Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 26 February 2015
Sec. Cognitive Science

The influence of negative stimulus features on conflict adaption: evidence from fluency of processing

  • 1Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
  • 2Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany

Cognitive control enables adaptive behavior in a dynamically changing environment. In this context, one prominent adaptation effect is the sequential conflict adjustment, i.e., the observation of reduced response interference on trials following conflict trials. Increasing evidence suggests that such response conflicts are registered as aversive signals. So far, however, the functional role of this aversive signal for conflict adaptation to occur has not been put to test directly. In two experiments, the affective valence of conflict stimuli was manipulated by fluency of processing (stimulus contrast). Experiment 1 used a flanker interference task, Experiment 2 a color-word Stroop task. In both experiments, conflict adaptation effects were only present in fluent, but absent in disfluent trials. Results thus speak against the simple idea that any aversive stimulus feature is suited to promote specific conflict adjustments. Two alternative but not mutually exclusive accounts, namely resource competition and adaptation-by-motivation, will be discussed.

Introduction

In an environment full of tempting opportunities and action affordances, appropriate action selection is a constant challenge. For example, grabbing the low fat yogurt instead of the rich but more delicious chocolate mousse from the fridge can be a hard decision. In situations like this, cognitive control supports the selection of the weaker but intended action in the face of a stronger but inadequate action (c.f. Miller and Cohen, 2001). Moreover, when confronted with response conflicts, cognitive control not only enables conflict resolution in the current trial but also adjusts processing parameters such that the cognitive system is better prepared when the response conflict repeats as indicated by reduced response interference in post-conflict trials (Gratton et al., 1992; Botvinick et al., 1999; Notebaert et al., 2001; Stürmer et al., 2002; Caessens et al., 2005; Egner, 2008; Wühr and Kunde, 2008). On a neuronal level, it has been suggested that it is the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) that detects conflicts and sends this information to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex which then increases control in the post-conflict trial (e.g., Botvinick et al., 2004; Kerns et al., 2004).

In the past decade, huge advances have been made to further our understanding of the underlying processes that enable such dynamic control adaptations (for recent approaches to conflict adaptation effects see for example Braem et al., 2014; Duthoo et al., 2014; Jiang et al., 2014). Questions of interest concerned the locus and specificity of the adaptation effect (e.g., Kiesel et al., 2006; Kunde and Wühr, 2006; Notebaert and Verguts, 2008; Wendt et al., 2012), the role of episodic retrieval and priming processes (e.g., Mayr et al., 2003; Hommel et al., 2004), the role of learning (e.g., Holroyd and Coles, 2002; Blais et al., 2007; Verguts and Notebaert, 2009; Blais and Verguts, 2012), timing (e.g., Goschke and Dreisbach, 2008; Scherbaum et al., 2011; Pastötter et al., 2013), conflict strength (e.g., Takezawa and Miyatani, 2005; Forster et al., 2011; Wendt et al., 2014), working memory load (Stürmer et al., 2005; Fischer et al., 2010; Soutschek et al., 2012), and context effects in general (e.g., Fischer et al., 2008; Funes et al., 2010).

Only recently, the question of how stress, affect and motivation might influence processing adjustments has moved into the focus of research (e.g., van Steenbergen et al., 2009, 2010, 2012; Kuhbandner and Zehetleitner, 2011; Padmala et al., 2011; Plessow et al., 2011; Stürmer et al., 2011; Braem et al., 2012; see Dreisbach and Fischer, 2012a, for a review). The role of affect in sequential conflict adaptation is of specific interest here due to the increasing evidence that conflicts themselves are experienced as aversive signals (Dreisbach and Fischer, 2012b; Schouppe et al., 2012, 2015; Fritz and Dreisbach, 2013, 2014). For example, presenting Stroop conflict stimuli (Stroop, 1935) as primes eased the evaluation of negative target stimuli and increased the frequency of negative judgments for neutral target stimuli (Dreisbach and Fischer, 2012b; Fritz and Dreisbach, 2013, 2014). Converging evidence in favor of the aversive conflict signal also comes from physiological studies showing increased heart rate (Renaud and Blondin, 1997), larger pupil dilatation (van Steenbergen and Band, 2013; Wendt et al., 2014), and enhanced skin conductance response (Kobayashi et al., 2007) in response to incongruent Stroop stimuli (but see Schacht et al., 2010, who, however, did not use Stroop stimuli but measured physiological activity during a go/no-go paradigm). Given that conflicts are detected by the ACC, and further given that the ACC is also activated by monetary loss (Rainville, 2002), social exclusion (Eisenberger et al., 2003), negative feedback (Nieuwenhuis et al., 2004), and pain (Singer et al., 2004), one might therefore speculate that it is not the response conflict per se but the aversive character of the response conflict that triggers the processing adjustments (Botvinick, 2007; Dreisbach and Fischer, in press a, in press b). In fact, Dreisbach and Fischer (2011) found that aversive stimuli can lead to sequential adaptation effects even in the absence of response conflicts. In that study, the authors made use of the fact that fluency of processing, i.e., the experienced ease of stimulus processing, is affectively marked, with low fluency being associated with negative and high fluency with positive affect (Reber et al., 1998; Winkielman et al., 2003)1. Dreisbach and Fischer (2011) let participants categorize number words according to magnitude that were either written in an easy (fluent) or hard to read (disfluent) font. In three experiments, the authors found sequential modulations of the fluency effect (performance difference between disfluent and fluent trials) in terms of a smaller fluency effect following disfluent trials. Moreover, van Steenbergen and colleagues repeatedly showed that presenting positive symbols in the inter-trial-intervals of an Eriksen Flanker task (Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974) eliminated conflict adaptation effects (van Steenbergen et al., 2009, 2012; but see Stürmer et al., 2011; Braem et al., 2013b; Notebaert and Braem, in press). Van Steenbergen and colleagues interpreted this result as indication that the positive symbol counteracted the aversive signal of the response conflict and thus eliminated conflict adaptation.

In sum, the observations that (1) aversive signals without response conflict promote sequential processing adjustments (Dreisbach and Fischer, 2011) and (2) positive signals following response conflict eliminate processing adjustments (van Steenbergen et al., 2009, 2012), might suggest that the aversive characteristic of conflicts itself triggers the processing adjustments2. One straightforward way to address the question whether it is the aversive conflict signal that triggers conflict adaptation is to increase the aversiveness of a given conflict stimulus and investigate its effects on conflict adaptation. As already mentioned above, perceptual fluency serves as an affective signal with high perceptual fluency being associated with positive affect and low perceptual fluency being associated with negative affect (Reber et al., 1998; Winkielman et al., 2003). Therefore, we manipulated the aversive quality of a conflict signal by presenting classical response interference tasks either with high perceptual fluency (fluent) or with low perceptual fluency (disfluent), expecting that disfluent incongruent trials are more aversive than fluent incongruent trials. Consequently, if it is an unspecific aversiveness conveyed by the conflict that triggers conflict adaptation, we should find increased conflict adaptation on disfluent trials as compared to fluent trials, as disfluency is assumed to increase the general aversiveness of conflicts even further.

Experiment 1

Method

Participants

Thirty students of the University of Regensburg were tested (23 female; 24 right-handers; mean age: 26.6, SD = 4.1). All participants signed informed consent before the experiment and received 3 Euros or partial course credit after its completion. Data of two participants with RTs that were more than two standard deviations (SDs) above at least one group cell mean were excluded from the analysis, leaving a final sample of 28 participants.

Material and procedure

Stimuli consisted of a central color-square horizontally flanked by two color-squares, one on each side. The three horizontally aligned squares subtended a visual angle of 19.9° × 6.6° at a viewing distance of 60 cm. Square colors were red, green, and blue. Congruency was manipulated by color match or mismatch: The color of the central square could either match (congruent stimulus) or mismatch (incongruent stimulus) the color of the two flanking squares which were always of the same color. Fluency was manipulated by figure-ground contrast differences. In fluent stimuli, color saturation was 100%, in disfluent stimuli, color saturation was 50%. Fluency of relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions was manipulated to the same extent by stimulus contrast such that no effect on the conflict per se is to be expected (Miles and Proctor, 2009). The stimuli were presented centrally on a white background. Participants were instructed to quickly and accurately identify the color of the central square by pressing one of three keys on a QWERTZ keyboard (“c” for green, “v” for red, “b” for blue, respectively) with their index, middle and ring finger of their dominant hand (see Larson et al., 2009, for a similar procedure).

Each trial started with a plus sign as fixation cross for 250 ms, followed by the imperative stimulus that was presented until a response was given. For correct responses, the next trial started after 1000 ms. For errors, the German word for error (Fehler) appeared and remained on screen for 1000 ms. After an additional 500 ms, the next trial started. The experiment started with a short test to exclude color blindness, followed by a color-to-key-mapping practice block of 12 randomly presented imperative stimuli and a second practice block of 24 imperative stimuli where participants were introduced to the fluency manipulation. After that, one practice block of 120 trials followed consisting of 30 congruent and 30 incongruent fluent and disfluent trials, respectively. This practice block was followed by three experimental blocks of 120 trials each. Blocks were separated from one another by self-paced breaks. Repetition of identical target stimuli was not allowed. Because we were interested in how fluency modulates conflict adaptation, we presented fluent and disfluent trials in runs of 10 in a given block while for the assessment of conflict adaptation, conflict vs. non-conflict trials varied randomly from trial to trial. The experiment lasted about 25 min.

Design

A 2 (CongruencyN) × 2 (CongruencyN-1) × 2 (Fluency) repeated measures design was used.

Data preprocessing

We excluded the first two trials of each fluency block of 10 trials length in order to remove possible transition effects from the previous fluency condition. In order to decrease the influence of low-level feature repetitions and to maximize cognitive control involvement in conflict adaptation (e.g., Egner, 2007), partial priming trials [whenever the color of either the central or flanking stimulus repeated from trial N-1 to trial N (42.3%)] and negative priming trials [whenever the color of the flanking squares in trial N-1 was the color of the central square in trial N (18.5%)] were excluded prior to analysis (see also Ullsperger et al., 2005; Bugg, 2008; Larson et al., 2009; Wendt et al., 2014)3. For error data analysis, mean error rates for the remaining data (on average 134 trials per participant) were computed for each cell of the 2 (CongruencyN) × 2 (CongruencyN-1) × 2 (Fluency) design and entered into a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). Additionally, for RT data analysis, erroneous as well as post-error trials (together 6.7%) and all RTs that exceeded more than two SDs from the individual cell mean (4.9%) were excluded prior to analysis. For the remaining data (on average 121 trials per participant), mean RTs for each cell of the 2 (CongruencyN) × 2 (CongruencyN-1) × 2 (Fluency) design were computed and a repeated measures ANOVA was conducted.

Results and Discussion

Error data

There was a marginally significant interaction of CongruencyN × CongruencyN-1, F(1,27) = 3.375, p = 0.077, η2 = 0.111, reflecting a typical conflict adaptation effect. The congruency effect was less pronounced following incongruentN-1 (–1.16%) as compared to following congruentN-1 trials (2.42%). Importantly, this conflict adaptation effect was further modulated by Fluency, F(1,27) = 4.675, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.145 (see Figure 1). In fluent trials, participants showed a significant conflict adaptation effect, F(1,27) = 5.783, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.176, i.e., an inverted congruency effect for trials following incongruent trials (–3.67%) as compared to following congruent trials (2.69%). In disfluent trials, however, the effect of CongruencyN was unaffected by CongruencyN-1, F < 1, p > 0.706, η2 < 0.006. No further effects were significant, all Fs < 2.873, all ps > 0.101, all η2s < 0.097.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. RTs (ms) and error rates (%) as a function of CongruencyN and CongruencyN-1 for fluent (A) and disfluent (B) trials of Experiment 1. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean. The symbol “*” denotes a significant interaction CongruencyN × CongruencyN-1.

RT data

The main effects of CongruencyN, F(1,27) = 22.515, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.464, and Fluency, F(1,27) = 5.051, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.163, were significant. RT was lower for congruentN (633.45 ms) as compared to incongruentN (673.90 ms) trials and lower for fluent (642.59 ms) as compared to disfluent (664.76 ms) trials. Furthermore, there was a significant interaction of CongruencyN × Fluency, F(1,27) = 7.239, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.218: The Congruency effect was less pronounced in fluent (12.63 ms) as compared to disfluent trials (68.27 ms). No further effects were significant, all Fs < 1.019, all ps > 0.321, all η2s < 0.039.

The main results of Experiment 1 can be summarized as follows: The higher order interaction of CongruencyN × CongruencyN-1 × Fluency found in the error data showed the usual significant conflict adaptation effect on fluent trials (i.e., stimuli with high stimulus contrast as used in standard paradigms), and a significantly reduced and virtually absent conflict adaptation effect for disfluent trials. The results thus suggest that, if anything, increasing the general aversiveness of conflicts by reducing the stimulus contrast eliminates the conflict adaptation effect. This contradicts the idea that adding unspecific aversiveness to a conflict stimulus increases specific adaptation effects. In order to consolidate the findings from Experiment 1, we ran a second experiment with a different response conflict paradigm, i.e., a manual version of the Stroop task (Stroop, 1935). If the results from Experiment 1 (the modulation of the conflict adaptation effect by fluency with an elimination thereof in disfluent trials) can be replicated in Experiment 2, it can be ruled out that the effects were driven by paradigm specific parameters and thus highlight the findings’ generalizability.

Experiment 2

Method

Participants

Thirty students of the University of Regensburg were tested (23 female; 28 right-handers; mean age: 23.1, SD = 3.7). All participants signed informed consent before the experiment and received 3 Euros or partial course credit after its completion. Data of three participants with RTs that were more than two SDs above at least one group cell mean were excluded from the analysis, leaving a final sample of 27 participants.

Material and procedure

Stimuli were the German color words for RED (rot), GREEN (grün), and BLUE (blau) printed in red, green, and blue (RGB values of 255,0,0; 0,255,0; and 0,0,255, respectively). The words were written in Arial bold, 24 point, each letter subtending a visual angle of approximately 0.8° × 0.8° at a viewing distance of 60 cm. Congruency was manipulated by color-word match or mismatch: The print color could either match (congruent stimuli) or mismatch (incongruent stimuli) the word meaning of the stimulus. Again, fluency was manipulated by figure-ground contrast differences. In fluent stimuli, color saturation was 100%, in disfluent stimuli, color saturation was 50%. The stimuli were presented centrally on a white background. Participants’ task was to quickly and accurately identify the print color of the word while ignoring its meaning by pressing one of three keys on a QWERTZ keyboard (“c” for green, “v” for red, “b” for blue, respectively) with their index, middle, and ring finger of their dominant hand. Trial and block procedure remained the same as in Experiment 1.

Design

A 2 (CongruencyN) × 2 (CongruencyN-1) × 2 (Fluency) repeated measures design was used.

Data preprocessing

We excluded the first two trials of each fluency block of 10 trials length in order to remove possible transition effects from the previous fluency condition. Furthermore, partial priming trials [whenever the color or color word repeated from trial N-1 to trial N (46.6%)] and negative priming trials [whenever the color word in trial N-1 was the color of the stimulus in trial N (16.2%)] were excluded prior to analysis to ensure that priming effects did not mask conflict adaptation. For error data analysis, mean error rates for the remaining data (on average 130 trials per participant) were computed for each cell of the 2 (CongruencyN) × 2 (CongruencyN-1) × 2 (Fluency) design and entered into a repeated measures ANOVA. Additionally, for RT data analysis, erroneous as well as post-error trials (together 7.5%) and all RTs that exceeded more than two SDs from the individual cell mean (4.6%) were excluded prior to analysis. For the remaining data (on average 118 trials per participant), mean RTs for each cell of the 2 (CongruencyN) × 2 (CongruencyN-1) × 2 (Fluency) design were computed and a repeated measures ANOVA was conducted.

Results and Discussion

Error data

There was a significant main effect of CongruencyN, F(1,26) = 16.335, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.386, and a marginally significant effect of Fluency, F(1,26) = 3.028, p = 0.094, η2 = 0.104. Error rates were lower for congruentN (2.39%) as compared to incongruentN (4.65%) trials and lower for fluent (3.19%) as compared to disfluent trials (3.85%). No further effects were significant, all Fs < 2.331, all ps > 0.138, all η2s < 0.083.

RT data

The main effects of CongruencyN, F(1,26) = 86.941, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.770, CongruencyN-1, F(1,26) = 10.821, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.294, and Fluency, F(1,26) = 5.371, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.171, were significant. RT was lower for trials following incongruent trials (636.34 ms) as compared to congruent trials (659.30 ms), lower for congruentN (582.21 ms) as compared to incongruentN (713.42 ms) trials and lower for fluent (636.71 ms) as compared to disfluent (658.92 ms) trials. As in the accuracy data of Experiment 1, the interaction of CongruencyN × CongruencyN-1 × Fluency was significant, F(1,26) = 6.604, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.203 (see Figure 2). In fluent trials, participants showed a significant conflict adaptation effect, F(1,26) = 8.220, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.240, i.e., a smaller congruency effect for trials following incongruent trials (104.56 ms) as compared to following congruent trials (154.42 ms). In disfluent trials, however, the congruency effect was unaffected by CongruencyN-1 (137.98 ms following incongruent and 127.86 ms following congruent trials), F = 0.323, p = 0.574, η2 = 0.012. No further effects were significant, all Fs < 1.932, ps > 0.175, all η2s < 0.070.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. RTs (ms) and error rates (%) as a function of CongruencyN and CongruencyN-1 for fluent (A) and disfluent (B) trials of Experiment 2. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean. The symbol “*” denotes a significant interaction CongruencyN × CongruencyN-1.

In Experiment 2, the higher order interaction CongruencyN × CongruencyN-1 × Fluency was significant in the RT data. Again, the conflict adaptation effect was only significant in fluent trials but was eliminated in disfluent trials. Taken together, both experiments brought up converging evidence that sequential conflict adaptation, if present in fluent trials, is entirely reduced in disfluent trials. This contradicts the idea that any aversive signal is suited to trigger stimulus-specific adaptation effects.

General Discussion

Based on a theoretical framework of ACC functioning (Botvinick, 2007) and recent findings of (1) conflict aversiveness (Dreisbach and Fischer, 2012b; Schouppe et al., 2012, 2015; Fritz and Dreisbach, 2013, 2014), (2) elimination of conflict adaptation by positive action effects (van Steenbergen et al., 2009, 2012), and (3) sequential adaptation triggered by non-conflict aversive (disfluent) stimuli (Dreisbach and Fischer, 2011), we directly tested whether increasing the aversive value of conflict stimuli also increases sequential adaptation effects. To this end, we presented conflict stimuli with either high or low perceptual fluency. Because disfluency is experienced as aversive signal (Reber et al., 1998), this manipulation is suited to modulate the affective valence of conflict stimuli. If conflict adaptation is triggered by the aversive nature of conflict stimuli independently from the conflict information, then the increased negative valence of disfluent incongruent as compared to fluent incongruent trials might increase adaptation effects.

Results from both experiments, however, did not support this idea. In contrast, whenever the typical conflict adaptation was found for fluent trials (in the error data in Experiment 1 and in the RT data in Experiment 2), disfluency eliminated conflict adaptation effects entirely. And this cannot be explained by reduced conflict strength on disfluent trials because conflict was either unaffected by the fluency manipulation (Experiment 2) or even increased (Experiment 1) for disfluent trials4.

In both experiments, conflict adaptation was only present in one of the dependent measures, i.e., error rates in Experiment 1 and RT data in Experiment 2. Thus, neither RT data in Experiment 1 nor error rates in Experiment 2 were further modulated by disfluency. Indeed, there have been many studies reporting similar findings, i.e., conflict adaptation effects being only present in RT data OR error data (see, e.g., Ullsperger et al., 2005; Bugg, 2008; van Steenbergen et al., 2010, 2012; Puccioni and Vallesi, 2012; Soutschek et al., 2012). So far, there has been no study that directly addressed why the conflict adaptation effect is sometimes found in the RT data while it is found in the error data in other cases. The important result of our study, however, is that the dependent measure that showed the typical conflict adaptation effect on fluent trials in the respective experiment (i.e., error rates in Experiment 1 and RT data in Experiment 2) also brought up a higher order interaction with fluency: While conflict adaptation is intact on fluent trials, disfluency leads to its elimination.

These consistent findings from two independent experiments have an important implication: They demonstrate that increasing unspecific aversiveness, for example by decreasing fluency of processing, does not inevitably lead to stronger conflict adaptation but in contrast may even diminish it. Thus, it is conceivable that aversiveness might need to be tied to conflict processing and not to stimulus processing in general.

An important question, however, remains: why does reduced fluency of processing (and thus, increased aversiveness) not only not increase or not affect conflict adaptation, but eliminates it? Here, a potential answer could be that reducing fluency of processing might come with side effects other than the aversive connotation that could directly have affected conflict adaptation.

For example, processing of disfluent stimuli might have increased processing demands and invested effort (e.g., Dreisbach and Fischer, 2011). There is already ample evidence that conflict adaptation is modulated by processing demands of primary task processing. For example, Fischer et al. (2008) had participants complete a number magnitude task (i.e., indicate whether a given number was bigger or smaller than 5) combined with a Simon task (i.e., numbers appeared on the right or left side of the screen). They found the typical sequential conflict adaptation in the Simon task which was further modulated by the cognitive demand of the number magnitude task: Following numbers close to the reference standard (high processing demand), the Simon adaptation was smaller than following numbers far from the reference standard (low processing demand). Likewise, Soutschek et al. (2012) reported evidence that high working memory load eliminates conflict adaptation in the Stroop task. Applied to our results presented here, one might thus argue that disfluent trials draw on processing resources that were then not available for conflict adaptation. Does that imply that the aversive character of disfluency had no effect in our study? Interestingly, Pessoa (2009) claimed that not only different cognitive processes share and compete for the same restricted resource capacities, but that cognitive and affective processes do so as well. Indeed, it has been shown that performance in incongruent trials decreased when preceded by an affective task-irrelevant picture (Hart et al., 2010), suggesting that the processing of the affective stimulus consumed resources that would otherwise have benefited conflict resolution. In the same line and more directly related to our study, Padmala et al. (2011) reported that presenting highly arousing negative pictures as compared to neutral pictures in inter-trial-intervals of a Stroop-like word-face task eliminated conflict adaptation effects (see also Braem et al., 2013b). The authors, too, explained this finding in terms of resource competition: The resources that are necessary for post-conflict adaptation were consumed by the processing of the arousing aversive pictures and were then lacking for conflict adaptation. In the light of these findings, the resource competition account might explain our data, as well. As disfluency is associated with negative affect, the processing of the aversive quality in disfluent blocks may have demanded resources that would otherwise have been used to adapt control in post-conflict trials5.

A second, very recent line of research that is of interest to our results deals with the motivational impact of conflict resolution on conflict adaptation. According to this adaptation-by-motivation account, conflict adaptation is triggered by the rewarding experience of conflict resolution (Braem et al., 2012). This idea is grounded on the observation that solving a difficult task is more rewarding than solving an easy task (Shalley and Oldham, 1985; Satterthwaite et al., 2012). For example, Satterthwaite et al. (2012) used an n-back task and were able to show that the activation in the ventral striatum, a key region of dopamine production, increased with increasing task difficulty. The most direct evidence for the role of reward for conflict adaptation has recently been put forward by Braem et al. (2012). In that study, participants were presented with an Eriksen Flanker task (Experiment 1). In the experimental condition, 25% of trials of a given block were rewarded for correct and fast performance, whereas in the remaining 75% of the trials, no reward was given. In the control condition, no reward was given ever. Results brought up sequential conflict adaptation effects in the control condition (no reward) and in rewarded trials in the experimental condition. Intriguingly, no such conflict adaptation was found for unrewarded trials in the reward context. According to the authors, the extrinsic reward signal on 25% of trials replaced or overshadowed the intrinsic reward signal normally generated in standard (no-reward) conflict tasks. As a consequence, the no-reward trials lacked the intrinsic rewarding experience that would have been necessary to trigger conflict adaptation.

Back to the data presented here, the adaptation-by-motivation account also fits with our findings. Notably, fluency of processing has been shown to modulate motivation directly. For example, Song and Schwarz (2008) found that participants were less motivated to carry out a task that was described in a hard to read (disfluent) font as compared to a task described in an easy to read (fluent) font. Applied to our experiments, the continuous experience of disfluency throughout the mini-blocks of disfluent trials might have reduced the motivation to adapt. Put differently, in disfluent mini-blocks, the rewarding effect of a successful conflict resolution might have been counteracted by the discouraging continuous disfluent experience. Therefore, the repeated disfluent experience eliminated the intrinsic reward signal that typically follows successful conflict resolution, thereby decreasing the conflict adaptation effect. Further support for this motivational account comes from studies showing that an increase in participants’ motivation goes along with decreased RTs and error rates and decreased congruency effects, mimicking our results in the fluent as compared to the disfluent conditions (e.g., Veling and Aarts, 2010; Padmala and Pessoa, 2011; Soutschek et al., 2014).

In sum, the two accounts presented above, i.e., the adaptation-by-motivation account, and the resource competition account, are equally well suited to explain our results. In fact, they are not mutually exclusive but closely intervened. After all, the negative valence of disfluency (just as the negative valence of conflict stimuli, see Botvinick, 2007) might at least in part be due to the increased processing demands of disfluent (and incongruent) trials. The only caveat might be that our results are hard to reconcile with the interpretation of van Steenbergen et al. (2009) outlined in the Introduction. To reiterate, the authors found no conflict adaptation following positive signals and argued that the positive signals presumably counteracted the aversive character of the conflict stimulus. Alternatively, and in line with the adaptation-by-motivation account, the positive signals in the van Steenbergen study that were presented as non-contingent performance feedback might have signaled that successful performance is not a value by itself and thereby counteracted the intrinsic reward signal (see also Dreisbach and Fischer, 2012a, for a more thorough discussion). In sum, random reward (van Steenbergen et al., 2009, 2012; see also Stürmer et al., 2011), no-reward in a reward context (Braem et al., 2012) and repeated experience of disfluency (the results presented here) have all been found to reduce or eliminate conflict adaptation. The common underlying mechanism might be that in all these situations, the intrinsic reward signal after successful conflict resolution was reduced.

It is important to note that the present findings and the suggested interpretations do not at all contradict the repeatedly shown aversive nature of conflict stimuli and their role for conflict adaptation (Dreisbach and Fischer, 2012b; Schouppe et al., 2012, 2015; Fritz and Dreisbach, 2013, 2014). What we have shown here is that increasing the negative valence of conflict stimuli via disfluency (and thus independently from conflict strength) does not increase conflict adaptation effects. But at the same time, it is well documented that (1) disfluency triggers processing adjustments in terms of a reduced fluency effect (Dreisbach and Fischer, 2011) and that (2) conflict adaptation effects increase with increasing conflict strength (Takezawa and Miyatani, 2005; Forster et al., 2011; Wendt et al., 2014). Therefore, we argue that the aversive signal conveyed by the amount of conflict triggers conflict adaptation. Yet it seems that aversive stimulus information from different sources (here: from perceptual fluency vs. response conflict) does not add up to increase sequential conflict adaptation. That is, the aversiveness must be tied to conflict processing and not to stimulus processing in general.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

This research was supported by a grant to the third author (GD) from the German Research Foundation (DFG: DR 392/6-1) and furthermore within the DFG’s funding programme Open Access Publishing.

Footnotes

  1. ^ It is important to note that it has been shown empirically that not only is perceptual fluency associated with positive affect but also that perceptual disfluency is associated with negative affect. In three experiments, Reber et al. (1998) applied different manipulations of perceptual fluency and examined the consequences on preference judgments. In Experiment 1, participants judged drawings that were preceded by mismatching primes (i.e., perceptually disfluent stimuli) as being less pretty than drawings that were preceded by matching primes. In Experiment 3, participants stated to dislike stimuli more that were presented for shorter periods of time than stimuli that were presented for longer time periods. Finally, and most similar to the manipulation of perceptual fluency used in the present study, in Experiment 2, Reber et al. (1998) let participants judge circle stimuli that varied in figure-ground contrast. They found that the circles were judged as less pretty and more ugly with decreasing figure-ground contrast, i.e., with decreasing perceptual fluency.
  2. ^ There are a couple of studies, however, suggesting that it is not the affective value of conflicts but the rewarding effect of conflict resolution that leads to post-conflict adjustment (Braem et al., 2012; see also Schouppe et al., 2015).
  3. ^ Because the post hoc removal of all stimulus/feature repetitions is standard procedure in studying conflict adaptation, the experiment was from the beginning designed to provide a sufficiently large number of trials per cell. An a priory exclusion of critical stimulus sequences was not considered an option as it induces expectation biases. For a recent discussion of theoretical and practical guidelines in the investigations of conflict adaptation see Duthoo et al. (2014) and Egner (2014).
  4. ^ Miles and Proctor (2009) found that decreasing both, the discriminability of the relevant and irrelevant stimulus feature, does not change the magnitude of the congruency effect in two response conflict paradigms. Thus, we assumed that in our experiments, the fluency manipulation would likewise leave the congruency effect unaffected. However, the interaction of fluency with congruency was significant for the Flanker task in Experiment 1. Although we do not have an explanation for this effect, still, this does not weaken our argument: conflict adaptation is absent in disfluent as compared to fluent trials even though conflict magnitude was bigger in disfluent trials. It seems thus that the demotivating effect of continued disfluency (see Discussion below) overrules the effect of enhanced conflict aversiveness.
  5. ^ An alternative, yet similar, explanation of reduced conflict adaptation effects in affectively negative conditions grounds on the idea that the effects of negative mood (cf. van Steenbergen, 2015) or more generally, arousal (cf. Braem et al., 2013a), on cognitive control follow an inverted U shaped function: while moderate levels of arousal are beneficial for cognitive control processes, too little or too much arousal is detrimental. For example, Braem et al. (2013b) found that the influence of punishment on conflict adaptation critically depends on individual punishment sensitivity: while punishing individuals low in punishment sensitivity [as measured with the Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) scale] increased conflict adaptation in post-punishment trials, punishing highly punishment sensitive participants did not modulate conflict adaptation but lead to a general slow-down in RTs in post-punishment trials. The authors interpret their data in the framework of the Yerkes and Dodson (1908) law and suggest that for highly punishment sensitive individuals, punishment-associated arousal was too high to benefit conflict adaptation. However, we do not think that this framework is suited to thoroughly explain our results, given that disfluency can hardly be compared to the aversive experience of punishment.

References

Blais, C., Robidoux, S., Risko, E. F., and Besner, D. (2007). Item-specific adaptation and the conflict-monitoring hypothesis: a computational model. Psychol. Rev. 114, 1076–1086. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.4.1076

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blais, C., and Verguts, T. (2012). Increasing set size breaks down sequential congruency: evidence for an associative locus of cognitive control. Acta Psychol. 141, 133–139. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.07.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M. (2007). Conflict monitoring and decision making: reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 356–366. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.4.356

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M., Cohen, J. D., and Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 539–546. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature 402, 179–181. doi: 10.1038/46035

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braem, S., Abrahamse, E. L., Duthoo, W., and Notebaert, W. (2014). What determines the specificity of conflict adaptation? A review, critical analysis, and proposed synthesis. Front. Psychol. 5:1134. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01134

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braem, S., Duthoo, W., and Notebaert, W. (2013a). Punishment sensitivity predicts the impact of punishment on cognitive control. PLoS ONE 8:e74106. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braem, S., King, J. A., Korb, F. M., Krebs, R. M., Notebaert, W., and Egner, T. (2013b). Affective modulation of cognitive control is determined by performance-contingency and mediated by ventromedial prefrontal and cingulate cortex. J. Neurosci. 33, 16961–16970. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1208-13.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braem, S., Verguts, T., Roggeman, C., and Notebaert, W. (2012). Reward modulates adaptations to conflict. Cognition 125, 324–332. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bugg, J. M. (2008). Opposing influences on conflict-adaptation in the Eriksen flanker task. Mem. Cogn. 36, 1217–1227. doi: 10.3758/MC.36.7.1217

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caessens, B., Notebaert, W., Burle, B., and Soetens, E. (2005). Voluntary and involuntary control over automatic processing in spatial congruency tasks: editors’ introduction. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 17, 577–589. doi: 10.1080/09541440540000130

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duthoo, W., Abrahamse, E. L., Braem, S., Boehler, C. N., and Notebaert, W. (2014). The congruency sequence effect 3.0: a critical test of conflict adaptation. PLoS ONE 9:e110462. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0110462

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreisbach, G., and Fischer, R. (2011). If it’s hard to read… try harder! Processing fluency as signal for effort adjustments. Psychol. Res. 75, 376–383. doi: 10.1007/s00426-010-0319-y

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreisbach, G., and Fischer, R. (2012a). The role of affect and reward in the conflict-triggered adjustment of cognitive control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:342. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00342

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreisbach, G., and Fischer, R. (2012b). Conflicts as aversive signals. Brain Cogn. 78, 94–98. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.12.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreisbach, G., and Fischer, R. (in press a). Conflicts as aversive signals for control adaptation. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci.

Google Scholar

Dreisbach, G., and Fischer, R. (in press b). “Conflicts as aversive signals: motivation for control adaptation in the service of affect regulation,” in Motivation and Cognitive Control, ed. T. S. Braver (New York, NY: Psychology Press).

Google Scholar

Egner, T. (2007). Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 380–390.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Egner, T. (2008). Multiple conflict-driven control mechanisms in the human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12, 374–380. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.07.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egner, T. (2014). Creatures of habit (and control): a multi-level learning perspective on the modulation of congruency effects. Front. Psychol. 5:1247. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01247

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., and Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science 302, 290–292. doi: 10.1126/science.1089134

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksen, B. A., and Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophys. 16, 143–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03203267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, R., Dreisbach, G., and Goschke, T. (2008). Context-sensitive adjustments of cognitive control: conflict-adaptation effects are modulated by processing demands of the ongoing task. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 34, 712–718. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.712

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, R., Plessow, F., Kunde, W., and Kiesel, A. (2010). Trial-to-trial modulations of the Simon effect in conditions of attentional limitations: evidence from dual tasks. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 36, 1576–1594. doi: 10.1037/a0019326

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritz, J., and Dreisbach, G. (2013). Conflicts as aversive signals: conflict priming increases negative judgments for neutral stimuli. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 13, 311–317. doi: 10.3758/s13415-012-0147-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritz, J., and Dreisbach, G. (2014). The time course of the aversive conflict signal. Exp. Psychol. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000271 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forster, S. E., Carter, C. S., Cohen, J. D., and Cho, R. Y. (2011). Parametric manipulation of the conflict signal and control-state adaptation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 923–935. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21458

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funes, M. J., Lupianez, J., and Humphreys, G. (2010). Analyzing the generality of conflict adaptation effects. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 36, 147–161. doi: 10.1037/a0017598

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goschke, T., and Dreisbach, G. (2008). Conflict-triggered goal shielding: response conflicts attenuate background monitoring for prospective memory cues. Psychol. Sci. 19, 25–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02042.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., and Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 121, 480–506. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.480

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hart, S. J., Green, S. R., Casp, M., and Belger, A. (2010). Emotional priming effects during Stroop task performance. Neuroimage 49, 2662–2670. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.076

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holroyd, C. B., and Coles, M. G. H. (2002). The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol. Rev. 109, 679–709. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.109.4.679

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hommel, B., Proctor, R. W., and Vu, K.-P. L. (2004). A feature-integration account of sequential effects in the Simon task. Psychol. Res. 68, 1–17. doi: 10.1007/s00426-003-0132-y

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, J., Heller, K., and Egner, T. (2014). Bayesian modeling of flexible cognitive control. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 46, 30–43. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.06.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A., and Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303, 1023–1026. doi: 10.1126/science.1089910

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhbandner, C., and Zehetleitner, M. (2011). Dissociable effects of valence and arousal in adaptive executive control. PLoS ONE 6:e29287. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029287

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiesel, A., Kunde, W., and Hoffmann, J. (2006). Evidence for task-specific resolution of response conflict. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13, 800–806.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobayashi, N., Yoshino, A., Takahashi, Y., and Nomura, S. (2007). Autonomic arousal in cognitive conflict resolution. Auton. Neurosci. 132, 70–75. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2006.09.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kunde, W., and Wühr, P. (2006). Sequential modulations of correspondence effects across spatial dimensions and tasks. Mem. Cogn. 34, 356–367. doi: 10.3758/BF03193413

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larson, M. J., Kaufman, D. A. S., and Perlstein, W. M. (2009). Neural time course of conflict adaptation effects on the Stroop task. Neuropsychologia 47, 663–670. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayr, U., Awh, E., and Laurey, P. (2003). Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 450–452. doi: 10.1038/nn1051

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miles, J. D., and Proctor, R. W. (2009). Reducing and restoring stimulus-response compatibility effects by decreasing the discriminability of location words. Acta Psychol. 130, 95–102. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.10.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieuwenhuis, S., Yeung, N., Holroyd, C. B., Schurger, A., and Cohen, J. D. (2004). Sensitivity of electrophysiological activity from medial frontal cortex to utilitarian and performance feedback. Cereb. Cortex 14, 741–747. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh034

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Notebaert, W., and Braem, S. (in press). “Parsing the effects of reward on cognitive control,” in Motivation and Cognitive Control, ed. T. Braver (New York: NY: Psychology Press).

Google Scholar

Notebaert, W., Soetens, E., and Melis, A. (2001). Sequential analysis of a Simon task—evidence for an attention-shift account. Psychol. Res. 65, 170–184. doi: 10.1007/s004260000054

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Notebaert, W., and Verguts, T. (2008). Cognitive control acts locally. Cognition 106, 1071–1080. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padmala, S., Bauer, A., and Pessoa, L. (2011). Negative emotion impairs conflict-driven executive control. Front. Psychol. 2:192. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00192

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padmala, S., and Pessoa, L. (2011). Reward reduces conflict by enhancing attentional control and biasing visual cortical processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 231, 3419–3432. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pastötter, B., Dreisbach, G., and Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2013). Dynamic adjustments of cognitive control: oscillatory correlates of the conflict-adaptation effect. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 2167–2178. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00474

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pessoa, L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 160–166. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plessow, F., Fischer, R., Kirschbaum, C., and Goschke, T. (2011). Inflexibly focused under stress: acute psychosocial stress increases shielding of action goals at the expense of reduced cognitive flexibility with increasing time lag to the stressor. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3218–3227. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puccioni, O., and Vallesi, A. (2012). Sequential congruency effects: disentangling priming and conflict adaptation. Psychol. Res. 76, 591–600. doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0360-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rainville, P. (2002). Brain mechanisms of pain affect and pain modulation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 12, 195–204. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00313-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reber, R., Winkielman, P., and Schwarz, N. (1998). Effects of perceptual fluency on affective judgments. Psychol. Sci. 9, 45–48. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Renaud, P., and Blondin, J.-P. (1997). The stress of Stroop performance: physiological and emotional responses to color-word interference, task pacing, and pacing speed. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 27, 87–97. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00049-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satterthwaite, T. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J., Elliott, M. A., Gerraty, R. T., Calkins, M. E., et al. (2012). Being right is its own reward: load and performance related ventral striatum activation to correct responses during a working memory task in youth. Neuroimage 61, 723–729. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.060

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schacht, A., Dimigen, O., and Sommer, W. (2010). Emotions in cognitive conflicts are not aversive but are task specific. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neursoci. 10, 349–356. doi: 10.3758/CABN.10.3.349

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scherbaum, S., Fischer, R., Dshemuchadse, M., and Goschke, T. (2011). The dynamics of cognitive control: evidence for within-trial conflict adaptation from frequency-tagged EEG. Psychophysiology 48, 591–600. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01137.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schouppe, N., Braem, S., De Houwer, J., Silvetti, M., Verguts, T., Ridderinkhof, K. R., et al. (2015). No pain, no gain: the affective valence of congruency conditions changes following a successful response. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. doi: 10.3758/s13415-014-0318-3 [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schouppe, N., De Houwer, J., and Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2012). Conflict?: run! Reduced Stroop interference with avoidance responses. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 65, 1052–1058. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.685080

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shalley, C. E., and Oldham, G. R. (1985). Effects of goal difficulty and expected external evaluation on intrinsic motivation: a laboratory study. Acad. Manage. J. 28, 628–640. doi: 10.2307/256118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., and Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162. doi: 10.1126/science.1093535

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, H., and Schwarz, N. (2008). If it’s hard to read, it’s hard to do. Processing fluency affects effort prediction and motivation. Psychol. Sci. 19, 986–988. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02189.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soutschek, A., Strobach, T., and Schubert, T. (2012). Working memory demands modulate cognitive control in the Stroop paradigm. Psychol. Res. 77, 333–347. doi: 10.1007/s00426-012-0429-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soutschek, A., Strobach, T., and Schubert, T. (2014). Motivational and cognitive determinants of control during conflict processing. Cogn. Emot. 28, 1076–1089. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.870134

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662. doi: 10.1037/h0054651

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schröter, H., and Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 28, 1345–1363. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.28.6.1345

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stürmer, B., Nigbur, R., Schacht, A., and Sommer, W. (2011). Reward and punishment effects on error processing and conflict control. Front. Psychol. 2:335. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00335

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stürmer, B., Seiss, E., and Leuthold, H. (2005). Executive control in the Simon task: a dual-task examination of response priming and its suppression. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 17, 590–618. doi: 10.1080/09541440540000077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takezawa, T., and Miyatani, M. (2005). Quantitative relation between conflict and response inhibition in the Flanker task. Psychol. Rep. 97, 515–526. doi: 10.2466/pr0.97.2.515-526

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ullsperger, M., Bylsma, L. M., and Botvinick, M. M. (2005). The conflict adaptation effect: it’s not just priming. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 5, 467–472. doi: 10.3758/CABN.5.4.467

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Steenbergen, H. (2015). “Affective modulation of cognitive control: a biobehavioral perspective,” in Biobehavioral Foundations of Self-Regulation, eds G. Gendolla, M. Tops, and S. Koole (Heidelberg: Springer), 89–107.

Google Scholar

van Steenbergen, H., and Band, G. P. H. (2013). Pupil dilation in the Simon task as a marker of conflict processing. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:215. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00215

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Steenbergen, H., Band, G. P. H., and Hommel, B. (2009). Reward counteracts conflict adaptation. Evidence for a role of affect in executive control. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1473–1477. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02470.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Steenbergen, H., Band, G. P. H., and Hommel, B. (2010). In the mood for adaptation: how affect regulates conflict-driven control. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1629–1634. doi: 10.1177/0956797610385951

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Steenbergen, H., Band, G. P. H., and Hommel, B. (2012). Reward valence modulates conflict-driven attentional adaptation: electrophysiological evidence. Biol. Psychol. 90, 234–241. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veling, H., and Aarts, H. (2010). Cueing task goals and earning money: relatively high monetary rewards reduce failures to act on goals in a Stroop task. Motiv. Emot. 34, 184–190. doi: 10.1007/s11031-010-9160-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verguts, T., and Notebaert, W. (2009). Adaptation by binding: a learning account of cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 252–257. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.02.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wendt, M., Kiesel, A., Gehringswald, F., Purmann, S., and Fischer, R. (2014). Attentional adjustment to conflict strength: evidence from the effects of manipulating flanker-target SOA on response times and prestimulus pupil size. Exp. Psychol. 61, 55–67. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000227

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wendt, M., Luna-Rodriguez, A., and Jacobsen, T. (2012). Conflict-induced perceptual filtering. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 38, 675–686. doi: 10.1037/a0025902

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winkielman, P., Schwarz, N., Fazendeiro, T. A., and Reber, R. (2003). “The hedonic marking of processing fluency: implications for evaluative judgment,” in The Psychology of Evaluation: Affective Processes in Cognition and Emotion, eds J. Musch and K. C. Klauer (Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum), 189–217.

Google Scholar

Wühr, P., and Kunde, W. (2008). Die kognitive regulation von handlungskonflikten. Psychol. Rundsch. 59, 207–216. doi: 10.1026/0033-3042.59.4.207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yerkes, R. M., and Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit formation. J. Comp. Neurol. Psychol. 18, 459–482. doi: 10.1002/cne.920180503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: conflict adaptation, aversive signal, fluency of processing

Citation: Fritz J, Fischer R and Dreisbach G (2015) The influence of negative stimulus features on conflict adaption: evidence from fluency of processing. Front. Psychol. 6:185. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00185

Received: 12 November 2014; Accepted: 05 February 2015;
Published online: 26 February 2015.

Edited by:

Christoph T. Weidemann, Swansea University, UK

Reviewed by:

Senne Braem, Ghent University, Belgium
Alexander Soutschek, University of Zürich, Switzerland

Copyright © 2015 Fritz, Fischer and Dreisbach. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Julia Fritz, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Regensburg, Universitätsstraße 31, D-93053 Regensburg, Germany e-mail: julia.fritz@psychologie.uni-regensburg.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.