficulty staying concentrated on the task. Due to these difficulties among the youngest children, 25 movements were missing. A few movements (8) were also lost, having been inadvertently destroyed by the experimenter during the acquisition.

Signal processing and statistical analyses

Each x(t) and y(t) trajectory produced by a participant was input into the ScriptStudio software package for automatic lognormal segmentation and optimal parameter extraction. These parameters were then used to reconstruct the velocity profile of the original trajectory. The three performance criteria (SNR, nbLog, and SNR/nbLog) were computed from this reconstruction process. To observe if some of the performance criteria could show differences between young writers of GS, MS, and PS, we tested whether our three criteria were uniformly distributed in the different classes of writers (GS, MS, and PS), answering each of the following questions:


(Q1) Is the quality of reconstruction (SNR) evenly distributed among all classes of writers?

(Q2) Is average of the nbLog the same for all classes of writers?

(Q3) Is average of the ratio SNR/nbLog the same for all classes of writers?

(Q4) Is average of the SNR the same for all classes of writers?

We have analyzed the distribution of SNR in a contingency table and the averages of nbLog, of the ratio SNR/nbLog and SNR on our raw data.

Experiment 2: Aging

Moving away from lognormality

Numerous experiments have shown that movements become slower and less coordinated when people get older (Contreras-Vidal et al., 1998; Ketcham and Stelmach, 2004; Barry et al., 2005; Robinovitch et al., 2005) but it is still unclear if this is due to a motor system deterioration or if it is the result of compensatory strategies (Latash and Anson, 1996; Heuninckx et al., 2008). In a previous study (Woch et al., 2011), we asked seven subjects aged 63 to 70 and seven subjects aged 26 to 29 to produce handwriting strokes on a digitizer, in response to an audio stimulus. The subjects were instructed to make bidirectional strokes [i.e., delta-lognormal strokes that exhibit a significant return in their trajectory (Woch and Plamondon, 2010)] as fast as possible and with their dominant hand. Three of the older subjects did not produce enough bidirectional primitives reconstructed with a SNR >15 dB3 and were excluded from the subsequent data analysis. This investigation exposed a substantial increase in neuromuscular response delays and a decrease in command amplitudes with age. Both the agonist and antagonist systems were similarly affected. Furthermore, it was observed that age had a proportional effect on the various time characteristics of the movements. Among other things, this experiment pointed out that, even in the case of a significant slowing down of the neuromuscular systems, the elderly could still achieve optimal movement responses, characterized by the reconstruction of their gestures with a single delta-lognormal primitive, similar to those produced by young healthy subjects. The number of successful attempts was smaller in the older group. These preliminary results indicated a mathematical depiction of age-related movement alterations.

In the present study, we used reaching movements and triangular drawings to illustrate how aging phenomena affect handwriting, pointing out the increasing departure from the ideal lognormal behavior as the control of fine motricity begins to decline. In this experiment, reaching movements are investigated because they are one of the most elementary types of movement normally involving a single stroke. They can be modeled using a delta-lognormal function. These delta-lognormal movements are considered as a fundamental primitive used in synergies to compose more complex patterns such as those used in handwriting and in drawing. As for the triangular movements, they are investigated because they constitute a relatively simple task requiring the coordination of at least four stroke primitives, each one described by a single lognormal (see Figure 1C).

Participants

Two participant samples were studied to analyze the effect of age in healthy and less healthy populations. The first sample (hereafter labeled NRF for “no risk factor”) contained 29 women and 28 men, with age varying between 25 and 87 years old. It corresponds to the control subset of a sample of 120 subjects which participated in a study on the impact of brain stroke risk factors on movement kinematics (O'Reilly and Plamondon, 2010, 2011, 2013). The age distribution of the considered subsamples can be seen in Figure 2A. Participants were considered healthy, had no brain stroke history, and had none of the following brain stroke risk factors: alcoholism, cigarette smoking (CS), obesity (OB), hypertension (HT), hypercholesterolemia (HC), cardiac disease (CD), and diabetes (DM). The second sample (hereafter labeled WRF for “with risk factor”) was constituted of 39 women and 24 men, with age also varying between 25 and 87 years old. Each of these participants had at least one of the previously listed risk factors, except for alcoholism which was reported by none of the subjects. The age and gender distribution as well as the risk factor distribution within this sample can be seen, respectively, in Figures 2B,C.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Distribution of age and gender for the NRF subsample. (B) Idem, but for the WRF subsample. (C) Distribution of the risk factors in the WRF subsample. The height of the bars shows the overall number of subjects with each risk factor. As subjects may have more than one risk factor simultaneously, the bars are separated into colored sections indicating how many risk factors the subjects have. For example, 40 subjects have HT (height of the HT bar); among these, about 8 subjects have only the HT (the light gray portion of the HT bar) whereas one subject who has HT also has 4 other risk factors, for a total of 5 (the red portion of the HT bar).

As can be appreciated in Figures 2A,B, the distribution of gender and age appears to be distributed independently. This is confirmed by Kruskal–Wallis tests showing that the average age of men and women is not statistically different in both samples (NRF: W = 344, p = 0.3258; WRF: W = 412, p = 0.4319). Thus, gender is omitted in the following analyses since the effect of age and gender cannot be confounded, the gender being reasonably balanced with respect to age.

Participants in both samples were volunteers from the École Polytechnique community or patients from Hôpital De Réadaptation Villa Medica. They all gave informed written consent. The experimental protocol was approved by the Ethics Boards of École Polytechnique and Hôpital de Réadaptation Villa Medica.

Procedure and apparatus

Participants were submitted to a test battery of nine experiments. The full experimental protocol can be seen in (O'Reilly, 2012; O'Reilly and Plamondon, 2012b; Plamondon et al., 2014b). For the present analysis, we considered two sets of acquisitions. The first one contains the data of three reaction time experiments, two on simple stimulus (auditory and visual) and one on choice stimulus (visual). A targeted number of 15 valid samples were collected for the tasks using simple stimulus and 30 for the choice stimulus. The choice stimuli were leftward and rightward arrows—chosen at random—indicating the requested direction for the reaching movement. The targeted zones were very large such that no precision was required. The movement had to be performed on a Wacom Intuos2 tablet and the kinematic of the motion was digitized at 200 Hz. Figures 3A,B show the sheets that were placed under the transparent folding of the tablet to guide the subjects. A movement amplitude of at least 130 mm was asked for in reaction to simple stimuli, 38 mm for choice stimuli. The laps of time between the instant the subject took place at the starting position and the emission of the stimulus was randomly distributed following a flat hazard function (i.e., the exponential distribution), with delays varying between 0 and 10 s.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Guiding sheets used for the simple reaction time experiments (A), the choice reaction time protocol (B), and the triangular drawings (C–E). The starting position is shown as a dark circle and the target zones as gray areas. For triangular drawing sheets, the targets are 15 mm in diameter and are positioned at the apexes of equilateral triangles with vertexes of 135 mm (C), 90 mm (D), and 45 mm (E) long.

The second set of movements consisted of triangular drawings arranged in a factorial design, with three triangle sizes, two drawing orientations, and two repetitions. Triangles were constituted of a series of three targets to be hit sequentially and in a continuous trajectory with the movement coming to a full stop within the last target. Figures 3C–E shows the guiding sheets that were used for this task.

Signal processing and statistical analyses

For fast movements performed in response to stimuli, such as in reaction time tests, the delta-lognormal (Equation 4) is the most appropriate type of modeling and has thus, been adopted for this investigation. The system proposed by (Djioua and Plamondon, 2009b) cascaded with a supplementary non-linear optimization step was used to obtain the delta-lognormal parameters from the digitized movements. To test the hypothesis that, with normal aging, motor performances migrate away from a lognormal behavior, we studied the relationship between the age and the SNR of the delta-lognormal reconstruction. Since the nbLog is fixed de facto to two components in a delta-lognormal model, the two other performance criteria (nbLog and SNR/nbLog) were not useful here.

For triangular movements, a sigma-lognormal modeling using the Robust X0 algorithm (O'Reilly and Plamondon, 2009a) was adopted. Our three response variables (SNR, nbLog, and SNR/nbLog) were studied in relation with the movement direction and size as well as with the subject age as descriptive variables. For statistical analysis, results of repetitions were averaged, giving six outcomes per subject following the factorial plan of three sizes by two orientations. All three response variables were modeled with linear regression using our three descriptive variables, as well as the interaction between the subjects' age and the interaction between the triangles' size and drawing orientation.

Results

Learning

On children's trend to migrate toward a lognormal behavior

As seen in Figure 4A, for all oblique traces and bridges, the more advanced in the writing learning phase the writers are, the better is the reconstruction of their trajectories on average. To answer the question Q1 we performed the chi square test to check the hypothesis of independence between the SNR and the three different classes of writers χ2(2, N = 438) = 9.463. The result was significant (p = 0.009). So, we conclude that the quality of the reconstruction depends on the grade of writers A detailed comparison of the grade of writers per pairs showed, as reported in Table 1, that the quality of the reconstruction is dependent on the classes for the class of GS and PS χ2(1, N = 291) = 8.690, p = 0.003, respectively, MS and PS χ2(1, N = 288) = 4.919, p = 0.027 but it is independent of the grade for GS and MS χ2(1, N = 297) = 0.539, p = 0.463. Thus, the variable SNR can translate the migration to a lognormal behavior between writers groups PS and GS, and PS and MS.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. SNR (A), nbLog (B), and SNR/nbLog ratio (C) as functions of the writer class for all movements.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Test on classes for the SNR for oblique traces.

To answer the question Q2 we performed the Kruskal–Wallis test to verify whether the average number of lognormals, nbLog, was the same in the three classes of writers. The results were significant as shown in Table 2 (p-value = 2.025e-21), indicating a difference in averages. To confirm these trends, we performed Mann–Witney tests to verify whether the nbLog was the same in the classes of writers taken in pairs (GS, MS), (GS, PS), and (MS, PS). As shown in the same Table 2, results indicate that averages were significantly different (all p < 0.01) when using an α = 0.05 corrected at 0.0083 for multiple comparisons, using the Bonferonni approach.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Test on classes nbLog, SNR/nbLog, and SNR for oblique traces.

To answer the question Q3 we performed the same tests to check whether the distribution of the SNR/nbLog ratio was similar in the three classes of writers on the one hand and in all the classes of writers compared by pair on the other hand. Results indicate that all distribution had significantly different averages (Table 2) for the three classes of writers considered together (p-values = 4.071e-11) as well as for each class taken by pair except for the GS-MS pair. Finally to answer the question Q4, we performed the same tests to check whether the distribution of the SNR was similar in the three classes of writers and in all the classes of writers compared by pair. Results indicate that the distributions had significantly different averages (Table 2) for the three classes of writers considered together (p-values = 8.60e-3) as well as for the pairs of classes GS-PS, MS-PS, (p-values = 2.21e-4/0.0165). Only the distribution of the SNR for GS and MS was similar (p-value = 0.1742), which suggests that after 1 year of handwriting learning in the PS group, the children have already made substantial progresses. When they reach the MS and the GS groups, the quality of the reconstruction of their trajectories become similar but still differ in term of the number of lognormal needed to reconstruct these. In short, the box plots of the Figures 4B,C show that for classes MS, GS, the more the writers advance in their writing learning phase, the more they tend control their lognormal behavior. That is, they use fewer lognormals to perform a trace and their ability to control their motor system, as assessed by the ratio SNR/nbLog, is improved.

Aging

Reaching movements

Figure 5 shows, for both NRF and WRF samples, scatter plots displaying the relationship between the numbers of movements associated with a poorer delta-lognormal modeling according to the SNR < λ rule for the λ taken in decreasing order as 30, 25, 20, and 15 dB. In these graphs, a positive slope indicates that older participants have more difficulty producing neat delta-lognormal movements, suggesting poorer motor control.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Scatter plots of the proportion of rejected movements when considering the rejection criterion SNR < λ, for different values of λ (in dB). A logistic regression curves has been added to the plots to show the average tendency. The plots (A,C,E,G) refer to the population with No Risk Factor (NRF) having a SNR < 30,25, 20 and 15 dB, respectively. The plots (B,D,F,H) refer to the population With Risk Factor (WRF) under the same SNR conditions.

A logistic regression was performed to model the variation of the proportion of low SNR movements as a function of the age. The best fitting curve was added on the plots of Figure 5. Modeling coefficients and associated p-values are reported in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Modeling coefficients and associated p-values computed for logistic modeling of data shown in Figure 5.

As can be seen by the positive age coefficients in Table 3, regardless of the selected λ threshold, there is a significant overall increase of proportion of low-SNR movements with age. These data clearly support a positive relationship between age and the proportion of low-SNR movements.

The relationship between age and the delta-lognormal movement can also be appreciated in Figure 6, which shows a scatter plot of the robust average SNR as function of the subjects' age. To compute this average, SNR from the three reaction time tests was pooled since there were no statistically significant differences between the SNR obtained in these tests (p > 0.05). The Minimum Volume Ellipsoid algorithm (Rousseeuw and Leroy, 1987) was used for robust computation.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Scatter plot linking the average SNR to the subjects' age. The lines show the best linear models associated with these data. The plot (A) is for the population with No Risk Factor (NRF) and the plot (B) for the population With Risk Factor (WRF).

Using a linear model to regress the SNR toward the age, a significant effect of the age factor is obtained [NRF: F(1, 55) = 4.488, p = 0.039; WRF: F(1, 61) = 14.30, p = 0.00038], with a large variability causing a low coefficient of determination (R2NFR = 0.075, R2WFR = 0.19). Overall, the effect of age on the deterioration of the motor control causing a decrease of the SNR is supported by these data and the hypothesis of a migration away from a lognormal behavior with the aging process is clearly corroborated.

Triangular drawings

A regression analysis similar to the one performed in section Reaching Movements was performed for the data from triangular drawings, with the difference that beside age, the impact of the drawing orientation and triangle size are also modeled4. Using α = 0.05 as threshold for statistical significance, only age (pNFR = 0.03, pWFR = 0.0001) and triangle size (pNFR = 0.03, pWFR = 0.003 for the difference between large and medium triangles; pNFR = 6e−15, pWFR < 2−16 for the difference between large and small triangles) were found significant for the SNR, with higher SNR in older subjects and for smaller triangular movements.

For nbLog and SNR/nbLog, only age (pNFR = 1e−6, pWFR = 8e−11 for nbLog; pNFR = 0.0002, pWFR = 2e−11 for SNR/nbLog) is significant in both samples. For the WRF sample, the size difference of drawings also has a significant impact when comparing small triangles with large ones for SNR/nbLog (pWFR = 0.02).

A second analysis was performed on averaged values of response variables aggregated per subject, regardless of movement properties (i.e., triangle size and orientation). Estimated effects and p-values are reported in Table 4. P-values are of course less significant for this sigma-lognormal analysis than for the previous regressions on the delta-lognormal data because the sample size is smaller. However, the estimated statistical significances for demographic factors are more reliable because there is no repeated measurement in this analysis.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Modeling coefficients and associated p-values computed for the demographic factors.

As shown in this table, the SNR is not a discriminative factor here. All the subjects were able to carry out the expected task and the ScriptStudio software was able to reconstruct all the trajectories with very high SNR. It is the nbLog, and albeit to a lesser extent SNR/nbLog, that are more relevant. In other words, although the subjects were successful in this specific experiment, this success was obtained at the expense of using more lognormal components to execute their movements.

Discussion

Learning

The lognormal performance criteria allowed us to observe a tendency in the young writers to move toward a lognormal behavior as characterized by the three criteria used in this study. In fact, although the participants were early in their learning of the writing process, each of the three criteria (SNR, nbLog, and SNR/nbLog) allow us to observe that the more children advance in their learning, the more their movements tend to have the lognormal scriptural characteristics of better mastered graphomotricity. This phenomenon is more evident with the drawing of oblique traces than for the bridges, which are a more complex shape. From a global and operational perspective, such results can be interesting when considering the definition of rapid tests for the evaluation of young children's motor control abilities in school.

Aging

The hypothesis that age results in a divergence from lognormality is supported by our study of fast reaching and triangular movements. For the delta-lognormal reconstruction of rapid single strokes, the SNR decrease with age as a result of motor control degradation is generally supported. For the sigma-lognormal reconstruction of the triangles, taking into account the number of lognormal components used to model the movement makes apparent the divergence effect. In other words, the number of lognormal components used in sigma-lognormal modeling is robustly linked to the age factor in this specific experiment. This might suggest that the deterioration of motor control with aging is associated with the development of compensatory strategies such as emitting more motor commands to generate an adequate movement for a given task. One must also take into account that a small part of this effect might be associated with the fact that the Robust X0 extractor used for obtaining the sigma-lognormal parameters tries to increase the SNR up to 25 dB by adding lognormal components as long as they help to increase the SNR. However, this latter overestimation effect seems to be of mild important since the SNR/nbLog criterion confirms the aging tendency in one of our groups (i.e., WRF).

We also note that, in most cases, the same general trends have been observed in the two samples studied, which gives us high confidence that the relationships reported as significant on both population are not occurring by chance. Put together, the results of section Reaching Movements and Triangular Drawings indicate that lognormality (i.e., high SNR for lognormal modeling) and command efficiency (i.e., small number of lognormal components) are high for young adults and decrease with aging.

Conclusion

In this paper, we have investigated the concept of the ideal lognormal handwriter, as seen through the paradigm of the Kinematic Theory of rapid human movements. Starting from the fact that this theory predicts a convergence toward a lognormal impulse response for neuromuscular systems that are made up of well-synchronized subsystems, we have extended this interpretation to present the capacity to reconstruct the velocity profile of a movement with lognormal strokes as an indicator of the fine motor control capacity of the person who produced that movement. We first made a brief survey of the Kinematic Theory to clearly define the concept of lognormality and then we reported on some studies demonstrating that lognormality is indeed exploited by mature subjects to succeed in some required tasks. With this ideal descriptor in mind, we then investigated a corollary of this definition: the migration toward lognormality as young children grow up and the deviation from lognormality with aging.

For the first case, we have studied the handwriting of young children. We have shown that three indicators—SNR, nbLog, and SNR/nbLog as extracted from the sigma-lognormal model—can effectively point out the converging behavior toward lognormality of young writers producing simple movements. Indeed, the quality of the reconstruction increases with age and school-based learning of handwriting for oblique traces. For the oblique traces, the older the writer, the better is the control of the movements. In other words, the nbLog is smaller and the SNR/nbLog is higher. To more accurately characterize the PS group in this hierarchy, we will have to increase our dataset and eventually explore other indicators to be combined with those that are already at our disposal.

For the second case, we studied the handwriting of two populations of adults (with and without brain stroke risk factors), each participant being required to produce rapid straight strokes in reaction to a given stimulus and triangular movements. For rapid handwriting strokes, we have shown that the move away from lognormality with age was clearly observed as a decrease of the SNR with age. For triangular movements, the trajectories could be reconstructed with good SNR and the nbLog necessary for reconstruction was clearly associated with the effect of aging, with older participants needing more motor commands to perform the same type of movement.

Apart from using the concept of lognormality to characterize the level of learning in the first years of kindergarten and the effect of aging on human motor control, the same approach can be used to study departure from the ideal lognormal behavior when health problems affect handwriting production. For instance, a complete analysis of the whole population used in this experiment (O'Reilly and Plamondon, 2011, 2012a,b,c; O'Reilly, 2012; Plamondon et al., 2014b) clearly shows that there is a relationship between the presence of brain stroke risk factors and the characteristics of human movements as analyzed with the Kinematic Theory. Although a large part of this may be attributed to the effect of age and gender, there is convincing evidence that these two factors do not account for it all. Furthermore, in a recent study (Van Gemmert et al., 2013), it is observed that the nbLog variable was significantly larger for individuals with Parkinson's disease than for an age-matched control group. A similar analysis on Alzheimer's disease has just started (Impedovo et al., 2013). In this perspective, we can also assume that the same methodology could be used for monitoring the rehabilitation process after some injuries (Rohrer et al., 2002; O'Reilly and Plamondon, 2009b).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was partly supported by Grant RGPIN-915 from NSERC to Réjean Plamondon and a sponsorship from LAMIA to Céline Rémi. The authors thank Alain Dorville for his help in the selection of target patterns in the kindergarten study.

Footnotes

1. ^An exhaustive database of papers in these fields is available at http://www.graphonomics.org/, where the proceedings of all the biennal conferences of the International Graphonomics Society over the last 30 years are deposited.

2. ^Although many of these studies were using large strokes and trajectories for practical experimental purposes and to facilitate parameter extraction, the theory has been used for the analysis and the generation of movements of any size, from a few millimeters to decimeter letters, words, or signatures. Since the size of a trajectory is controlled by the parameter D, which determines the stroke amplitude, it has no effect on the lognormal convergence. It must also be noted that the theory is not limited to the control of finger movements. It has been used to analyze wrist, arm and head movements and eye saccades (Plamondon, 1995a) as well as 3D full upper limb planar trajectories (Leduc and Plamondon, 2001).

3. ^This threshold is less restrictive than the 25 dB threshold used in the present study because modeling bidirectional primitives with single delta-lognormals generally results in lower SNR.

4. ^Using the equation notation used in the S statistical language, the regression performed follows the formula SNR ~ age + size*direction.

References

Albaret, J.-M., and Santamaria, M. (1996). Utilisation des digitaliseurs dans l'étude des caractéristiques motrices de l'écriture. Evolutions Psychomotrices 8, 115–119.

Alimi, A., and Plamondon, R. (1994). “Analysis of the parameter dependence of handwriting generation models on movements characteristic»,” in Advances in Handwriting and Drawing: A Multidisciplinary Approach, eds C. Faure, G. Lorette, A. Vinter, and P. Keuss (Paris: Europia), 363–378.

Alimi, M. A. (2003). “Beta neuro-fuzzy systems,” in TASK Quaterly J. Special, Vol. 7, eds W. Duch and D. Rutkowska (Gdansk: Scientific Bulletin of the Academic Computer Centre), 23–41.

Almaksour, A., Anquetil, E., Plamondon, R., and O'Reilly, C. (2011). “Synthetic handwritten gesture generation using sigma-logormal model for evolving handwriting classifier,” in Proceedings of the 15th Biennal Conference of the International Graphonomics Society (Cancún: IGS), 98–101.

Awaida, S. M., and Mahmoud, S. A. (2012). State of the art in off-line writer identification of handwritten text and survey of writer identification of Arabic text. Educ. Res. Rev. 7, 445–463. doi: 10.5897/ERR11.303

CrossRef Full Text

Baron, R., and Plamondon, R. (1989). Acceleration measurement with an instrumented pen for signature verification and handwriting analysis. IEEE Trans. Instrum. Meas. 38, 1132–1138. doi: 10.1109/19.46414

CrossRef Full Text

Barry, B. K., Riek, S., and Carson, R. G. (2005). Muscle coordination during rapid force production by young and older adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 60, 232–240. doi: 10.1093/gerona/60.2.232

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bizzi, E., Hogan, N., Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A., and Giszter, S. (1992). Does the nervous system use equilibrium-point control to guide single and multiple joint movements? Behav. Brain Sci. 15, 603–613. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00072538

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bizzi, E. P., Dev, P. P., Morasso, P., and Polit, A. (1978). Effect of load disturbances during centrally initiated movements. J. Neurophysiol. 41, 542–556.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Bosga-Stork, I. M., Bosga, J., and Meulenbroek, R. G. J. (2011). Intentional control and biomechanical exploitation in preparatory handwriting. Hum. Mov. Sci. 30, 687–697. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2010.06.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bullock, D., and Grossberg, S. (1988). “The VITE model: a neural command circuit for generating arm and articulator trajectories,” in Dynamic Patterns in Complex Systems, eds J. A. S. Kelso, A. J. Mandell, and M. F. Shlesinger (Singapore: World Scientific Publishers), 305–326.

Carrières, P., and Plamondon, R. (1994). “An interactive handwriting teaching aid,” in Advances in Handwriting and Drawing: A Multidisciplinary Approach, eds C. Faure, G. Lorette, A. Vinter, and P. Keuss (Paris: Europia), 207–239.

Chartrel, E., and Vinter, A. (2004). L'écriture: Une activité longue et complexe à acquérir. ANAE 78, 174–180. Available online at: http://leadserv.u-bourgogne.fr/fr/publications/000076-l-ecriture-une-activite-longue-et-complexe-a-acquerir

Contreras-Vidal, J. L., Teulings, H. L., and Stelmach, G. E. (1998). Elderly subjects are impaired in spatial coordination in fine motor control. Acta Psychol. (Amst)., 100, 25–35. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(98)00023-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Denier van der Gon, J. J., and Thuring, J. P. H. (1965). The guiding of human movements, Kybernetik 14, 145–148.

Djeziri, S., Guerfali, W., Plamondon, R., and Robert, J. M. (2002). Learning handwriting with pen-based systems: computational issues. Pattern Recogn. 35, 1049–1057. doi: 10.1016/S0031-3203(01)00093-0

CrossRef Full Text

Djioua, M., and Plamondon, R. (2009a). Studying the variability of handwriting patterns using the kinematic theory. Hum. Mov. Sci. 28, 588–601. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2009.01.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Djioua, M., and Plamondon, R. (2009b). A new alogorithm and system for the characterization of handwriting strokes with delta-lognormal parameter. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 31, 2060–2072. doi: 10.1109/TPAMI.2008.264

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Djioua, M., and Plamondon, R. (2010). The limit profile of a rapid movement velocity. Hum. Mov. Sci. 29, 48–61. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2009.02.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Djioua, M., Plamondon, R., Della Cioppa, A., and Marcelli, A. (2007). Deterministic and evolutionary extraction of delta-iognormal parameters: perfomance comparaison. Int. J. Patt. Recogn. Artif. Intell. 21, 21–41. doi: 10.1142/S0218001407005284

CrossRef Full Text

Doermann, D., and Tombre, K. (eds.). (2013). Handbook of Document Image Processing and Recognition. Berlin: Springer.

Dooijes, E. H. (1983). Analysis of handwriting movements. Acta Psychol. 54, 99–114. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(83)90026-4

CrossRef Full Text

Duval, T. (2012). Étude comparative de méthode pour la description hybride de tracés à main levée acquis en ligne: cas des modèles lognormaux de Plamondon, Mémoire de maîtrise. LAMIA, Université des Antilles et de la Guyanne.

Duval, T., Rémi, C., Plamondon, R., and O'Reilly, C. (2013). “On the use of the sigma-lognormal model to study children handwriting,” in Proceedings of the 16th Biennial Conference of the Graphonomics Society, (Nara), 26–29.

Edelman, S. T., and Flash, T. (1987). A model of handwriting. Biol. Cybern. 57, 25–36. doi: 10.1007/BF00318713

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Enderle, J. D., and Wolfe, J. W. (1987). Time-optimal control of saccadic eye movements. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 34, 43–55. doi: 10.1109/TBME.1987.326014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Engelbrecht, S. E. (2001). Minimum principles in motor control. J. Math. Psychol. 45, 497–542. doi: 10.1006/jmps.2000.1295

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Feldman, A. G. (1966). Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of movement or maintenance of a steady posture. II. Controllable parameters of the muscle. Biophysics 11, 565–578.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Feldman, A. G., and Latash, M. L. (2005). Testing hypotheses and the advancement of science: recent attempts to falsify the equilibrium point hypothesis. Exp. Brain Res. 161, 91–103. doi: 10.1007/s00221-004-2049-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Feng, C., Woch, A., and Plamondon, R. (2002). “A comparative study of two velocity profile for rapid stroke analysis,” in Proceedings of the 16th International Conference on Pattern Recognition, (Québec: ICPR), Vol. 4, 52–55.

Flash, T., and Hogan, N. (1985). The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model. J. Neurosci. 5, 1688–1703.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Frélicot, C., Rémi, C., and Courtellemont, P. (2002). “School level recognition from children's drawings and writings,” in Proceedings of the 16th International Conference on Pattern Recognition, (Québec: ICPR), Vol. 3, 493–496

Galbally, J., Plamondon, R., Fierrez, J., and Ortega-Garcia, J. (2012a). Synthetic on-line signature generation, Part I: methodology and algorithms. Pattern Recognit. 45, 2610–2621. doi: 10.1016/j.patcog.2011.12.011

CrossRef Full Text

Galbally, J., Fierrez, J., Ortega-Garcia, J., and Plamondon, R. (2012b). Synthetic on-line signature generation Part II: experimental validation. Pattern Recognit. 45, 2622–2632. doi: 10.1016/j.patcog.2011.12.007

CrossRef Full Text

Gangadhar, G. D., Joseph, D. V. S., and Chakravarthy, V. S. (2007). An oscillatory neuromotor model of handwriting generation. Int. J. Doc. Anal. Recognit. 10, 69–84. doi: 10.1007/s10032-007-0046-0

CrossRef Full Text

Guerfali, W., and Plamondon, R. (1998). A new method for the analysis of simple and complex planar rapid movements. J. Neurosci.Methods 82, 35–45. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(98)00027-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harris, C. M., and Wolpert, D. M. (1998). Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394, 780–784. doi: 10.1038/29528

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hermes, H., and LaSalle, J. P. (1969). Functional Analysis and Time Optimal Control. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Heuninckx, S., Wenderoth, N., and Swinnen, S. P. (2008). Systems neuroplasticity in the aging brain: recruiting additional neural resources for successful motor performance in elderly persons. J. Neurosci. 28, 91–99. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3300-07.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hogan, N. (1984). An organization principle for a class of voluntary movements. J. Neurosci. 4, 2745–2754.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Hollerbach, J. M. (1981). An oscillation theory of handwriting. Biol. Cybern. 39, 139–156. doi: 10.1007/BF00336740

CrossRef Full Text

Impedovo, D., Pirlo, G., Mangini, F. M., Barbuzzi, D., Rollo, A., Balestrucci, A., et al. (2013). “Writing generation model for health care neuromuscular system investigation,” in Proceedings of the 10th International Meeting on Computational Intelligence Methods for Bioinformatics and Biostatistics (Nice).

Impedovo, D., Pirlo, G., and Plamondon, R. (2012). “Handwritten signature verification: new advancements and open issues, in Proceedings 13th Int conf. on Frontiers in Handwriting Recognition, (ICFHR2012), 367–372.

Kalveram, K. T. (1998). “A neural oscillator model learning given trajectories, or how an allo-imitation algorithm can be implemented into a motor controller,” in Motor behavior and human skill: a multidisciplinary approach, ed J. P. Piek (Champaign, IC: Human Kinetics), 127–140.

Kandel, S., Soler, O., Valdois, S., and Gros, C. (2006). Graphemes as motor units in the acquisition of writing skills. Read. Writ. 19, 313–337. doi: 10.1007/s11145-005-4321-5

CrossRef Full Text

Kao, H. S. R., Van Galen, G. P., and Hoosain, R. (eds.). (1986). Graphonomics: Contemporary Research in Handwriting. North Holland; Amsterdam; New York, NY; Oxford, Tokyo: Elsevier Sci.

Karldottir, R. (1996). Development of cursive handwriting. Percept. Motor Skills 82, 659–673. doi: 10.2466/pms.1996.82.2.659

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Katanoda, K., Yoshikawa, K., and Sugishita, M. (2001). A functional MRI study on the neural substrates for writing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 13, 34–42. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kelso, S. (1995). Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Ketcham, C. J., and Stelmach, G. E. (2004). “Movement control in the older adult,” in Technology for Adaptive Aging, eds R. W. Pew and S. B. Van Hemel (Washington, DC: National Academic Press), 64–92.

Koerich, K. L., Sabourin, R., and Suen, C. Y. (2003). Large vocabulary off-line handwriting recognition: a survey. Pattern Anal. Appl. 6, 97–121. doi: 10.1007/s10044-002-0169-3

CrossRef Full Text

Latash, M. L., and Anson, J. G. (1996). What are “normal movements” in atypical populations? Behav. Brain Sci. 19, 55. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00041467

CrossRef Full Text

Leclerc, F., and Plamondon, R. (1994). Automatic signature verification: the state of the art, 1989-1993. Int. J. Patt. Rec. Art. Int. 8, 643–660. doi: 10.1142/S0218001494000346

CrossRef Full Text

Leclerc, F., Plamondon, R., and Lorette, G. (1992). Des gaussiennes pour la modélisation des signatures et la segmentation des tracés manuscrits. Traitement du Signal, 9, 4, 347–358.

Leduc, N., and Plamondon, R. (2001). “A new approach to study human movements: the three dimensional delta-lognormal model,” in Proceedings 10th Biennial Conference of the International Graphonomics Society, (IGS 2001), August 6-8 2001, Nijmegen, 98–102.

Lorigo, L. M., and Govindaraju, V. (2006), Offline Arabic handwriting recognition: a survey. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 28, 712–724. doi: 10.1109/TPAMI.2006.102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Maarse, F. J. (1987). The Study of Handwriting Movement: Peripheral Models and Signal Processing Tehcniques. Lisse: Swets and Zertlinger

Meulenbroek, R. G. J., and Van Galen, G. P. (1988). “Acquisition of skilled handwriting: discontinuous trends in kinematic variables,” in Cognition and Action in Skilled Behavior, ed A. M. Cooley and J. R. Beech (Amsterdam), 273–281. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4115(08)60627-5

CrossRef Full Text

Morasso, P., Mussa Ivaldi, F. A., and Ruggiero, C. (1983). How a discontinuous mechanism can produce continuous patterns in trajectory formation and handwriting. Acta Psychol. 54, 83–98. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(83)90025-2

CrossRef Full Text

Neilson, P. D., and Neilson, M. D. (2005). An overview of adaptive model theory: solving the problems of redundancy, resources and nonlinear interactions in human movement control. J. Neural. Eng. 2, 279–312. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/2/3/S10

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Neilson, P. D. (1993). The problem of redundancy in movement control: the adaptive model theory approach. Psychol. Res. 55, 99–106. doi: 10.1007/BF00419640

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nelson, W. L. (1983). Physical principles for economies of skilled movements. Biol. Cybern. 46, 135–147. doi: 10.1007/BF00339982

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nouboud, F., and Plamondon, R. (1990). On line recognition of handprinted characters: survey and beta tests. Pattern Recognit. 23, 1031–1044. doi: 10.1016/0031-3203(90)90111-W

CrossRef Full Text

O'Reilly, C. (2012). Développement d'outils d'analyse de la motricité fine pour l'investigation de troubles neuromusculaires: théorie, prototype et mise en application dans le contexte des accidents vasculaires cérébraux. Ph.D. thesis, École polytechnique de Montréal.

O'Reilly, C., and Plamondon, R. (2009a). Development of a sigma-lognormal representation for on-line signatures, pattern recognition, special issue on frontiers in handwriting Recognition 42, 3324–3337.

O'Reilly, C., and Plamondon, R. (2009b). “A lognormal framework for human movement rehabilitation, in Rehabilitation Engineering, ed Tan Yen Kheng INTECH Publishing, 157–172. Available online at: http://sciyo.com/books/show/title/rehabilitation-engineering.

O'Reilly, C., and Plamondon, R. (2010). “Prototype-based methodology for the statistical analysis of local features in stereotypical handwriting tasks,” in Proc. Int. Conf. Pattern Recognition, (ICPR 2010), 1864–1867.

O'Reilly, C., and Plamondon, R. (2011). Impact of the principal stroke risk factors on human movements. Hum. Mov. Sci. 30, 792–806. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2010.07.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Reilly, C., and Plamondon, R. (2012a). “Looking for the brain stroke signature,” in Proceedings 21st International Conference on Pattern Recognition, (Tsukuba: ICPR), 1811–1814.

O'Reilly, C., and Plamondon, R. (2012b). “Design of a neuromuscular disorders diagnostic system using human movement analysis,” Proceedings 11th Int. Conf. on Information Science, Signal Processing and their Applications, (Montréal: ISSPA), 787–792.

O'Reilly, C., and Plamondon, R. (2012c). A globally optimal estimator for the Delta-Lognormal modeling of fast reaching movements. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern. B Cybern. 42, 1428–1442. doi: 10.1109/TSMCB.2012.2192109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O'Reilly, C., and Plamondon, R. (2013). Agonistic and antagonistic interaction in speed/accuracy tradeoff: A Delta-Lognormal perspective. Hum. Mov. Sci. 32, 1040–1055.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

O'Reilly, C., Plamondon, R., Landou, M. K., and Stemmers, B. (2013). Prediction of the time occurrence a motor erp based on the kinematic analysis of movement with the delta-lognormal model. Eur. J. Neurosci. 37, 173–180. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12039

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pirlo, G., and Impedovo, D. (2008). Automatic signature verification:the state of the art. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern. 38, 609–635. doi: 10.1109/TSMCC.2008.923866

CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R. (1994). The design of an on-line signature verification system: from theory to practice. Int. J. Patt. Recogn. Artif. Intell. 8, 795–811. doi: 10.1142/S0218001494000413

CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R. (1995a). A kinematic theory of rapid human movements: part I: Movement representation and generation. Biol. Cybern. 72, 295–307. doi: 10.1007/BF00202785

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R. (1995b). A kinematic theory of rapid human movements: part II: Movement time and control. Biol. Cybern. 72, 309–320. doi: 10.1007/BF00202786

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R. (1998). A kinematic theory of rapid human movements: part III: Kinetic outcomes. Biol. Cybern. 78, 133–145. doi: 10.1007/s004220050420

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R. (2013). “The quest for lognormality in a curved gaussian space-time: an on-line handwriting generation journey,” in Proceedings. 12th Int. Conf. on Document Analysis and Recognition (ICDAR 2013), xxxi. Availlable online at: http://www.icdar2013.org/docs/ICDAR2013-Plamondon.pdf

Plamondon, R., and Alimi, A. (1997). Speed/Accuracy tradeoffs in target-directed movements. Behav. Brain Sci. 20, 279–349. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X97001441

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R., Alimi, A., Yergeau, P., and Leclerc, F. (1993). Modeling velocity profiles of rapid movements: a comparative study. Biol. Cybern. 69, 119–128. doi: 10.1007/BF00226195

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R., and Djioua, M. (2006). A multi-level representation paradigm for handwriting stroke generation. Hum. Mov. Sci. 25, 586–607. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2006.07.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R., Djioua, M., and O'Reilly, C. (2009). La théorie cinématique des mouvements humains rapides: développements récents. Traitement du Signal 26, 377–394.

Plamondon, R., Feng, C., and Woch, A. (2003). A kinematic theory of rapid human movements: part IV: a formal mathematical proof and new insights. Biol. Cybern. 89, 126–138. doi: 10.1007/s00422-003-0407-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R., and Guerfali, W. (1998). The generation of handwriting with delta-lognormal synergies. Biol. Cybern. 78, 119–132. doi: 10.1007/s004220050419

CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R., and Lamarche, F. (1986). “Modelization of handwriting: a system approach,” in Graphonomics: Contemporary Research in Handwriting, eds H. S. R. Kao, G. P. van Galen, and R. Hoosain (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 169–183. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4115(09)60079-0

CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R., Li, X., and Djioua, M. (2007). Extraction of delta lognormal parameters from handwriting Strokes. Front. Comput. Sci. China 1, 106–113. doi: 10.1007/s11704-007-0009-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R., Lopresti, D., Schomaker, L. R. B., and Srihari, R. (1999). “On-line handwriting recognition,” in Encyclopedia of Electrical and Electronics Engineering, Vol. 15, ed J. G. Webster (New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons, 123–146. doi: 10.1002/047134608X.W5508

CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R., and Lorette, G. (1989). Automatic signature verification and writer identification: the state of the art. Pattern Recognit. 22, 107–131. doi: 10.1016/0031-3203(89)90059-9

CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R., Pirlo, G., and Impedovo, D. (2013a). “On-line signature verification,” in Handbook of Document Image Processing and Recognition, eds D. Doermann and K. Tombre (Berlin: Springer), 1–31.

Plamondon, R., Djioua, M., and Mathieu, P.-A. (2013b). The kinematic theory of rapid human movements: experimental observation of the proportional effect hypothesis. Hum. Mov. Sci. 32, 1026–1039.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Plamondon, R., O'Reilly, C., Galbally, J., Almaksour, A., and Anquetil, É. (2014a). Recent developments in the study of rapid human movements with the kinematic theory: applications to handwriting and signature synthesis. Pattern Recognit. Lett. 35, 225–235. doi: 10.1016/j.patrec.2012.06.004

CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R., O'Reilly, C., and Ouellet-Plamondon, R. (2014b). Strokes against Strokes, Strokes for Strides. Pattern Recognit. 47, 929–944.

Plamondon, R., and Privitera, C. M. (1995). A neural model for generating and learning a rapid movement sequence. Biol. Cybern. 74, 117–130. doi: 10.1007/BF00204200

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R., and Srihari, S. (2000). On-line and off-line handwriting recognition: a comprehensive survey. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 22, 63–84. doi: 10.1109/34.824821

CrossRef Full Text

Plamondon, R., Suen, C. Y., and Simner, M. (1989). Computer Recognition and Human Production of Handwriting, New Jersey; London; Hong Kong: World Scientific Publishing, 395.

Richards, T. L., Berninger, V. W., Stock, P., Altemeier, L., Trivedi, P., and Maravilla, K. R. (2011). Differences between good and poor child writers on fMRI contrasts for writing newly taught and highly practiced letter forms. Read. Writ. 24, 493–516. doi: 10.1007/s11145-009-9217-3

CrossRef Full Text

Robinovitch, S. N., Normandin, S. C., Stotz, P., and Maurer, J. D. (2005). Time requirement for young and elderly women to move into a position for breaking a fall with outstretched hands. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 60, 1553–1557. doi: 10.1093/gerona/60.12.1553

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rohrer, B., Fasoli, S., Krebs, H. I., Hughes, R., Volpe, B., Frontera, W. R., et al. (2002). Movement smoothness changes during stroke recovery. J. Neurosci. 22, 8297–8304.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rousseeuw, P. J., and Leroy, A. M. (1987). Robust Regression and Outlier Detection. New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons, 329. doi: 10.1002/0471725382

CrossRef Full Text

Schmidt, R. A., and Lee, T. D. (1999). Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis, 3rd Edn. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 493.

Schomaker, L. (2007). “Advances in writer identification and verification,” in Ninth International Conference on Document Analysis and Recognition, (Curitiba: ICDAR). doi: 10.1109/ICDAR.2007.4377119

CrossRef Full Text

Schomaker, L. R. B. (1991). Simulation and Recognition of Handwriting Movement: a Vertical Approach to Modeling Human Motor Behavior, Ph.D thesis, Nijmegen University, Nijmegen.

Schomaker, L. R. B., Thomassen, A. J. W. M., and Teulings, H. L. (1989). “A computational model of cursive handwriting,” in Computer Recognition and Human Production of Handwriting, eds R. Plamondon, C.Y. Suen, and M. Simner (Singapore: World Scientific), 153–177.

Seitz, R. J. (2009). Brain representations of writing. Dusseldorf: German as Foreign Language Journal.

Senatore, R., and Marcelli, A. (2012). “A neural scheme for procedural motor learning of handwriting,” in Proceedings ICFHR 2012, Bari, 655–660. doi: 10.1109/ICFHR.2012.160

CrossRef Full Text

Shah, C., Erhard, K., Ortheil, H.-J., Kaza, E., Kessler, C., and Lotze, M. (2013). Neural correlates of creative writing: an fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1088–1101. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21493

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sreeraj, M., and Idicula, S. M. (2011). A survey on writer identification schemes. Int. J. Comp. Appl. 26, 23–33. doi: 10.5120/3075-4205

CrossRef Full Text

Srihari, S. N., Yang, X., and Ball, G. R. (2007). Offline Chinese handwriting recognition: an assessment of current technology. Front. Comput. Sci. China 1, 135–157. doi: 10.1007/s11704-007-0015-2

CrossRef Full Text

Tagougui, N., Kherallah, M., and Alimi, A. (2011). On-line arabic handwriting recognition: a survey. Int. J. Doc. Anal. Recognit. 14, 15–23. doi: 10.1007/s10032-010-0124-6

CrossRef Full Text

Tanaka, H., Krakauer, J. W., and Qian, N. (2006). An optimization principle for determining movement duration. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 3875–3886. doi: 10.1152/jn.00751.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Teulings, H. L. (1996). “Handwriting movement control,” in Handbook of Perception and Action, Vol. 2 Motor Skills, eds S. W. Keele and H. Heuer (London: Academic Press), 561–613.

Thomassen, A. J. W. M., Keuss, P. J. G., and van Galen, G. P. (1983). Motor aspects of handwriting. Acta Psychol. 54:354.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Thomassen, A. J. W. M., and Teulings, H. L. (1983). “The development of handwriting,” in The Psychology of Written Language: Developmental and Educational Perspectives, ed M. Martlew (New York, NY: Wiley), 179–213.

Uno, Y., Kawato, M., and Suzuki, R. (1989). Formation, Control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement. Biol. Cybern. 61, 89–101. doi: 10.1007/BF00204593

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Galen, G. P. (1991). Handwriting: issues for a psychomotor theory. Hum. Mov. Sci. 10, 165–191. doi: 10.1016/0167-9457(91)90003-G

CrossRef Full Text

Van Galen, G. P., and Teulings, H. L. (1983). The independent monitoring of form and scale parameters in handwriting. Acta Psychol. 54, 9–22. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(83)90020-3

CrossRef Full Text

Van Gemmert, A, Plamondon, R., and O'Reilly, C. (2013). “Using the Sigma-lognormal model to investigate handwriting of individuals with Parkinson's disease,” in Proceedings16th Biennial Conf. of the Graphonomics Society, Nara, 119–122.

Vinter, A., and Mounoud, P. (1991). “Isochrony and accuracy of drawing movements in children: effects of age and context,” in Development of Graphic Skills, eds J. Wann, A. M. Wing, and N. Søvik (Londres: Academic Press), 113–134.

Wada, Y., and Kawato, M. (1995). A theory for cursive handwriting based on the minimization principle. Biol. Cybern. 73, 3–13. doi: 10.1007/BF00199051

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Woch, A., and Plamondon, R. (2010). Characterization of bi-directional movement primitives and their agonist-antagonist synergy with the delta-lognormal model. Human Kinetics Journal, Champaign Illinois, Motor Control 14, 1–25.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Woch, A., Plamondon, R., and O'Reilly, C. (2011). Kinematic characteristics of successful movement primitives in young and older subjects: a Delta-Lognormal comparison, Hum. Mov. Sci. 30, 1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2009.10.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zanone, P.-G., Athènes, S., Sallagoity, I., and Albaret, J.-M. (2005). “Switching among graphic patterns is governed by coordination dynamics of handwriting,” in Proceedings 12th Biennial Conf. of the Int. Graphonomics Society, Salerno, 255–260.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Zesiger, P. (1995). Écrire: Approches Cognitive, Neuropsychologique et Développementale Paris, Ed. PUF.

Zesiger, P., Deonna, T., and Mayor, C. (2000). L'acquisition de l'écriture. Enfance 53, 295–304. doi: 10.3406/enfan.2000.3186

CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: kinematic theory, lognormal models, handwriting analysis and generation, neuromuscular systems, learning, aging, lognormality

Citation: Plamondon R, O'Reilly C, Rémi C and Duval T (2013) The lognormal handwriter: learning, performing, and declining. Front. Psychol. 4:945. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00945

Received: 03 July 2013; Accepted: 29 November 2013;
Published online: 19 December 2013.

Edited by:

Marieke Longcamp, Aix-Marseille Univserity and CNRS, France

Reviewed by:

Andrew D. Wilson, Indiana University, Bloomington, USA
Jeremy Danna, CNRS - Aix-Marseille Université, France
Jindrich Kodl, Weizmann Institute of Science, Israel

Copyright © 2013 Plamondon, O'Reilly, Rémi and Duval. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Réjean Plamondon, Laboratoire Scribens, Département de Génie Électrique, École Polytechnique de Montréal, C.P. 6079 Succ. Centre-Ville, Montréal, QC H3C3A7, Canada e-mail: rejean.plamondon@polymtl.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.