Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 31 October 2014
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic Interdisciplinary Approaches to Multilingualism View all 13 articles

The facilitation effect and language thresholds

  • Teaching and Learning, Policy and Leadership, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA

Lechner and Siemund (2014) set out to determine whether bilinguals have an advantage for learning additional languages over monolinguals, purporting to evaluate the Threshold Hypothesis of Cummins (1979a) in this context. The study investigated the attainment of English literacy by Turkish-German, Vietnamese-German, and Russian-German simultaneous and sequential bilinguals for whom English is a third language, and found significant correlations (at 0.01 level) for the second language (German) with the third language (English) at 0.580, and for the heritage language with the second language at 0.638; while the heritage language correlated with the third language at 0.277, the result was non-significant (N = 34). This crosslinguistic transfer effect is well-documented in the scholarly literature for first language (L1) and second language (L2) learners (Genesee et al., 2006; Goldenberg, 2011), but very little prior work has been done to examine crosslinguistic transfer of literacy among trilinguals. The Threshold Hypothesis specifically points to ability levels in the first language as the mechanism which facilitates attainment in the second language (extended to a third language for Lechner and Siemund). The primary conceptual problem with “ability” in the first language is that it lacks any grounded theoretical description of “levels,” and simply equates social status with linguistic ability much as classical prescriptivist ideology does (MacSwan and Rolstad, 2003, 2010; Wiley and Rolstad, 2014).

Cummins (1976) developed the Threshold Hypothesis to account for an apparent conflict in findings regarding the cognitive benefits of bilingualism. Earlier studies concluded that cognitive progress and school achievement were negatively affected by bilingualism, while more recent research appeared to show “positive cognitive consequences.” Cummins noted that the studies that found a negative effect involved linguistic minorities, and those finding a positive effect involved a condition of “additive bilingualism” in which linguistic majority children are learning an additional language. Cummins theorized that the linguistic minorities were undergoing loss of their first language, and that “the level of linguistic competence attained by a bilingual child may mediate the effects of his bilingual learning experiences on cognitive growth.” That is, reports of negative effects of bilingualism for “cognitive and scholastic progress” related to minority children's (hypothesized) lower level of linguistic proficiency in the first language, as affected by acquiring a second, while children in the “additive” bilingual programs had the benefit of continued support of their first language. As Cummins (1976) put it,

Subtractive bilingualism, where L1 [first language] is being replaced by L2 [second language], implies that as a bilingual in a language minority group develops skills in L2, his competence in L1 will decrease. It seems likely that, under these circumstances, many bilingual children in subtractive bilingual learning situations may not develop native-like competence in either of their two languages (p. 20).

In later work, Cummins (1979a) extended his analysis to another similar problem. Swain (1978) had made the case that immersion programs, in which linguistic majorities are (partially or totally) immersed in an L2, differ in important respects from submersion programs, in which language minority children are immersed in a majority language (Cohen and Swain, 1976; Swain, 1978). Today, considerable research on program effectiveness has borne out this expectation, as it shows that children in bilingual programs generally outperform similar children in English immersion programs in the US, and that children with more access to home language support do even better than children with less access (see Rolstad et al., 2005, and works cited there).

To address these observed differences, Cummins (1979b, p. 223) proposed “a theoretical framework which assigns a central role to the interaction between socio-cultural, linguistic and school program factors,” in which “the level of competence bilingual children achieve in their two languages acts as an intervening variable in mediating the effects of their bilingual learning experiences” (Cummins, 1976, p. 229). Background characteristics, child input factors, and educational treatment variables acted together to influence “child process variables,” in Cummins' theory, resulting in minority children's differential ability in L1 and L2. A potentially resulting condition of semilingualism is thus posited to explain academic achievement differences among children. Embedded in the Threshold Hypothesis,

negative cognitive and academic effects are hypothesized to result from low levels of competence in both languages or what Scandinavian researchers (e.g., Hansegård, 1968; Skutnabb-Kangas and Toukomaa, 1976) have termed “semilingualism” or “double semilingualism” … Essentially, the lower threshold level of bilingual competence proposes that bilingual children's competence in a language may be sufficiently weak as to impair the quality of their interaction with the educational environment through that language (1979a, p. 230).

In Cummins' theory, semilingualism is a potential characteristic of minority language children, but not of majority language children, and is the cause of their weaker academic performance. For children in an additive situation, semilingualism does not degrade the quality of interactions in the classroom, generally leading to school success.

Lechner and Siemund take care to note, based on critical discussion in MacSwan (2000a) and MacSwan and Rolstad (2006, 2010), that literacy and language are different constructs, and seek to remove the blemish of semilingualism from the Threshold Hypothesis. In doing so, the authors observe, “We do not regard the Threshold Hypothesis as a competence-related construct, but rather as a performance-based concept relating to educational attainment.” Elaborating,

We believe that it is necessary to distinguish between linguistic competence and performance (langue versus parole) in the interpretation of the Threshold Hypothesis. Cummins seems to use the term “language proficiency” to refer to both competence and performance, including school literacy. MacSwan (2000a, pp. 33–34) argues that if the Threshold Hypothesis refers to language competence, it is spurious because there is no evidence to suggest that subtractive bilinguals did not know the underlying principles of their language.

However, the competence/performance distinction does not help in a general way to overcome the conceptual weaknesses of the Threshold Hypothesis. Competence refers to linguistic knowledge, and performance to the use of that knowledge in concrete, everyday situations (Chomsky, 1965). We utter things all the time which we immediately recognize to be ill-formed, reflecting on our linguistic competence, due to fatigue, distraction, memory loss, or other language-external factors. Because our underlying system of competence relies on recursive generative rules, it can theoretically produce sentences that are infinitely long; but as finite beings, we can't stick around long enough to say them. Claiming that “ability” levels differ according to linguistic performance rather than linguistic competence seems to achieve little or nothing, and still demands supporting evidence, just as it did when these differences were conceptualized in terms of linguistic competence. And as before, in Cummins' original proposals, relevant evidence is lacking, and other work indicates that the hypothesized “levels” are not to be found (MacSwan et al., 2002; MacSwan and Rolstad, 2006).

Granted, “linguistic performance” is used as a large, undifferentiated container of many very different kinds of psychological phenomena which competence-focused linguists generally wish to set aside, and context-sensitive language use, such as pragmatics and discourse, might reasonably be regarded as governed by the theory of linguistic performance, in part. Indeed, Cummins provides a four-component definition of “language proficiency,” following Canale and Swain (1980) and Canale (1983): Grammatical competence, sociolinguistic competence, discourse competence, and strategic competence. Cummins' (1981) definition of “sociolinguistic competence” is similar to what Chomsky (1978, p. 224) called “pragmatic competence,” defined as “knowledge of conditions and manners of appropriate use, in conformity with various purposes.” For Cummins (1981), discourse competence consists of “knowledge of how to combine meanings and forms to achieve a unified text in different modes” (p. 7), and strategic competence is the “mastery of verbal and non-verbal strategies” which assist under conditions of breakdowns in other competence domains. In this broader context, Cummins (1981) settled on a framework in which “literacy is viewed as one aspect of communicative proficiency” (p. 14).

If we think of school-based literacy and the particular language used at school as a different language register, or a Discourse in Gee's (1996) sense, then we are well-positioned to characterize school language as domain- and place-focused—the language of school differs from the language of skateboarding just as the language of boatbuilding differs from the language of farming. But notice that this is not the concept of language proficiency embedded in the Threshold Hypothesis, where groups are said to differ by ability levels, not contexts, and so the appeal to linguistic performance, or even context of language use, does not help to avoid the prescriptivist character inherent in the model.

Rather than try to salvage the Threshold Hypothesis, we suggest that an alternative theoretical framework be pursued, and that the original Threshold Hypothesis be discarded. In our own work, we have advocated the facilitation theory (MacSwan and Rolstad, 2005)—the view that cognitive architecture permits and facilitates transfer of literacy cross-linguistically precisely because it is essentially language-external. This view is also consistent with the approach in Riches and Genesee (2006), who posit “a common underlying reservoir of literacy abilities” available to L2 learners who are good L1 readers.

As argued in MacSwan and Rolstad (2005), the neuropsychological evidence suggests that language is separate and discrete from other mental faculties, taking psychological modularity as our framework (as is typical in the cognitive neurosciences). In the case of bilingualism, both languages are represented in the human language faculty (Epstein et al., 1996; MacSwan, 2000b, 2014). But unlike language and other perceptual systems, western-style literacy is a recent invention, and is absent from many human cultures (MacSwan, 2000a; Gee, 2001; MacSwan and Rolstad, 2010).

In fact, literacy seems to rely on a wide range of cognitive faculties; besides knowledge of language, these include background knowledge, systems of reasoning, motor control, visual processing, shape recognition, and context. Reading and writing are independent of special-purpose mental faculties like language, and should be conceptualized as a technological invention. Thus, literacy recruits knowledge as needed from relevant cognitive systems. Evidence from cases of selective impairment in which a blow to the posterior regions of the brain renders a person suddenly unable to read but with all normal language faculties intact adds additional support to this view (Gardner, 1974). School literacy may therefore be seen as one of several ways language is used to satisfy human purposes; it uses linguistic and other cognitive resources to represent language, but it is not itself an aspect of language ability in the linguistic sense.

We suggest, then, that “transfer” of literacy across languages occurs because in the bilingual brain, both languages have access to the same cognitive resources. As Genesee et al. (2006) observe in a comprehensive review, transfer of first language literacy to the second language context is found in studies of word reading (across age, linguistic typology, and L2 proficiency), reading comprehension (across age, typology, language status, direction of transfer, and tasks), and reading strategies. They found phonological processes underlying word recognition to be influenced by orthography, but a facilitation effect was still present. (We note that two of the three heritage language groups assessed in Lechner and Siemund's study—Vietnamese and Russian—use non-Latinate orthographies, contrasting with English and German, and the sample size of the Turkish-background group was relatively very small, at N = 5; these factors may underlie the positive but non-significant result for the correlation of heritage language literacy with English literacy in Lechner and Siemund's results.)

It is apparent, then, that one does not see a relationship between “first language ability,” however that is to be conceived, and second language literacy, but between literacy in the first language and literacy in the second language; in other words, literacy is relatively independent of the particular language for which it was initially acquired. In this regard, the usual meaning of transfer, which implies that a process “moves” knowledge from one language to another, should be seen as strictly a metaphor: Rather than move, both languages have access to the same store of knowledge, available to learners regardless of how the knowledge was acquired in the first place, as Riches and Genesee (2006) also suggested. Access to knowledge acquired in a first language has a facilitation effect on learning in the second language context.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Canale, M. (1983). “From communicative competence to communicative language pedagogy,” in Language and Communication, eds J. Richard and R. Schmidt (New York, NY: Longman), 2–27.

Canale, M., and Swain, M. (1980). Theoretical bases of communicative approaches to second language teaching and testing. Appl. Linguist. 1, 1–47. doi: 10.1093/applin/1.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Chomsky, N. (1978). “Language and unconscious knowledge,” in Psychoanalysis and Language, Psychiatry and the Humanities, Vol. 3, ed J. H. Smith (New Haven, CT: Yale University Press), 3–44.

Cohen, A., and Swain, M. (1976). “Bilingual education: the “immersion” model in North American context,” in English as a Second Language in Bilingual Education: Selected TESOL Papers, eds J. Alatis and K. Twaddell (Washington, DC: Teachers of English to Speakers of Other Languages), 55–64.

Google Scholar

Cummins, J. (1976). The influence of bilingualism on cognitive growth: a synthesis of research findings and explanatory hypotheses. Work. Pap. Biling. 9, 1–43.

Google Scholar

Cummins, J. (1979a). Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual children. Rev. Educ. Res. 49, 221–251. doi: 10.3102/00346543049002222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cummins, J. (1979b). Cognitive/academic language proficiency, linguistic interdependence, the optimum age question and some other matters. Work. Pap. Biling. 19, 121–129.

Google Scholar

Cummins, J. (1981). “The role of primary language development in promoting educational success for language minority students,” in Schooling and Language Minority Students: a Theoretical Framework, 1st Edn., ed California State Department of Education, Office of Bilingual Education (Sacramento, CA: California State Department of Education), 3–49.

Epstein, S., Flynn, S., and Martohardjono, G. (1996). Second language acquisition: theoretical and experimental issues in contemporary research. Behav. Brain Sci. 19, 677–758. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00043521

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, H. (1974). The Shattered Mind: the Person after Brain Damage. New York, NY: Vintage Books.

Gee, J. P. (1996). Social Linguistics and Literacies: Ideology in Discourses, 2nd Edn. London: Taylor & Francis.

Google Scholar

Gee, J. P. (2001). “Progressivism, critique, and socially situated minds,” in The Fate of Progressive Language Policies and Practices, eds C. Dudley-Marling and C. Edelsky (Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English), 31–58.

Genesee, F., Geva, E., Dressler, C., and Kamil, M. (2006). “Synthesis: cross-linguistic relationships,” in Developing Literacy in Second-Language Learners, eds D. August and D. Shanahan (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics), 153–174.

Google Scholar

Goldenberg, C. (2011). “Reading instruction for English language learners,” in Handbook of Reading Research, Vol. IV, eds M. Kamil, P. D. Pearson, E. Moje, and P. Afflerbach (Newark, NJ: International Reading Association), 684–710.

Google Scholar

Hansegård, N. E. (1968). Tvåspråkighet Eller Halvspråkighet? Stockholm: Aldus series 253.

Lechner, S., and Siemund, P. (2014). Double threshold in bi- and multilingual contexts: preconditions for higher academic attainment in English as an additional language. Front. Psychol. 5:546. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00546

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacSwan, J. (2000a). The Threshold Hypothesis, semilingualism, and other contributions to a deficit view of linguistic minorities. Hisp. J. Behav. Sci. 22, 3–45. doi: 10.1177/0739986300221001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacSwan, J. (2000b). The architecture of the bilingual language faculty: evidence from codeswitching. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 3, 37–54. doi: 10.1017/S1366728900000122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacSwan, J. (2014). “Programs and proposals in codeswitching research: unconstraining theories of bilingual language mixing,” in Grammatical Theory and Bilingual Codeswitching, ed J. MacSwan (Cambridge: MIT Press), 1–33.

MacSwan, J., and Rolstad, K. (2003). “Linguistic diversity, schooling, and social class: rethinking our conception of language proficiency in language minority education,” in Sociolinguistics: The Essential Readings, eds C. B. Paulston and R. Tucker (Oxford: Blackwell), 329–340.

MacSwan, J., and Rolstad, K. (2005). Modularity and the facilitation effect: psychological mechanisms of transfer in bilingual students. Hisp. J. Behav. Sci. 27, 224–243. doi: 10.1177/0739986305275173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacSwan, J., and Rolstad, K. (2006). How language tests mislead us about children's abilities: implications for special education placements. Teach. Coll. Rec. 108, 2304–2328. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9620.2006.00783.x

CrossRef Full Text

MacSwan, J., and Rolstad, K. (2010). “The role of language in theories of academic failure for linguistic minorities,” in International Perspectives on Bilingual Education: Policy, Practice, and Controversy, ed J. Petrovic (Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing), 173–195.

Google Scholar

MacSwan, J., Rolstad, K., and Glass, G. V. (2002). Do some school-age children have no language? Some problems of construct validity in the Pre-LAS Español. Biling. Res. J. 26, 213–238. doi: 10.1080/15235882.2002.10668718

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riches, C., and Genesee, F. (2006). “Literacy: crosslinguistic and crossmodal issues,” in Educating English Language Learners, eds F. Genesee, K. Lindholm-Leary, W. Saunders, and D. Christian (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 64–108. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511499913.004

CrossRef Full Text

Rolstad, K., Mahoney, K., and Glass, G. (2005). The big picture: a meta-analysis of program effectiveness research on English Language Learners. Educ. Policy 19, 572–594. doi: 10.1177/0895904805278067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skutnabb-Kangas, T., and Toukomaa, P. (1976). Teaching Migrant Children's Mother Tongue and Learning the Language of the Host Country in the Context of the Socio-Cultural Situation of the Migrant Family. Helsinki: The Finnish National Commission for UNESCO.

Swain, M. (1978). French immersion: early, late or partial? Can. Mod. Lang. Rev. 34, 557–585.

Google Scholar

Wiley, T., and Rolstad, K. (2014). The common core state standards and the great divide. Int. Multiling. Res. J. 8, 38–55. doi: 10.1080/19313152.2014.852428

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: bilingualism, biliteracy, Threshold Hypothesis, facilitation theory, language minority education

Citation: Rolstad K and MacSwan J (2014) The facilitation effect and language thresholds. Front. Psychol. 5:1197. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01197

Received: 05 September 2014; Accepted: 03 October 2014;
Published online: 31 October 2014.

Edited by:

Mary Grantham O′Brien, University of Calgary, Canada

Reviewed by:

Simone Lechner, University of Hamburg, Germany

Copyright © 2014 Rolstad and MacSwan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: macswan@umd.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.