Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 26 March 2015
Sec. Psychopathology

Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Denver, Denver, CO, USA
  • 2Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, USA

Executive function (EF) is essential for successfully navigating nearly all of our daily activities. Of critical importance for clinical psychological science, EF impairments are associated with most forms of psychopathology. However, despite the proliferation of research on EF in clinical populations, with notable exceptions clinical and cognitive approaches to EF have remained largely independent, leading to failures to apply theoretical and methodological advances in one field to the other field and hindering progress. First, we review the current state of knowledge of EF impairments associated with psychopathology and limitations to the previous research in light of recent advances in understanding and measuring EF. Next, we offer concrete suggestions for improving EF assessment. Last, we suggest future directions, including integrating modern models of EF with state of the art, hierarchical models of dimensional psychopathology as well as translational implications of EF-informed research on clinical science.

Introduction

Executive function (EF) is essential for successfully navigating nearly all of our daily activities. EF is comprised of a set of cognitive control processes, mainly supported by the prefrontal cortex (PFC), which regulate lower level processes (e.g., perception, motor responses) and thereby enable self-regulation and self-directed behavior toward a goal, allowing us to break out of habits, make decisions and evaluate risks, plan for the future, prioritize and sequence our actions, and cope with novel situations (e.g., Banich, 2009; Miyake and Friedman, 2012). Individual differences in EF are associated with many important aspects of human health and functioning, including academic and occupational functioning (e.g., Best et al., 2009; Miller et al., 2012b; Valiente et al., 2013), interpersonal problems (e.g., Sprague et al., 2011; De Panfilis et al., 2013), substance use (e.g., Nigg et al., 2006; Ersche et al., 2012), physical health (e.g., Hall et al., 2006; Falkowski et al., 2014), and mental health (e.g., Willcutt et al., 2005; Bora et al., 2009; Mesholam-Gately et al., 2009; Snyder, 2013).

Of critical importance for clinical psychological science, EF impairments are associated with most forms of psychopathology, as discussed below. Moreover, poor EF predicts rumination (e.g., Whitmer and Banich, 2007; De Lissnyder et al., 2012; Demeyer et al., 2012; Zetsche et al., 2012), worry (Crowe et al., 2007; Snyder et al., 2010, 2014) and poor use of adaptive emotion regulation strategies (e.g., reappraisal, McRae et al., 2012; Andreotti et al., 2013), which are all potent risk factors for multiple forms of psychopathology (e.g., Ruscio et al., 2007; Aldao et al., 2010; Abela and Hankin, 2011; McLaughlin and Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011). Thus, it has been proposed that EF deficits may be transdiagnostic intermediate phenotypes or risk factors for emotional, behavioral, and psychotic disorders (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins, 2011; Buckholtz and Meyer-Lindenberg, 2012; Goschke, 2014).

However, despite the proliferation of research on EF in clinical populations, the history of cognitive approaches in psychopathology has followed a curious path, best illustrated as mostly parallel play, between two predominantly independent scientific traditions: clinical psychology/psychiatry and cognitive psychology/cognitive neuroscience. With notable exceptions, this theme of parallel play between clinical and cognitive science is largely reflected up to the present, and sometimes leads to failures to apply theoretical and methodological advances in one field to the other field, hindering progress.

This paper has three main goals. First, we review the current state of knowledge of EF impairments associated with psychopathology and limitations to the previous research in light of recent advances in understanding and measuring EF. Specifically, while EF impairments appear to be transdiagnostically associated with psychopathology, conceptual and methodological limitations of prior research make existing evidence difficult to interpret–thus, the specific nature and pattern of EF impairments, both across different aspects of EF and across forms of psychopathology, remains unclear. We argue that investigating how specific aspects of psychopathology affect, and are affected by, specific aspects of EF is critical for pushing clinical psychological science forward toward beginning to understand the underlying cognitive, neural, and genetic mechanisms involved at a level that will enable translational research to improve interventions. Next, we offer concrete suggestions for improving assessment of EF, based on both conceptual and methodological issues in current research practices with EF, to advance clinical psychological science. We advocate for better assessment of EF using the best current, validated models of EF and best methods for assessing EF. Specifically, we provide recommendations for applying validated models of EF to clinical research, using multiple tasks to obtain purer measures of EF, and selecting and analyzing tasks in ways that minimize the inherent noisiness of EF data. Last, we suggest future directions in research with EF and clinical psychological science, including integrating modern models of EF with state of the art, hierarchical models of dimensional psychopathology as well as translational implications of EF-informed research on clinical science.

Ef Impairments Associated With Psychopathology: Current State of Knowledge

Executive function is best characterized as consisting of separable but related cognitive processes, with both unique and shared individual differences, genetic influences, and neural substrates (e.g., Miyake and Friedman, 2012), a topic we return to in the following section. Table 1 defines the aspects of EF that have been most heavily studied in clinical populations, including shifting, inhibition, updating, and working memory (WM). Importantly, many of these components can be further subdivided. For example, manipulating information in WM places heavier demands on EF (i.e., the central executive component of WM) than simple maintenance (e.g., Baddeley and Repovs, 2006). WM maintenance can further be divided into verbal (e.g., words, letters and numbers) and visuospatial (e.g., shapes, patterns and spatial locations), while the central executive component of WM is believed to be domain-general (e.g., Baddeley and Repovs, 2006). It should be noted that in the literature, terms such as attentional control and executive attention are sometimes used to refer to the same tasks and cognitive processes referred to elsewhere as EF. This is largely a matter of differences in terminology rather than substance, as there is often agreement between those employing different terminology on the underlying cognitive and neural mechanisms (e.g., Petersen and Posner, 2012). We further elaborate on EF constructs in Conceptual Models below.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Executive function (EF) processes and measures.

Only tasks with emotionally neutral materials (i.e., “cold” EF tasks) were included in the current review. This was done to avoid confounding altered emotional processing with EF impairments. That is, impairments on tasks involving affective or disorder-related materials could arise from either impairments in EF processes or increased salience of these materials, and thus distractibility, for individuals with psychopathology, making results difficult to interpret. However, it should be noted that impairments on “hot” EF tasks are also present across many disorders, and in some cases may be larger than those on “cold” EF tasks, especially when disorder-specific materials are used (for reviews, see Williams et al., 1996; Bar-Haim et al., 2007; Peckham et al., 2010; Cisler et al., 2011).

The previous research on EF impairments associated with psychopathology reviewed in this section has primarily used cross-sectional (case-control) designs in adult samples, and assessed EF with the traditional neuropsychological tasks in the third column of Table 1. Despite considerable knowledge, as summarized in Table 2 and below, there are several important limitations inherent in the primary literature. These methodological and conceptual issues impose constraints on the state of knowledge and what can be determined through meta-analysis. First, many neuropsychological EF measures tap multiple aspects of EF as well as non-EF abilities. Such traditional but non-specific tasks may be useful for screening individuals for severe EF deficits, however, they are too broad to answer fine-grained questions about specific aspects of EF and potential underlying mechanisms relating EF aspects to forms of psychopathology. For example, verbal fluency tasks have been a perennial favorite for assessing EF. However, the verbal fluency and other complex neuropsychological tests tap a wide variety of cognitive processes, including not only multiple aspects of EF (e.g., shifting between subcategories, WM for what items have already been named), but also non-executive abilities (e.g., semantic memory). Even seemingly more specific tasks, such as the Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST) require other complex cognitive processes (e.g., learning from feedback). Second, because they were developed to detect more severe deficits (e.g., due to brain damage) many traditional neuropsychological tasks may lack sensitivity to detect more subtle EF deficits. That is, in some cases effect sizes for a particular EF task may be smaller than another not because of true differences in the magnitude of impairments on different aspects of EF, but merely because one task suffers from ceiling effects.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Summary of recent EF meta-analyses.

Finally, these limitations carry over into meta-analyses. Both in our own classification in Table 2 and many of the original meta-analyses summarized in the table, tasks are grouped into the EF processes they are commonly considered to tap (e.g., WCST is classified as a shifting task). However, these categories may be lumping together tasks that may actually be tapping different, and multiple, processes. Specifically, tasks classified as tapping a particular aspect of EF may not be sensitive measures of that process, and are not pure measures of that EF process, since they often require other EF processes as well as multiple non-EF processes.

Despite these limitations, the meta-analytic evidence summarized in Table 2 indicates that EF deficits are pervasive across disorders and EF tasks, although the magnitudes of these deficits vary. We first briefly summarize what is currently known based on this previous research, and argue that progress in understanding the nature, origins, and consequences of EF impairments associated with psychopathology will require more specific measures and better conceptual models. We then lay out some concrete suggestions for advancing research in these directions.

Impairments on More Specific EF Components: Inhibition, Shifting, Updating, and Working Memory

The largest EF deficits are found for individuals with schizophrenia, with large effect sizes on measures of shifting, inhibition, updating, visuospatial WM, and verbal manipulation, and a medium effect size for simple verbal WM maintenance. These EF tasks are also impaired in individuals with mood disorders, although the magnitude of these deficits is somewhat smaller than those in schizophrenia. Meta-analytic evidence demonstrates that individuals with major depression (MDD) are significantly impaired, with similar small-to-medium effect sizes, on measures tapping shifting, inhibition, updating, and WM. Similarly, while individuals with bipolar disorders (BDs) have somewhat larger impairments than individuals with MDD, they are also relatively uniformly impaired across EF domains, with medium effect sizes for shifting, inhibition, visuospatial WM, and verbal WM manipulation, and a small but significant effect for verbal WM maintenance. There is little research on updating in individuals with BD. Individuals with obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) also have impaired performance across these core EF domains, with small but significant effect sizes for shifting, inhibition, visuospatial WM, and verbal WM manipulation, but a large effect size for updating. In contrast, simple WM maintenance appears to be unimpaired in individuals with OCD. Importantly, while depression frequently co-occurs with OCD, EF deficits in OCD are not driven by co-occurring depression, as even those with low levels of depressive symptoms show the same level of EF deficits (Snyder et al., 2015).

A recent meta-analysis found that compared to trauma-exposed individuals who did not develop post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), individuals with PTSD had worse performance on measures of shifting, with a medium effect size, and visuospatial WM, with a small effect size, but not the Stroop measure of inhibition (other EF components were not analyzed; Polak et al., 2012). However, a review of the literature including a wider range of inhibition tasks suggests that individuals with PTSD do experience inhibition deficits (Aupperle et al., 2012). A recent meta-analysis, which analyzed all EF tasks in a single analysis without distinguishing between EF components found an effect size of d = 0.45 (Scott et al., 2015). Importantly, unlike OCD, co-occurring depression may account for EF deficits in individuals with PTSD, although more research in individuals without severe depressive symptoms is needed to confirm this finding (Polak et al., 2012).

There has been little research on EF in anxiety disorders, and that limited literature has yielded mixed findings. While a few studies have found impairments in shifting associated with panic disorder, social anxiety disorder and generalized anxiety disorder (Cohen et al., 1996; Airaksinen et al., 2005; Mantella et al., 2007), others have found no evidence of impairment in shifting (Purcell et al., 1998; Airaksinen et al., 2005; Boldrini et al., 2005), or inhibition (van den Heuvel et al., 2005; Van der Linden et al., 2005; Price and Mohlman, 2007). However, research in non-clinical samples suggests that trait anxiety, and especially anxious apprehension (worry) is associated with impairments in a specific aspect of EF, inhibiting competing responses (Bishop, 2008; Snyder et al., 2010, 2014; Eysenck and Derakshan, 2011). There has been very little research on WM in individuals with anxiety disorders, but there have been reports of impaired visuospatial WM in individuals with panic disorder (Boldrini et al., 2005), and impaired verbal WM manipulation, but not maintenance, in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder (Christopher and MacDonald, 2005). In addition, there is evidence that poor EF might contribute to attentional bias toward threat in anxious individuals, which in turn is involved in maintenance of anxiety (Heeren et al., 2013). A full discussion of the extensive attention bias literature is beyond the scope of this review, and we refer interested readers to recent comprehensive reviews of this topic (Bar-Haim et al., 2007; Heeren et al., 2013).

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in both children and adults is associated with impairments in shifting, inhibition, visuospatial WM and verbal WM manipulation, with small-to-medium effect sizes, while verbal WM maintenance is much less impaired (small effect size). Updating has not been widely studied in individuals with ADHD. While earlier theories posited a core inhibitory deficit that secondarily disrupts other aspects of EF (e.g., Barkley, 1997), recent meta-analyses demonstrate that only motor response inhibition tasks (e.g., stop signal and go/no-go) show substantial deficits, while the Stroop measure of inhibition shows only a small effect size. EF is also impaired in other externalizing disorders, including oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder, but these deficits may be accounted for at least in part by co-occurring ADHD (see Ogilvie et al., 2011 for meta-analysis).

Finally, there is evidence of deficits in EF associated with substance use disorders. Meta-analyses suggest that there are deficits in response inhibition associated with use/dependence on cocaine, MDMA, methamphetamine, tobacco, and alcohol, but not opioids or cannabis (Smith et al., 2014), and deficits in shifting and inhibition associated with cocaine use (Spronk et al., 2013). Another review found shifting, inhibition, and WM impairments across most substance use disorders, generally with medium effect sizes, but did not perform a formal meta-analysis (Fernandez-Serrano et al., 2011). However, a meta-analysis found no deficits on a composite of inhibition and shifting tasks for chronic opioid users (Baldacchino et al., 2012), only a very small effect size for MDMA use (EF composite d = 0.25, Zakzanis et al., 2007). There is evidence that EF deficits persist in medium term abstinence for cocaine (EF composite d = 0.32, WM composite d =0.52; Potvin et al., 2014) and alcohol dependence (EF composite d = 0.57, WM composite d = 0.49; Stavro et al., 2012). These meta-analytic reviews on EF impairments in substance use are particularly difficult to interpret. First, the inclusion of poly-substance users makes effects of individual drugs of abuse difficult to isolate. Second, given the neurotoxic effects of alcohol and many drugs of abuse, it is unclear to what extent EF deficits are a cause or consequence of substance use. A recent review of the limited number of longitudinal studies of adolescent heavy drinkers suggests that poor EF is likely both a risk factor and consequence of heavy drinking in adolescents (Peeters et al., 2014). There is also some longitudinal evidence that poor EF may be a risk factor for other substance use disorders (e.g., Nigg et al., 2006), but much more research is needed on this topic.

Complex Tasks: Verbal Fluency and Planning

Many complex tasks may tap multiple aspects of EF. For example, verbal fluency tasks (generating words starting with a certain letter or from a category) likely tap several cognitive processes (e.g., Rende et al., 2002). Planning tasks are also complex, involving multiple cognitive demands (Goel and Grafman, 1995), and so may not represent a single EF ability. This is problematic if the goal is to understand which specific EF processes are impaired, an issue we return to in the Methodological Issues section. These tasks have nonetheless been frequently used in studies of EF in clinical populations.

Deficits in verbal fluency are widespread across disorders. Indeed, meta-analyses show that out of all the EF tasks included in meta-analyses, the largest deficit for adults with schizophrenia and depression is found on the semantic verbal fluency task, with large and medium effect sizes respectively (Table 2). Semantic verbal fluency is also impaired in individuals with BD (medium effect sizes), OCD (small effect sizes), and ADHD (small effect sizes), while there is inconsistent evidence for verbal fluency in individuals with PTSD (Aupperle et al., 2012). For schizophrenia, BD, and MDD, effect sizes for phonemic verbal fluency are somewhat smaller than those for semantic, although still significant. In contrast, individuals with OCD have equal impairments in the two forms of verbal fluency, and verbal fluency deficits associated with ADHD appear to be larger for phonemic verbal fluency than semantic verbal fluency. There has been little research on verbal fluency in anxiety disorders: one study reported impaired phonemic verbal fluency in individuals with panic disorder (Gladsjo et al., 1998), while others found no impairment in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder (Airaksinen et al., 2005) or social anxiety disorder (Hood et al., 2010). However, conclusions are premature given the paucity of evidence.

Why might semantic and phonemic verbal fluency tasks be differentially affected in different disorders? Verbal fluency tasks impose multiple EF demands (e.g., shifting among subcategories, monitoring for repeated words, memory retrieval). One possibility as to why semantic verbal fluency is more impaired than phonemic verbal fluency in individuals with schizophrenia, BD, and depression is that it may place heavier demands on shifting, and particularly on switching between subcategories in a self-directed manner (Snyder and Munakata, 2010, 2013). For example, an individual who has difficulty switching between subcategories might name five farm animals when naming animals, and then fruitlessly continue to try to think of additional farm animals rather than switching to pets or zoo animals. Another possibility is that deficits in semantic memory retrieval may contribute to semantic verbal fluency impairment, particularly in individuals with schizophrenia. For example, a meta-analysis found that individuals with schizophrenia have large deficits on semantic verbal fluency both for switching between subcategories, an index of EF, and semantic clustering, an index of semantic memory (Doughty and Done, 2009). In contrast, the larger effect for phonemic verbal fluency in individuals with ADHD could potentially be due to deficits in phonological processing in many individuals with ADHD, since ADHD and reading disabilities frequently co-occur (Willcutt et al., 2007). Thus, deficits in verbal fluency may arise from a variety of sources, and illustrate the difficulty of interpreting results from complex tasks.

Planning has been much less studied than verbal fluency. Individuals with depression and BD have significant impairments in planning (Table 2). In individuals with ADHD, two meta-analyses found quite different effect sizes for planning tasks, one small, one medium. Likewise, meta-analyses of individuals with OCD found different effect sizes for planning, two small and one medium (Table 2). Finally, there is inconsistent evidence for planning deficits associated with PTSD (Aupperle et al., 2012). Thus, while planning tasks in theory tap multiple aspects of EF, standard measures of planning may be less sensitive than other EF tasks in detecting more subtle EF deficits associated with some disorders.

Summary of Previous Findings

In sum, it is clear that the preponderance of evidence shows that deficits on a wide variety of EF tasks are associated, at least concurrently, with many prevalent psychopathologies. Most disorders are associated with fairly uniform deficits EF tasks, although there are some notable variations in effect sizes (e.g., larger deficits for updating than other aspects of EF in OCD, and larger deficits for motor response inhibition than Stroop for ADHD). Thus, the results appear to be consistent with broad, and transdiagnostic, impairment in EF. The exception is simple verbal WM maintenance, which shows smaller, and in for some disorders non-significant, deficits. The finding that manipulation is more impaired than maintenance, along with evidence that visuospatial and verbal WM manipulation are equally impaired, both support the view that WM deficits in these disorders are due to impairment in the central executive aspect of WM, rather than the content-specific maintenance systems (Barch, 2005), again consistent with the view that there are broad impairments in EF associated with psychopathology, rather than impairments in a few individual specific aspects of EF.

Given the wealth of evidence already collected, at this point and with the current state of knowledge, the field generally does not need more cross-sectional, case-control designs comparing a group with one specific disorder to healthy controls on individual standard neuropsychological EF tasks. Such studies only address the question of whether there is a difference in EF task performance between groups, and that question has been satisfactorily answered in the affirmative, at least for disorders and tasks reviewed here (with the exception of the less-studied anxiety disorders). Rather, there is now the opportunity to build on the foundation of such previous studies to better understand the specific mechanisms and causal processes contributing to EF deficits in psychopathology, and to move toward translational applications.

Limitation of Previous Research and Suggestions for Future Research

Executive function is a challenging topic to study – it is both elusive to define (e.g., Jurado and Rosselli, 2007) and difficult to measure. Critically, interpretation of both the primary literature and meta-analyses is limited because tasks classified as tapping a particular aspect of EF may not be sensitive measures of that process, and are not pure measures of that EF process, since they often require other EF processes as well as multiple non-EF processes. Here we outline limitations in how EF has been defined, conceptualized and measured in previous research with clinical populations, and we present concrete suggestions for addressing these limitations in future research. This selective review is intended to survey the fundamentals of current models of EF, and best practices for assessing EF, for a clinical scientific audience; reviews and resources on specific topics are referenced throughout for those desiring more in-depth information on these topics.

Conceptual Issues: Models of EF

Many previous studies of EF in clinical populations have either treated EF as unitary, or conversely as a long list of separate, specific abilities. The first of these approaches over-lumps diverse tasks into a single construct, for example drawing conclusions about EF in general on the basis of single tasks, which differ from study to study. The second approach over-splits, treating a laundry list of tasks, such as decision making, planning and verbal fluency tasks, as if they were assessing separate abilities rather than a common set of component processes that support completion of these more complex tasks.

Rather than showing only “unity” or “diversity” as these two approaches imply, the best current evidence indicates that individual differences in EFs show both unity and diversity, an idea originally proposed by Teuber (1972). That is, different components of EF correlate with one another, thus tapping some common underlying ability (unity), but they also show some separability (diversity). This general structure of both common and specific elements of EF is shared by several different models of EF (e.g., Duncan et al., 1997; Baddeley and Repovs, 2006), although different models have focused on different components of EF, and different levels of analysis (e.g., behavioral vs. neural). At the behavioral level of analysis, different prominent models have focused on partly overlapping sets of EF components. For example, Baddeley (1996, 2012) has proposed a central executive system containing subsystems for coordinating performance between tasks, focusing/resolving interference from distractors, switching between tasks, and interfacing with long term memory, while Diamond (2013) has proposed inhibition, WM and cognitive flexibility as core aspects of EF. Others have proposed two-factor models of EF, including top–down modulation of lower-level processes and monitoring processes (Shallice, 2002) or maintenance of task goals in WM and resolution of response competition (Engle and Kane, 2003). At the neural level, models have proposed that distinct but interconnected prefrontal regions support functions such as setting task goals, initiating responses and monitoring performance (Stuss and Alexander, 2007; Stuss, 2011), or using task goals to modulate lower-level processes, resolving competition and evaluating responses (Banich, 2009). It is important to note that while these different models differ in some important ways, they also have many points of convergence, and in many cases largely agree on the core cognitive and neural mechanisms involved in EF despite frequently employing different terms for those processes. These different models and components of EF can all be important to consider in addressing different research questions.

Here we focus on one such model, the unity/diversity model (Miyake et al., 2000; Friedman et al., 2008; Miyake and Friedman, 2012), because it captures several features of what we believe to be the key components of EF, is practical to use for understanding EF at the behavioral level (e.g., as opposed to models at the neural level which require neuroimaging evidence), and has the potential to shed light on commonalities and differences in EF impairments across clinical populations by differentiating common and specific components of EF. The unity/diversity model focuses on three aspects of EF: (i) updating WM, (ii) shifting, and (iii) inhibition, as well as a common EF ability which spans these components. There are substantial but far from perfect (i.e., 1.0) correlations among shifting, updating and inhibition factors (Friedman et al., 2011), illustrated here in a large sample of 17 years old twins (Figure 1A). This general unity/diversity pattern has been consistently found in other samples, including children (e.g., Lehto et al., 2003; Rose et al., 2011), young adults (Miyake et al., 2000), and older adults (e.g., Vaughan and Giovanello, 2010), although only a single unitary factor (e.g., Wiebe et al., 2008, 2011) or two factors (Miller et al., 2012a) may be evident in preschool children. Each EF ability (e.g., updating) can be decomposed into what is common across all three EFs, or unity (common EF), and what is unique to that particular ability, or diversity (e.g., updating-specific ability; Figure 1B).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Unity/diversity model of EF. Two complimentary ways of representing the unity/diversity model (Adapted from Friedman et al., 2011). In both latent variable models, individual tasks are combined to form latent factors. Numbers on arrows are standardized factor loadings (range 1 to -1), that indicate the extent to which each task is predicted by the latent factor. Those on curved double-headed arrows are correlations between the latent variables, which indicate how strongly they are related. (A) The updating, shifting, and inhibition components are substantially correlated (unity), but are separable (i.e., not correlated 1.0; diversity). (B) Unity and diversity are more clearly shown with a bifactor model. All nine tasks load onto a common EF factor (unity), and updating and shifting tasks also load onto their respective specific factors (diversity). Note that there is no inhibition-specific factor (i.e., inhibition task variance is fully accounted for by common EF).

Thus, in order to specify the cognitive and biological underpinnings of EF, the unity/diversity framework suggests it is necessary to decompose task performance into common (common EF) and specific (updating- specific, and shifting-specific) abilities that may more cleanly map onto the underlying cognitive processes. This approach is relatively new, but has already produced some important discoveries. First, after accounting for common EF, there is no unique variance left for inhibition (i.e., no inhibition-specific factor), a finding that has been replicated in two independent samples (Friedman et al., 2008, 2011) – that is, individual differences in common EF fully account for individual differences in inhibition (Figure 1B). This finding is consistent with the view that the ability thought to be captured by common EF-actively maintaining task goals and goal-related information and using this information to effectively bias lower-level processing – is the key EF requirement of response inhibition (whereas stopping itself may be relatively automatic; (e.g., Munakata et al., 2011; Chatham et al., 2012), but see (e.g., Aron, 2007; Diamond, 2013) for alternative views of inhibition).

Second, common EF and shifting-specific components sometimes show opposing patterns of correlations with other measures, consistent with hypothesized trade-offs between stability (common EF) and flexibility (shifting-specific) suggested in the literature (e.g., Goschke, 2000). For example, rumination is associated with better performance on an EF task requiring goal maintenance (stability) but worse performance on an EF task requiring rapid shifting (flexibility; Altamirano et al., 2010), and young children who show good self-restraint (not reaching for an attractive toy they have been told not to touch) go on to have higher levels of common EF (stability) but lower levels of shifting-specific EF (flexibility) as adolescents (Friedman et al., 2011). Critically, these findings suggest that specific deficits in stability or flexibility will only be apparent when performance on shifting tasks is decomposed into common EF and shifting-specific factors.

Most importantly for clinical research, the different components of EF identified by the unity/diversity model differentially predict individual differences in clinically important behaviors (Friedman et al., 2007, 2011; Young et al., 2009). In particular, recent evidence points to common EF as the primary source of such predictive power. For example, poor common EF is associated with behavioral disinhibition, a general vulnerability factor hypothesized to underlie externalizing behavior problems, such as ADHD, conduct disorder, substance use, and novelty seeking/risk taking (Young et al., 2009). The similarity of effect sizes across the core EF domains in other disorders (Table 1) suggests that psychopathology more broadly may be associated with impairment in common EF. While this possibility has not been formally tested for most disorders, it suggests that decomposing EF into its common and specific components may have important implications for understanding EF deficits associated with psychopathology, an issue we return to in Future Directions.

Methodological Issues

Multiple Measures

Arguably the most vexing problem in effectively and precisely measuring EF is the task-impurity problem (Figure 2). Because any target EF must be embedded within a specific task context (so that the target EF has something to operate on), all EF tasks necessarily include systematic variance attributable to non-EF processes associated with that specific task context (e.g., color processing and articulation speed in the Stroop task, visuospatial processing in a spatial n-back task; e.g., Miyake et al., 2000). Unfortunately, this systematic non-EF variance and measurement error (random noise in the data) are substantial, making it difficult to cleanly measure the EF variance of interest (Figure 2). In addition, even targeted EF tasks tap both specific and common aspects of EF (e.g., common EF plus updating-specific for updating tasks). Because most studies of EF in clinical populations have used only a single task to assess EF processes of interest, results reported in this literature are nearly always a mixture of non-EF, common EF, and specific EF component effects, making interpretation of the results difficult. For example, poor performance on a spatial n-back task could arise from impaired common EF, updating-specific EF, or non-EF spatial processing problems.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Task impurity problem. A score on an EF task is composed of (A) systematic variance attributable to the specific aspect of EF targeted by that task (e.g., shifting-specific or updating-specific variance), (B) systematic variance attributable to common EF (i.e., variance shared across multiple types of EF tasks, hypothesized to be related to task-goal maintenance), (C) systematic variance attributable to non-EF aspects of the task (e.g., articulation speed, visual processing), and (D) non-systematic (error) variance. Use of single tasks to measure EF is thus problematic because this task-impurity makes interpreting the results difficult and because the amount of variance attributable to EF (A,B) can be relatively small compared to non-EF variance (C,D).

This task impurity problem can be alleviated by using multiple measures of each EF component under investigation. In this approach, multiple exemplar tasks are chosen that capture the target ability (e.g., three tasks that require updating WM; Miyake et al., 2000; Friedman et al., 2008) but seem different on the surface (e.g., the nature of the information to be updated is different in each). If exemplar tasks are chosen such that they share little systematic non-EF variance, one can statistically extract what is common across those tasks and use the resulting “purer” latent variable as the measure of EF. We thus suggest that whenever possible, researchers administer multiple tasks that target the specific aspect of EF they have hypotheses about, while also including some additional tasks to demonstrate the specificity of effects (c.f. Goschke, 2014). If researchers are interested in measuring Common EF, measures of each EF component (shifting, updating, and inhibition) should be used, and then aggregated.

Several methods are available for combining data from multiple measures, depending on the sample size of the study. The simplest and most versatile approach is to calculate a z-mean across tasks, which can be done with any sample size. When z-means across tasks are used in place of individual tasks, variance in the scores not related to the construct of interest (e.g., updating) can no longer drive the effects, so long as it is not systematic across the averaged tasks (e.g., the updating tasks do not share other task requirements in common). The disadvantage of this approach is that it merely combines scores – the error variance is not removed, and can still be a source of reduced power. For this reason, if sample size is large enough, it is preferable to use latent variable approaches for extracting only the variance shared across tasks while removing the error variance, using such multivariate statistical techniques as confirmatory factor analysis and structural equation modeling. While this approach may not be practical when there are a limited number of participants available for study (e.g., for disorders with low prevalence rates) or strict limitations on the amount of time available for testing, it has great potential for testing theories of relations between specific aspects of EF and psychopathology, as discussed further below. If time limitations only permit collecting data on a single task, researchers should be aware of the inherent limitations this imposes on the conclusions that can be drawn given the task impurity problem, and thus suitably cautious in interpreting the results.

Task Selection

In addition to using multiple tasks, it is essential to pick tasks carefully. Many studies of EF in clinical populations currently use traditional neuropsychological EF measures that tap multiple aspects of EF as well as non-EF abilities. These tasks may be useful for screening individuals for severe EF deficits, however, they are too broad to answer fine-grained questions about specific aspects of EF that may be implicated in psychopathology. As discussed above, complex neuropsychological tests tap a wide variety of cognitive processes, including not only multiple aspects of EF, but also non-executive abilities, making such measures difficult to interpret. For example, multiple disorders are associated with impairments in verbal fluency, but it is unknown if those impairments arise from deficits in shifting, WM, or non-EF aspects of the tasks, or some mixture of these factors, and if this differs among disorders. These concerns can be addressed by using tasks designed to more specifically place demands on individual aspects of EF (e.g., Miyake et al., 2000; Aron, 2008; Goschke, 2014). These tasks need not necessarily entirely replace traditional neuropsychological tasks, but if they are used it is important to also include more specific tasks in order to identify what specific processes account for impairment on the broader neuropsychological tasks.

In addition, in place of EF tasks, many studies in the clinical literature have used questionnaires to assess self or other (e.g., parent, teacher) report of behaviors putatively related to EF (e.g., Behavioral Rating Inventory for EF, measures of effortful control temperament). However, these questionnaires correlate relatively poorly with task-based measures of EF (Toplak et al., 2012), and thus should not be assumed to be measuring the same constructs. Questionnaire-based measures ask about behavior in complex real-world situations (e.g., completing tasks on time, staying organized). This has advantages in terms of ecological validity, and some have argued in favor of using questionnaires rather than EF tasks (e.g., Barkley and Fischer, 2011). However, questionnaire measures pose interpretational problems even greater than those posed by complex tasks like verbal fluency. That is, these real-world behaviors involve multiple executive and non-executive processes, and can also be heavily influenced by contextual factors – for example, responses to a question about completing homework on time may depend not only on various aspects of EF, but also motivation to do well in school, and whether there is a quiet place to work at home away from distractions, among other factors. Thus, specific questions about EF impairments are best addressed using targeted EF tasks. Questionnaire measures may be valuable to include as a measure of real-world behavior, but should not be interpreted as necessarily reflecting EF per se.

In addition to selecting more targeted EF tasks, it is important to consider the sensitivity and reliability of EF tasks. Tasks should be selected that are sensitive to the magnitude of deficits expected for the sample being tested. Many traditional neuropsychological tests (e.g., Trail Making Test, WCST) were originally designed to assess EF deficits in patients with frontal lobe damage or dementia. These tasks may not be sufficiently demanding to be sensitive to more subtle deficits in EF associated with psychopathology (i.e., they may have ceiling effects, with all participants performing well).

The reliability of tasks is also an important issue. Tasks with low reliability necessarily have poor correlations with other measures (e.g., measures of psychopathology). Unfortunately, complex EF tasks tend to have relatively low internal and/or test–retest reliability, potentially because people adopt different strategies at different times when completing the tasks (Miyake et al., 2000). It is also important to note that reliability is sample specific – for example, reliability may be high in severely impaired individuals (e.g., they reliably fail to maintain more than a few items in a WM task) but reliability may be lower in a less impaired sample (e.g., they start out doing poorly but then are able to improve their performance by using a more effective rehearsal strategy).

It is thus important to select the most reliable tasks available, determine the reliability of tasks within the population of interest (e.g., specific diagnostic group, healthy control group, age group, etc.), and plan sample sizes accordingly to achieve adequate power to detect the expected effect sizes given the reliability of the measures. Problems with task sensitivity and reliability are problematic because they may lead to false negative findings that either result in the study not being published (the file drawer problem), or being published with the erroneous conclusion that EF is not impaired in the clinical group (lower effect sizes, and thus problematic for accurate reviews via meta analysis). Indeed, many studies in the clinical literature have argued against the existence of EF deficits in particular populations based on null results that may have arisen from power limitations due to poor task sensitivity and reliability and/or small sample size, rather than reflecting a true lack of impairment.

The cognitive psychology and cognitive neuroscience literatures contain a rich source of targeted and sensitive paradigms, and these can easily be used for clinical studies as well. For example Miyake et al. (2000), Friedman and Miyake (2004) and Friedman et al. (2008) have developed and adapted multiple tasks to assess inhibition, shifting, and updating. Additional more specific EF tasks are listed in the fourth column of Table 1. Thus, once researchers determine what aspects of EF they wish to investigate, it should not be too difficult to find established tasks offering much more specificity and construct validity than the traditional, less specific neuropsychological measures frequently used in clinical studies.

There are also a number of commercially available task batteries that include tasks assessing EF (e.g., the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery, CANTAB), as well as some freely available task batteries (e.g., the NIH Toolbox). These batteries have both advantages and disadvantages. On the plus side, these batteries often have more extensive psychometric evaluations and norms, and their standardization allows for clear comparison across studies. However, these batteries generally do not provide comprehensive coverage of different components of EF aligned with current models (e.g., CANTAB has visuospatial WM, planning and shifting tasks, but no tasks assessing other aspects of EF), and do not generally provide multiple measures of each construct needed for latent variable approaches. Moreover, since these test batteries have been heavily used in most clinical populations, further case-control studies with these tasks are unlikely to yield new insights. These advantages and disadvantages should be carefully considered when deciding whether to use a pre-packaged task battery vs. selecting EF tasks from the cognitive psychology/neuroscience literature.

Other Methodological Considerations

Of almost equal importance to what tasks are used to evaluate EF is how the data from those tasks are collected and analyzed. When the total individual variance in EF task performance is broken into EF, task-specific and error components, the “noise” of non-EF task-specific variance and error variance can be quite large, while the “signal” of EF-specific variance may be quite small (Figure 2). Thus, in order to detect the signal that is of central importance for scientific inquiry, it is critically important to minimize error variance and maximize power. First, there is a strong need to increase sample size to improve power. Many previous studies have been underpowered, which likely leads to a file drawer problem and lack of replicability (e.g., Pashler and Wagenmakers, 2012), and problematically also leads to potentially erroneous claims that there is no EF impairment (e.g., that anxiety does not impair updating, (Eysenck et al., 2007). Second, once the data are collected, the reliability and validity of the measures depend critically on how they are screened and analyzed. For any given task, it is important to use the most specific, sensitive, and reliable measure of task performance. For the suggested more specific EF tasks in Table 1, the citation for each task provides a description of how to calculate measures of task performance. It is also important to screen for and appropriately address the presence of outliers, both outlier trials for each participant and outlier participants. Such outliers contribute to error variance and distort results, potentially leading to either false negatives or false positives. Taking these steps to collect and derive the highest quality EF measures possible maximizes the chances of detecting EF deficits and producing valid, interpretable results in clinical science.

Future Directions

Thus far we have reviewed evidence that multiple forms of psychopathology are associated with impairment on multiple measures of EF, and discussed what we see as the key conceptual and methodological limitations to this previous research. Namely, many previous studies of EF in clinical populations have either treated EF as unitary, or conversely as a long list of separate, specific abilities, counter to the best current evidence indicating that individual differences in EFs show both unity and diversity. (Here we focus on one such model, the unity/diversity model Miyake et al., 2000; Friedman et al., 2008; Miyake and Friedman, 2012), which we believe may be a particularly useful framework for clinical research, however, the same points largely apply to other models of EF.) In order to apply these current, best supported models of EF to clinical research, it will also be necessary to address a number of methodological limitations of previous research, by using multiple, specific, sensitive, and appropriately analyzed measures of different components of EF. Moreover, the vast majority of previous research has taken the form of cross-sectional case-control studies in adults, which are unable to differentiate between different possible causal links between EF and psychopathology (e.g., cause, consequence, or correlate).

Given these limitations to previous research and the goal of understanding links between EF and psychopathology at a level of detail and specificity that can support translational research, we propose two broad directions for future research. First, we suggest that the problem of understanding the seemingly undifferentiated nature of EF impairments across disorders may be made more tractable by testing models that include both unity and diversity, in both psychopathology and EF. Second, we suggest that research will need to move beyond cross-sectional case-control designs to test different possible causal links between EF and psychopathology.

Testing Models of Unity/Diversity Across Both EF and Psychopathology

What gives rise to broad patterns of impairment in EF across most disorders? First, these deficits cannot be easily explained by non-specific factors such as psychomotor slowing, differences in IQ or education, or medication use (e.g., Barch, 2005; Forbes et al., 2009; Snyder, 2013; Snyder et al., 2015). Second, in most cases, effect sizes are similar across the core EF domains. This pattern of broad impairment across most EF tasks found by meta-analyses is consistent with the theory that individuals with multiple forms of psychopathology have impairments in the unitary component of EF (i.e., common EF), posited to be the ability to actively maintain task goals and use this information to provide top–down support for task-relevant responses (Friedman et al., 2008; Miyake and Friedman, 2012).

We view this theory as fully compatible with others who have posited impairment in “executive attention” associated with psychopathology (e.g., Gooding et al., 2006; Pacheco-Unguetti et al., 2010; Orellana et al., 2012; Maurage et al., 2014). First, conceptually, in the dominant model of attention, the executive attention network is defined as similarly to common EF, as involving task set maintenance to provide top–down control supporting resolution of competition between response options (Posner and Petersen, 1990; Petersen and Posner, 2012). Second, empirically, this executive attention process is predominantly assessed with the flanker interference component of the Attentional Networks Task (ANT, Fan et al., 2002), and flanker task interference is strongly correlated with prepotent response inhibition at the latent level (Friedman and Miyake, 2004), which in turn is fully accounted for by common EF (e.g., Friedman et al., 2008). Finally, other attentional processes (alerting and orienting) appear to be largely unimpaired in individuals with psychopathology e.g., Gooding et al., 2006; Pacheco-Unguetti et al., 2010; Orellana et al., 2012; Maurage et al., 2014), suggesting that deficits are not due to lower-level attentional difficulties. Thus, we argue that the finding of impairments on the executive attention component of the ANT is fully compatible with impairment in Common EF. Although other explanations are also possible (e.g., multiple specific aspects of EF could be independently impaired), impairment in common EF is the most parsimonious interpretation.

Importantly, psychopathology has also been shown to consist of both common and specific factors. Specifically, latent variable models of psychopathology in both adolescents and adults find that there is a common factor that spans all aspects of common psychopathologies, in addition to factors for more specific aspects of psychopathology (internalizing and externalizing; e.g., Lahey et al., 2012; Tackett et al., 2013; Caspi et al., 2014). This general psychopathology factor, recently dubbed the “p Factor,” is related to broad negative emotionality (neuroticism) and associated with low conscientiousness and agreeableness, and more life impairment (Caspi et al., 2014).

This raises the possibility that broad, transdiagnostic impairments in EF might be explained by a link between this p Factor and common EF. Indeed, the p Factor has been shown to be associated with poorer performance on cognitive tasks including EF tasks, indicators of poor cerebrovascular functioning, and self-reported cognitive and self-control problems, assessed as early as 3 years of age (Caspi et al., 2014). These findings suggest that early neurological and cognitive problems may be a general liability factor for psychopathology. However, the nature of these cognitive problems has not yet been conclusively tested. Future research could test the hypothesis that these seemingly broad cognitive problems associated with common psychopathology are best explained as a deficit in common EF.

It is also possible that individuals with psychopathology have processing-specific impairments in shifting and/or updating (recall that there is no inhibition-specific component, e.g., Friedman et al., 2008) in addition to deficits in common EF, which could either be associated with common psychopathology (p Factor), or more specific aspects of psychopathology (e.g., depression, OCD, ADHD, etc.). Examining links between both common and specific aspects of EF and psychopathology has the potential to greatly clarify the nature of EF impairments associated with particular forms of psychopathology, and thus accelerate progress in understanding how EF impairments may contribute to both comorbidity across disorders and heterogeneity within disorders (e.g., anhedonia vs. broad negative affect in depression, anxious arousal vs. anxious apprehension in anxiety disorders, etc.).

While examining common EF as a potential transdiagnositic risk factor for common psychopathology is a highly promising direction for future research, it is important to bear in mind that cognitive factors that appear transdiagnostic at one level of analysis may not be when more detailed measures at multiple levels of analysis are considered. Just as many problems with a car (e.g., a dead battery, broken starter, or being out of gas) could all lead to the same outcome (the car won’t start), the same cognitive endpoint might be reached by many different underlying mechanisms (equifinality). Thus, while EF deficits appear to be a transdiagnostic feature of psychopathology at the level of performance on neuropsychological tasks, in some cases these shared behavioral deficits may arise from distinct neural mechanisms (e.g., different perturbations in neurotransmitter systems, (e.g., Gigante et al., 2012; Luykx et al., 2012). Thus, determining whether a product or process is truly transdiagnostic requires escaping both diagnostic and methodological silos to consider underlying mechanisms at multiple levels of analysis.

Causal Models

Though there are notable exceptions, a general shortcoming of the broad field of cognitive risks in psychopathology across the lifespan is the frequent lack of consideration of possible models of how cognitive impairments and psychopathology may be causally related. Specifically, it is unknown if EF deficits (a) precede, and are a potential causal risk factor for, developing psychopathology, (b) follow, and are a consequence of psychopathology, (c) are a correlate of psychopathology without playing a causal role (e.g., both poor EF and psychopathology may be related to a third factor), or some combination of these models (e.g., transactional models; c.f. Goschke, 2014). It is important to note that these logical models are not mutually exclusive. Indeed, it is highly likely that different models will hold true for different forms of psychopathology or aspects of EF, or even for the same disorder and cognitive process at different times or for different individuals.

While many studies explicitly or implicitly assume a particular causal model, there have been far fewer attempts to try to rule out, or in, particular models based on the evidence. Critically, cross-sectional case-control studies are not capable of differentiating between these possible models. While there is still a place for cross-sectional research in clarifying the nature of EF deficits in different clinical populations (e.g., using latent variable models to examine common vs. specific deficits), an important next step will be to build on these cross-sectional results with longitudinal, neural, and behavior genetic studies that can be informative in testing putative causal models.

For example, a small number of prospective longitudinal studies have been conducted, and suggest that impairments in EF and related brain systems predict later psychosis, ADHD, and PTSD, suggesting that cognitive deficits may be a risk factor for many disorders (Cannon et al., 2006; Parslow and Jorm, 2007; Campbell and Stauffenberg, 2008). Moreover, there is some evidence that EF deficits are primarily state-independent (manifest even when illness symptoms are not present, (e.g., Kurtz and Gerraty, 2009; Snyder, 2013) and present in attenuated form in unaffected family members of individuals with schizophrenia, BD, OCD, and PTSD, suggesting EF deficits may be an endophenotype for many forms of psychopathology (e.g., Barch, 2005; Gilbertson et al., 2006; Menzies et al., 2007; Bora et al., 2009). While these data showing associations between premorbid EF or genetic factors and EF impairments associated with psychopathology are consistent with causal risk factor or endophenotype models, in other cases there is evidence supporting the consequence model. For example, a meta-analysis found progressive loss of gray matter in the PFC and temporal lobe in individuals with schizophrenia, especially during the first episode, suggesting that the onset of schizophrenia triggers a neurodegenerative process that could impair EF (Vita et al., 2012).

In sum, the causal links between EF and psychopathology have not been well established, and the cascade of mechanisms connecting EF to psychopathology are unknown and in need of theoretical and empirical investigation. Besides being of importance for basic research, these questions have important implications for prevention and treatment. For example, if EF deficits are a risk factor for psychopathology, individuals who are vulnerable to, but have not yet developed, psychopathology (e.g., due to parental history) might benefit from early intervention to teach compensatory strategies to mitigate the effects of EF impairments, a topic we expand on next.

Treatment Implications

Better understanding EF deficits associated with psychopathology has important implications for evidence-based assessment and intervention advancement, including enhancing screening, prevention, and treatment and better understanding treatment mechanisms. In terms of prevention and treatment approaches, current evidence suggests that approaches aimed at teaching compensatory strategies may be the most promising direction for future translational research. Importantly, there is little evidence in support of direct training of EF (i.e., targeting the weakness rather than compensatory strategies). In general, the majority of studies have found that while task performance improves, there is little evidence that training effects generalize to real-world function or improve clinical symptoms (e.g., for review see Rabipour and Raz, 2012). That is, these interventions appear to improve the task-specific non-EF processes (Figure 2C) but not the EF per se (Figures 2A,B). One possible exception is EF training in children with ADHD, which some studies indicate training can improve performance on untrained EF tasks, and in some cases parent report of symptoms (Rabipour and Raz, 2012). However, a recent meta-analysis found that EF training did not reliably transfer to academic performance and blinded subjective ratings of children with ADHD (i.e., ratings by individuals who did not know about the training intervention), although there were very small but significant improvements on non-trained cognitive tasks (Rapport et al., 2013). Thus, the jury remains out on possible benefits of direct EF training, but the majority of existing evidence does not indicate effective transfer to improved daily functioning or symptom reduction.

It is not clear to what extent these findings reflect genuine limitations of cognitive training in general, vs. problems with the specific training programs (many of them commercial products). For example, many of the programs focus on training the least impaired aspect of EF in children with ADHD, simple WM maintenance (Rapport et al., 2013). This leaves open the possibility that types of training that better target areas of weakness might provide better transfer. Intriguing findings suggest that certain types of EF training may change the underlying neural mechanisms to be more efficient rather than changing strategy use only (Owens et al., 2013), suggesting that such training might transfer more broadly to processes involving the same neural mechanisms, although this has not yet been tested. As an alternative to training, there is evidence that directly manipulating prefrontal function through non-invasive brain stimulation techniques (repetitive transcranial stimulation and transcranial direct current stimulation) can produce short-term improvements in performance on EF tasks (Brunoni and Vanderhasselt, 2014). However, evidence for efficacy of these techniques in producing long-term improvements in cognition in clinical groups is currently promising but inconclusive (e.g., Demirtas-Tatlidede et al., 2013).

Given the current lack of evidence for effective transfer of EF training, treatment, and prevention programs involving compensatory strategies may be a more promising direction for translational research. For example, goal management techniques (e.g., Goal Management Training; (e.g., Cicerone et al., 2006) may help individuals compensate for poor Common EF by teaching them to break goals into manageable sub-goals and monitor their progress. While these types of compensatory training have most frequently been used with individuals who have sustained brain damage, there is emerging evidence that they may be helpful for individuals with psychopathology as well. Cognitive rehabilitation interventions aimed at teaching compensatory strategies (e.g., use of lists and cues, dividing tasks into smaller steps, etc.), have been shown to improve functional outcomes (e.g., occupational/academic functioning) in individuals with schizophrenia (for review see Kluwe-Schiavon et al., 2013), BD (e.g., Deckersbach et al., 2010), and ADHD (e.g., Hahn-Markowitz et al., 2011). There is less research on cognitive remediation in individuals with depression or anxiety disorders, although it is intriguingly suggestive that some therapies (e.g., behavioral activation; (e.g., Dimidjian et al., 2011) incorporate compensatory strategies (e.g., cues to engage in an activity, like putting walking shoes by the door).

In addition to augmenting treatment with compensatory strategy training, there may be a need to adapt and personalize current treatment approaches to match clients’ EF abilities. Better understanding the EF profile of each patient may be helpful in tailoring treatment approaches. There is some preliminary evidence that pre-treatment EF predicts treatment response to CBT (e.g., Mohlman and Gorman, 2005), potentially because EF is needed to engage effectively with many treatment and prevention strategies. For example, individuals in CBT are asked to do thought restructuring exercises, formulate and implement behavioral plans, and monitor their own cognition and behavior, all of which involve EF (e.g., Mohlman and Gorman, 2005). Thus, identifying specific aspects of EF associated with psychopathology will be critical for determining who is most likely to benefit from existing interventions and who needs adaptations to those interventions or new, and different, interventions, such as those for whom CBT (or other high EF interventions) may not be as efficacious (i.e., personalization of intervention, or what works for whom). In many cases, adapting current interventions to be more manageable for individuals with poor EF may simply be a matter of providing additional support and structure. For example, knowing that an individual has reduced ability to select among multiple competing options might suggest personalization by reducing the number of options offered in the course of therapy (e.g., have the depressed patient choose from only 2–3 behavioral activation options to improve mood instead of choosing from an overwhelming menu of 36 pleasant activities).

Executive function deficits also have important implications for psychopharmacological treatments. First, as for behavioral therapies, pre-treatment EF has been shown to predict drug treatment response. In particular, EF predicts pharmacotherapy response in individuals with depression (e.g., McLennan and Mathias, 2010), schizophrenia (e.g., Kim et al., 2008), OCD (e.g., D’Alcante et al., 2012), and BD (e.g., Gruber et al., 2008). Although the precise reasons are unclear (e.g., there could be neurobiological explanations), poor medication compliance is the most likely and parsimonious explanation because these findings hold across several different types of psychiatric medications. Thus, individuals with poor EF may benefit from additional supports for successful medication management (e.g., pill boxes that sound an alarm when it is time to take medication).

Second, better understanding pathophysiology of EF deficits associated with psychopathology may lead to improved targeting of drug treatments to enhance EF. Currently, there are few medications directly aimed at improving EF, with the exception of stimulant medications for ADHD. The majority, but not all, studies find that stimulant medications improve EF performance in individuals with ADHD (for review see Pietrzak et al., 2006). There are also interesting suggestions that certain medications (e.g., Modafinil, a cognitive enhancer) have potential for improving outcomes in individuals with depression [e.g., improved response to antidepressant treatment with Modafinil, (Abolfazli et al., 2011)], but these effects are not yet well established with EF (Murrough et al., 2011).

Better understanding the specific EF deficits associated with different forms of psychopathology could enhance targeting of medications that affect the neurotransmitter systems known to be involved in those EF processes. For example, GABA and glutamate have been implicated in specific EF processes (e.g., Krystal et al., 2001; Castner and Williams, 2007; Snyder et al., 2010; de la Vega et al., 2014), and these neurotransmitter systems are known to be affected in depression and anxiety disorders (for review see Möhler, 2012; Tokita et al., 2012). Already, promising GABA and glutamate medications exist, or are under development and testing, for anxiety and depression (Möhler, 2012; Krystal et al., 2014). Findings from studies of EF at multiple levels of measurement can inform continued drug development and personalization by identifying the current drugs that better target the most critical pathophysiological processes to maximize efficacy.

Finally, measuring EF over the course of treatment may help identify treatment mechanisms, which in turn can lead to refinements to treatment approaches to better target those mechanisms. For example, there is some emerging evidence that mindfulness interventions increase cognitive flexibility (for review see Chiesa et al., 2011), which may partly mediate positive effects of the intervention on some outcomes (Heeren et al., 2009). However, this hypothesis has not been tested with latent variable approaches that allow flexibility (i.e., shifting-specific EF) to be differentiated from common EF and non-EF aspects of the tasks, an important area for future research. There are many other potential mechanisms of action involving EF that are conceptually plausible but untested, for example, that some positive effects of behavioral activation approaches could be partially mediated by improved goal maintenance. Identifying such mechanisms of action has been noted as an important step in improving treatment efficacy and advancing evidenced-based psychological interventions (e.g., Kazdin, 2007; Emmelkamp et al., 2014).

Conclusion

In recent decades, a proliferation of research has investigated EF in clinical populations, and for good reason: individual differences in EF are associated with many important aspects of human health and functioning, including most forms of psychopathology. However, despite the strongly interdisciplinary nature of this topic, poised between clinical and cognitive science, these fields have followed largely independent paths. Here we have argued that it will be necessary to move past this model of ‘parallel play’ in order to push clinical psychological science forward toward a better understanding of how and why EF is so broadly compromised across mental health disorders. Specifically, we advocate for better assessment of EF using the best current, validated models of EF and best methods for assessing EF. Critically, interpretation of both the primary literature and meta-analyses to date is limited because typical methods of assessing EF in the clinical literature often lack specificity and sensitivity to the particular aspect of EF they are intended to measure.

To address these limitations, we provided recommendations for applying validated models of EF to clinical research, using multiple tasks to obtain purer measures of EF, and selecting and analyzing tasks in ways that minimize the inherent noisiness of EF data. Specifically, to address the task impurity problem and improve reliability, we recommend carefully choosing EF components to focus on, based on theory and/or past research, and using multiple measures of each EF component of interest and combining them using composite scores or latent variable analysis. When possible, investigating both common and specific components of EF and psychopathology using latent variable approaches holds great promise for making the problem of understanding the seemingly undifferentiated nature of EF impairments across disorders more tractable. We also urge researchers to consider using more specific EF measures (see Table 1), instead of or in addition to, traditional, but overly broad, neuropsychological tests. Given the inherent noisiness of even the best EF tasks, it is also critically important to ensure studies are adequately powered, calculate the most specific, sensitive and reliable measure possible from each task (recognizing that this may differ from what is typically reported for traditional neuropsychological tasks; see Table 1 for method citations), screen for outliers and trim data appropriately both within and across subjects. Taken together, we hope these suggestions for combining the best current theoretical and methodological advances of clinical and cognitive science can help to advance the field toward understanding the underlying mechanisms involved in EF impairments at a level that can enable translational research to improve treatment.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

Preparation of this manuscript was supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health (R01MH077195, R21MH102210, F32MH098481).

References

Abela, J. R. Z., and Hankin, B. L. (2011). Rumination as a vulnerability factor to depression during the transition from early to middle adolescence: a multiwave longitudinal study. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 120, 259–271. doi: 10.1037/a0022796

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abolfazli, R., Hosseini, M., Ghanizadeh, A., Ghaleiha, A., Tabrizi, M., and Akhondzadeh, S. (2011). Double-blind randomized parallel-group clinical trial of efficacy of the combination fluoxetine plus modafinil versus fluoxetine plus placebo in the treatment of major depression. Depress. Anxiety 28, 297–302. doi: 10.1002/da.20801

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abramovitch, A., Abramowitz, J. S., and Mittelman, A. (2013). The neuropsychology of adult obsessive–compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33, 1163–1171. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.09.004

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abwender, D. A., Swan, J. G., Bowerman, J. T., and Connolly, S. W. (2001). Qualitative analysis of verbal fluency output: review and comparison of several scoring methods. Assessment 8, 323–338. doi: 10.1177/107319110100800308

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Airaksinen, E., Larsson, M., and Forsell, Y. (2005). Neuropsychological functions in anxiety disorders in population-based samples: evidence of episodic memory dysfunction. J. Psychiatr. Res. 39, 207–214. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2004.06.001

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev 30, 217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alderson, R. M., Kasper, L. J., Hudec, K. L., and Patros, C. H. G. (2013). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and working memory in adults: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 27, 287–302. doi: 10.1037/a0032371

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alderson, R. M., Rapport, M. D., and Kofler, M. J. (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and behavioral inhibition: a meta-analytic review of the stop-signal paradigm. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 35, 745–758. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9131-6

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altamirano, L. J., Miyake, A., and Whitmer, A. J. (2010). When mental inflexibility facilitates executive control: beneficial side effects of ruminative tendencies on goal maintenance. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1377–1382. doi: 10.1177/0956797610381505

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andreotti, C., Thigpen, J. E., Dunn, M. J., Watson, K., Potts, J., and Compas, B. E. (2013). Cognitive reappraisal and secondary control coping: associations with working memory, positive and negative affect, and symptoms of anxiety/depression. Anxiety Stress Coping 26, 20–35. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2011.631526

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R. (2007). The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. Neuroscientist 13, 214–228. doi: 10.1177/1073858407299288

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R. (2008). Progress in executive-function research. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 17, 124–129. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00561.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arts, B. M. G., Jabben, N., Krabbendam, L., and van Os, J. (2008). Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives. Psychol. Med. 38, 771–785. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001675

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aupperle, R. L., Melrose, A. J., Stein, M. B., and Paulus, M. P. (2012). Executive function and PTSD: disengaging from trauma. J. Affect. Disord. 62, 686–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.02.008

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 1–29. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. 49A, 5–28. doi: 10.1080/713755608

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D., and Repovs, G. (2006). The multi-component model of working memory: explorations in experimental cognitive psychology. Neuroscience 139, 5–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.12.061

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldacchino, A., Balfour, D. J. K., Passetti, F., Humphris, G., and Matthews, K. (2012). Neuropsychological consequences of chronic opioid use: a quantitative review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 2056–2068. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.06.006

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bálint, S., Czobor, P., Komlósi, S., Mészáros,Á., Simon, V., and Bitter, I. (2009). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): gender- and age-related differences in neurocognition. Psychol. Med. 39, 1337–1345. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004236

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banich, M. T. (2009). Executive function: the search for an integrated account. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 18, 89–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01615.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barch, D. M. (2005). The cognitive neuroscience of schizophrenia. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 1, 321–353. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143959

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kransberg, M. J., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Bull. 133, 1–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol. Bull. 121, 65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A., and Fischer, M. (2011). Predicting impairment in major life activities and occupational functioning in hyperactive children as adults: self-reported executive function (EF) deficits versus EF tests. Dev. Neuropsychol. 36, 137–161. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549877

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berch, D. B., Krikorian, R., and Huha, E. M. (1998). The corsi block-tapping task: methodological and theoretical considerations. Brain Cogn. 38, 317–338. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1998.1039

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., and Jones, L. L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5: changes and correlates. Dev. Rev. 29, 180–200. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2009.05.002

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, S. J. (2008). Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 92–98. doi: 10.1038/nn.2242

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bokat, C. E., and Goldberg, T. E. (2003). Letter and category fluency in schizophrenic patients: a meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 64, 73–78. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00282-7

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boldrini, M., Del Pace, L., Placidi, G. P. A., Keilp, J., Ellis, S. P., and Cappa, S. F. (2005). Selective cognitive deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder compared to panic disorder with agoraphobia. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 111, 150–158. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2004.00247.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, M. A., Oosterlaan, J., Sergeant, J. A., and Buitelaar, J. K. (2005). Executive functioning in adult ADHD: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Med. 35, 1097–1108. doi: 10.1017/S003329170500499X

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bora, E., Yucel, M., and Pantelis, C. (2009). Cognitive endophenotypes of bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives. J. Affect. Disord. 113, 1–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.06.009

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bora, E., Yücel, M., Pantelis, C., and Berk, M. (2011). Meta-analytic review of neurocognition in bipolar II disorder. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 123, 165–174. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01638.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunoni, A. R., and Vanderhasselt, M.-A. (2014). Working memory improvement with non-invasive brain stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Cogn. 86, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.01.008

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckholtz, J. W., and Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2012). Psychopathology and the human connectome: toward a transdiagnostic model of risk for mental illness. Neuron 74, 990–1004. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.002

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, S. B., and Stauffenberg, C. (2008). Delay and inhibition as early predictors of ADHD symptoms in third grade. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 37, 1–15. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9270-4

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cannon, M., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Murray, R. M., Harrington, H., and Poulton, R. (2006). Neuropsychological performance at the age of 13 years and adult schizophreniform disorder: prospective birth cohort study. Br. J. Psychiatry 189, 463–464. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.020552

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-Mellor, S. J., Harrington, H., and Moffitt, T. E. (2014). The p factor: one general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2, 119–137. doi: 10.1177/2167702613497473

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castner, S. A., and Williams, G. V. (2007). Tuning the engine of cognition: a focus on NMDA/D1 receptor interactions in prefrontal cortex. Brain Cogn. 63, 94–122. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.11.002

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatham, C. H., Claus, E. D., Kim, A., Curran, T., Banich, M. T., and Munakata, Y. (2012). Cognitive control reflects context monitoring, not motoric stopping, in response inhibition. PLoS ONE 7:e31546. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031546

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiesa, A., Calati, R., and Serretti, A. (2011). Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 449–464. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.11.003

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christopher, G., and MacDonald, J. (2005). The impact of clinical depression on working memory. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 10, 379–399. doi: 10.1080/13546800444000128

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cicerone, K., Levin, H., Malec, J., and Stuss, D. (2006). Cognitive rehabilitation interventions for executive function: moving from bench to bedside in patients with traumatic brain injury. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1212–1222. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.7.1212

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cisler, J. M., Walitzky-Tayler, K. B., Adams, T. G., Babson, K. A., Badour, C. L., and Willems, J. L. (2011). The emotional Stroop task and posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev 31, 817–828. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.03.007

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, L. J., Hollander, E., DeCaria, C. M., Stein, D. J., Simeon, D., Liebowitz, M. R.,et al. (1996). Specificity of neuropsychological impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison with social phobic and normal control subjects. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 8, 82–85. doi: 10.1176/jnp.8.1.82

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, A. R., Kane, M., and Bunting, M. (2005). Working memory span tasks: a methodological review and user’s guide. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 769–786. doi: 10.3758/BF03196772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowe, S. F., Matthews, C., and Walkenhorst, E. (2007). Relationship between worry, anxiety and thought suppression and the components of working memory in a non-clinical sample. Aust. Psychol. 42, 170–177. doi: 10.1080/00050060601089462

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Alcante, C. C., Diniz, J. B., Fossaluza, V., Batistuzzo, M. C., Lopes, A. C., and Hoexter, M. Q. (2012). Neuropsychological predictors of response to randomized treatment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 39, 310–317. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.07.002

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Vega, A., Brown, M. S., Snyder, H. R., Singel, D., Munakata, Y., and Banich, M. T. (2014). Individual differences in the balance of GABA to glutamate in pFC predict the ability to select among competing options. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 2490–2502. doi: 10.1162/jocn-a-00655

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H. W., Goubert, L., Onraedt, T., Vanderhasselt, M.-A., and De Raedt, R. (2012). Cognitive control moderates the association between stress and rumination. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 43, 519–525. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.07.004

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Panfilis, C., Meehan, K. B., Cain, N. M., and Clarkin, J. F. (2013). The relationship between effortful control, current psychopathology and interpersonal difficulties in adulthood. Compr. Psychiatry 54, 454–461. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.12.015

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deckersbach, T., Nierenberg, A. A., Kessler, R., Lund, H. G., Ametrano, R. M., and Dougherty, D. (2010). Cognitive rehabilitation for bipolar disorder: an open trial for employed patients with residual depressive symptoms. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 16, 298–307. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-5949.2009.00110.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demeyer, I., De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H. W., and De Raedt, R. (2012). Rumination mediates the relationship between impaired cognitive control for emotional information and depressive symptoms: a prospective study in remitted depressed adults. Behav. Res. Ther. 50, 292–297. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.02.012

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demirtas-Tatlidede, A., Vahabzadeh-Hagh, A. M., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2013). Can noninvasive brain stimulation enhance cognition in neuropsychiatric disorders? Neuropharmacology 64, 566–578. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.06.020

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickinson, D., Ramsey, M. E., and Gold, J. M. (2007). Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64, 532–542. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.532

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dimidjian, S., Barrera, M., Martell, C., Muñoz, R. F., and Lewinsohn, P. M. (2011). The origins and current status of behavioral activation treatments for depression. Ann. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 7, 1–38. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104535

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doughty, O. J., and Done, D. J. (2009). Is semantic memory impaired in schizophrenia? A systematic review and meta-analysis of 91 studies. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 14, 473–509. doi: 10.1080/13546800903073291

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, J., Johnson, R., Swales, M., and Freer, C. (1997). Frontal lobe deficits after head injury: unity and diversity of function. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 14, 713–741. doi: 10.1080/026432997381420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emmelkamp, P. M. G., David, D., Beckers, T., Muris, P., Cuijpers, P., and Vervliet, B. (2014). Advancing psychotherapy and evidence-based psychological interventions. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 23, 58–91. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1411

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engle, R. W., and Kane, M. J. (2003). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 44, 145–199. doi: 10.1016/s0079-7421(03)44005-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ersche, K. D., Turton, A. J., Chamberlain, S. R., Muller, U., Bullmore, E. T., and Robbins, T. W. (2012). Cognitive dysfunction and anxious-impulsive personality traits are endophenotypes for drug dependence. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 926–936. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091421

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W., and Derakshan, N. (2011). New perspectives in attentional control theory. Pers. Individ. Dif. 50, 955–960. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.019

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, M., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., and Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion 7, 336–353. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falkowski, J., Atchison, T., DeButte-Smith, M., Weiner, M. F., and O’Bryant, S. (2014). Executive functioning and the metabolic syndrome: a project FRONTIER study. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 29, 47–53. doi: 10.1093/arclin/act078

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 340–347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez-Serrano, M. J., Pérez-García, M., and Verdejo-Garcéa, A. (2011). What are the specific vs. generalized effects of drugs of abuse on neuropsychological performance? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 377–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.008

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forbes, N. F., Carrick, L. A., McIntosh, A. M., and Lawrie, S. M. (2009). Working memory in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 39, 889–905. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708004558

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frazier, T. W., Demaree, H. A., and Youngstrom, E. A. (2004). Meta-analysis of intellectual and neuropsychological test performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology 18, 534–555. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.543.supp

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., Haberstick, B. C., Willcutt, E. G., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., Corley, R. P.,et al. (2007). Greater attention problems during childhood predict poorer executive functioning in late adolescence. Psychol. Sci. 18, 893–900. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01997.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., and Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. 133, 101–135. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Robinson, J. L., and Hewitt, J. K. (2011). Developmental trajectories in toddlers’ self-restraint predict individual differences in executive functions 14 years later: a behavioral genetic analysis. Dev. Psychol. 47, 1410–1430. doi: 10.1037/a0023750

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., and Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 137, 201–225. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gigante, A. D., Bond, D. J., Lafer, B., Lam, R. W., Young, L. T., and Yatham, L. N. (2012). Brain glutamate levels measured by magnetic resonance spectroscopy in patients with bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis. Bipolar Disord. 14, 478–487. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01033.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilbertson, M. W., Paulus, L. A., Williston, S. K., Gurvits, T. V., Lasko, N. B., Pitman, R. K.,et al. (2006). Neurocognitive function in monozygotic twins discordant for combat exposure: relationship to posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 115, 484–495. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.3.484

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gladsjo, J. A., Rapaport, M. H., McKinney, R., Lucas, J. A., Rabin, A., and Judd, L. L. (1998). A neuropsychological study of panic disorder: negative findings. J. Affect. Disord. 49, 123–131. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(98)00006-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goel, V., and Grafman, J. (1995). Are the frontal lobes implicated in “planning” functions? Interpreting data from the Tower of Hanoi. Neuropsychologia 33, 623–642. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)90866-P

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gooding, D. C., Braun, J. G., and Studer, J. A. (2006). Attentional network task performance in patients with schizophrenia–spectrum disorders: evidence of a specific deficit. Schizophr. Res. 88, 169–178. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.07.009

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goschke, T. (2000). “Involuntary persistence and intentional reconfiguration in task-set switching,” in Control of Cognitive Processes, eds S. Monsell and J. Driver (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 331–355.

Google Scholar

Goschke, T. (2014). Dysfunctions of decision-making and cognitive control as transdiagnostic mechanisms of mental disorders: advances, gaps, and needs in current research. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 23, 41–57. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1410

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruber, S. A., Rosso, I. M., and Yurgelun-Todd, D. (2008). Neuropsychological performance predicts clinical recovery in bipolar patients. J. Affect. Disord. 105, 253–260. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.04.014

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hahn-Markowitz, J., Manor, I., and Maeir, A. (2011). Effectiveness of cognitive-functional (Cog-Fun) intervention with children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a pilot study. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 65, 384–392. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2011.000901

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, P. A., Elias, L. J., and Crossley, M. (2006). Neurocognitive influences on health behavior in a community sample. Health Psychol. 25, 778–782. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.6.778

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heeren, A., De Raedt, R., Koster, E. H. W., and Philippot, P. (2013). The (neuro)cognitive mechanisms behind attention bias modification in anxiety: proposals based on theoretical accounts of attentional bias. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:119. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00119

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heeren, A., Van Broeck, N., and Philippot, P. (2009). The effects of mindfulness on executive processes and autobiographical memory specificity. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 403–409. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.01.017

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, J., and Crawford, J. (2005). A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency deficits in schizophrenia relative to other neurocognitive deficits. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 10, 1–33. doi: 10.1080/13546800344000309

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hervey, A. S., Epstein, J. N., and Curry, J. F. (2004). Neuropsychology of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 18, 485–503. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.485

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hood, S., Melichar, J., Taylor, L., Kalk, N., Edwards, T., and Nutt, D. J. (2010). Noradrenergic function in generalized anxiety disorder: impact of treatment with venlafaxine on the physiological and psychological responses to clonidine challenge. J. Psychopharmacol. 25, 78–86. doi: 10.1177/0269881109359099

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jurado, M. B., and Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol. Rev. 17, 213–233. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R. A., Miura, T. K., and Colflesh, G. J. H. (2007). Working memory, attention control, and the n-back task: a question of construct validity. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 33, 615–622. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.615

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kazdin, A. E. (2007). Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 3, 1–27. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, Y.-K., Lee, A.-R., Hur, J.-W., Yoon, H.-K., Lee, B.-H., and Ko, Y.-H. (2008). Cognitive factors for predicting treatment response in schizophrenic patients: one-year follow-up study. Psychiatry Invest. 5, 106–114. doi: 10.4306/pi.2008.5.2.106

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kluwe-Schiavon, B., Sanvicente-Vieira, B., Kristensen, C. H., and Grassi-Oliveira, R. (2013). Executive functions rehabilitation for schizophrenia: a critical systematic review. J. Psychiatr. Res. 47, 91–104. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.10.001

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krystal, J. H., Bennett, A., Abi-Saab, D., Belger, A., Karper, L. P., and Charney, D. S. (2001). Dissociation of ketamine effects on rule acquisition and rule implementation: possible relevance to NMDA receptor contributions to executive cognitive functions. Biol. Psychiatry 47, 137–143. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00097-9

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krystal, J. H., Sanacora, G., and Duman, R. S. (2014). Rapid-Acting glutamatergic antidepressants: the path to ketamine and beyond. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 1133–1141. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.03.026

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurtz, M. M., and Gerraty, R. T. (2009). A meta-analytic investigation of neurocognitive deficits in bipolar illness: profile and effects of clinical state. Neuropsychology 23, 551–562. doi: 10.1037/a0016277

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., Hakes, J. K., Zald, D. H., Hariri, A. R., and Rathouz, P. J. (2012). Is there a general factor of prevalent psychopathology during adulthood? J. Abnorm. Psychol. 121, 971–977. doi: 10.1037/a0028355

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lansbergen, M. M., Kenemans, J. L., and van Engeland, H. (2007). Stroop interference and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychology 21, 251–262. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.2.251

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehto, J. E., Juujarvi, P., Kooistra, L., and Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Dimensions of executive functioning: evidence from children. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 21, 59–80. doi: 10.1348/026151003321164627

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lijffijt, M., Kenemans, J. L., Verbaten, M. N., and van Engeland, H. (2005). A meta-analytic review of stopping performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: deficient inhibitory motor control? J. Abnorm. Psychology, 114, 216–222. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.216

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luykx, J. J., Laban, K. G., van den Heuvel, M. P., Boks, M. P. M., Mandl, R. C. W., Kahn, R. S.,et al. (2012). Region and state specific glutamate downregulation in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of 1H-MRS findings. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 198–205. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.05.014

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mann Wrobel, M. C., Carreno, J. T., and Dickinson, D. (2011). Meta-analysis of neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: an update and investigation of moderator variables. Bipolar Disord. 13, 334–342. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2011.00935.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mantella, R. C., Butters, M. A., Dew, M. A., Mulsant, B. H., Begley, A. E., and Lenze, E. J. (2007). Cognitive impairment in late-life generalized anxiety Disorder. Am. J. Geriat. Psychiatry 15, 673–679. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31803111f2

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maurage, P., de Timary, P., Billieux, J., Collignon, M., and Heeren, A. (2014). Attentional alterations in alcohol dependence are underpinned by specific executive control deficits. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 38, 2105–2112. doi: 10.1111/acer.12444

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLaughlin, K. A., and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Rumination as a transdiagnostic factor in depression and anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 49, 186–193. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.12.006

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLennan, S. N., and Mathias, J. L. (2010). The depression-executive dysfunction (DED) syndrome and response to antidepressants: a meta-analytic review. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 25, 933–944. doi: 10.1002/gps.2431

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McRae, K., Jacobs, S. E., Ray, R. D., John, O. P., and Gross, J. J. (2012). Individual differences in reappraisal ability: links to reappraisal frequency, well-being, and cognitive control. J. Res. Pers. 46, 2–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menzies, L., Achard, S., Chamberlain, S. R., Fineberg, N., Chen, C.-H., and Bullmore, E. (2007). Neurocognitive endophenotypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain 130, 3223–3236. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm205

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mesholam-Gately, R. I., Giuliano, A. J., Goff, K. P., Faraone, S. V., and Seidman, L. J. (2009). Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 23, 315–336. doi: 10.1037/a0014708

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, M. R., Giesbrecht, G. F., Muller, U., McInerney, R. J., and Kerns, K. A. (2012a). A latent variable approach to determining the structure of executive function in preschool children. J. Cogn. Dev. 13, 395–423. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2011.585478

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, M., Nevado-Montenegro, A. J., and Hinshaw, S. P. (2012b). childhood executive function continues to predict outcomes in young adult females with and without childhood-diagnosed ADHD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 40, 657–668. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9599-y

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., and Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Möhler, H. (2012). The GABA system in anxiety and depression and its therapeutic potential. Neuropharmacology 62, 42–53. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.08.040

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohlman, J., and Gorman, J. M. (2005). The role of executive functioning in CBT: a pilot study with anxious older adults. Behav. Res. Ther. 43, 447–465. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.03.007

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Munakata, Y., Herd, S. A., Chatham, C. H., Depue, B. E., Banich, M. T., and O’Reilly, R. C. (2011). A unified framework for inhibitory control. Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.) 15, 453–459. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.07.011

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murrough, J. W., Iacoviello, B., Neumeister, A., Charney, D. S., and Iosifescu, D. V. (2011). Cognitive dysfunction in depression: neurocircuitry and new therapeutic strategies. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 96, 553–563. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.06.006

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigg, J. T., Wong, M. M., Martel, M. M., Jester, J. M., Puttler, L. I., and Zucker, R. A. (2006). Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 45, 468–475. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000199028.76452.a9

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., and Watkins, E. R. (2011). A heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models of psychopathology: explaining multifinality and divergent trajectories. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 589–609. doi: 10.1177/1745691611419672

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogilvie, J. M., Stewart, A. L., Chan, R. C. K., and Shum, D. H. K. (2011). Neuropsychological measures of executive function and antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis. Criminol. 49, 1063–1107. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2011.00252.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orellana, G., Slachevsky, A., and Peña, M. (2012). Executive attention impairment in first-episode schizophrenia. BMC Psychiatry 12:154. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-154

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owens, M., Koster, E. H. W., and Derakshan, N. (2013). Improving attention control in dysphoria through cognitive training: transfer effects on working memory capacity and filtering efficiency. Psychophysiology 50, 297–307. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12010

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pacheco-Unguetti, A. P., Acosta, A., Callejas, A., and Lupiáñez, J. (2010). Attention and anxiety: different attentional functioning under state and trait anxiety. Psychol. Sci. 21, 298–304. doi: 10.1177/0956797609359624

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parslow, R., and Jorm, A. (2007). Pretrauma and posttrauma neurocognitive functioning and ptsd symptoms in a community sample of young adults. Am. J. Geriat. Psychiatry 164, 509–515. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.3.509

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pashler, H., and Wagenmakers, E. J. (2012). Editors’ introduction to the special section on replicability in psychological science: a crisis of confidence? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 528–530. doi: 10.1177/1745691612465253

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peckham, A. D., McHugh, R. K., and Otto, M. W. (2010). A meta-analysis of the magnitude of biased attention in depression. Depress. Anxiety 27, 1135–1142. doi: 10.1002/da.20755

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peeters, M., Vollebergh, W. A. M., Wiers, R. W., and Field, M. (2014). Psychological changes and cognitive impairments in adolescent heavy drinkers. Alcohol. Alcohol. 49, 182–186. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agt162

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, S. E., and Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 73–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pietrzak, R. H., Mollica, C. M., Maruff, P., and Snyder, P. J. (2006). Cognitive effects of immediate-release methylphenidate in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 1225–1245. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.002

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piskulic, D., Olver, J. S., Norman, T. R., and Maruff, P. (2007). Behavioural studies of spatial working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia: a quantitative literature review. Psychiatry Res. 150, 111–121. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.03.018

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polak, A. R., Witteveen, A. B., Reitsma, J. B., and Olff, M. (2012). The role of executive function in posttraumatic stress disorder: a systematic review. J. Affect. Disord. 141, 11–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.01.001

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., and Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 25–42. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potvin, S., Stavro, K., Rizkallah,É., and Pelletier, J. (2014). Cocaine and cognition: a systematic and quantitative reivew. J. Addict. Med. 8, 368–376. doi: 10.1097/ADM.0000000000000066

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Price, R. B., and Mohlman, J. (2007). Inhibitory control and symptom severity in late life generalized anxiety disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 2628–2639. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.06.007

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purcell, R., Maruff, P., Kyrios, M., and Pantelis, C. (1998). Neuropsychological deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison with unipolar depression, panic disorder, and normal controls. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 55, 415–423. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.5.415

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabipour, S., and Raz, A. (2012). Training the brain: fact and fad in cognitive and behavioral remediation. Brain Cogn. 79, 159–179. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.02.006

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapport, M. D., Orban, S. A., Kofler, M. J., and Friedman, L. M. (2013). Do programs designed to train working memory, other executive functions, and attention benefit children with ADHD? A meta-analytic review of cognitive, academic, and behavioral outcomes. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 8, 1237–1252. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.08.005

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rende, B., Ramsberger, G., and Miyake, A. (2002). Commonalities and differences in the working memory components underlying letter and category fluency tasks: a dual-task investigation. Neuropsychology 16, 309–321. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.16.3.309

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, T. W., James, M., Owen, A. M., Sahakian, B. J., Lawrence, A. D., McInnes, L.,et al. (1998). A study of performance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers: implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 4, 474–490. doi: 10.1017/S1355617798455073

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, L. J., Thompson, J., Gallagher, P., Goswami, U., Young, A., Ferrier, I.,et al. (2006). A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 93, 105–115. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.02.016

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rock, P. L., Roiser, J. P., Riedel, W. J., and Blackwell, A. D. (2013). Cognitive impairment in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 44, 2029–2040. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002535

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., and Jankowski, J. J. (2011). Modeling a cascade of effects: the role of speed and executive functioning in preterm/full-term differences in academic achievement. Dev. Sci. 14, 1161–1175. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01068.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruscio, A. M., Chiu, W. T., Roy-Byrne, P., Stang, P. E., Stein, D. J., Wïttchen, H.-U.,et al. (2007). Broadening the definition of generalized anxiety disorder: effects on prevalence and associations with other disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. J. Anxiety Disord. 21, 662–676. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.10.004

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, J. C., Matt, G. E., Wrocklage, K. M., Crnich, C., Jordan, J., and Schweinsburg, B. C. (2015). A quantitative meta-analysis of neurocognitive functioning in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol. Bull. 141, 105–140. doi: 10.1037/a0038039

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shallice, T. (2002). “Fractionation of the supervisory system,” in Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, eds D. T. Stuss and R. T. Knight (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 261–277. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195134971.003.0017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, N. Y., Lee, T. Y., Kim, E., and Kwon, J. S. (2013). Cognitive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 44, 1121–1130. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001803

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. L., Mattick, R. P., Jamadar, S. D., and Iredale, J. M. (2014). Deficits in behavioural inhibition in substance abuse and addiction: a meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 145, 1–33. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.08.009

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, H. R. (2013). Major depressive disorder is associated with broad impairments on neuropsychological measures of executive function: a meta-analysis and review. Psychol. Bull. 139, 81–132. doi: 10.1037/a0028727

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, H. R., Hutchison, N., Nyhus, E., Curran, T., Banich, M. T., O’Reilly, R. C.,et al. (2010). Neural inhibition enables selection during language processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 16483–16488. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1002291107

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, H. R., Kaiser, R. H., Warren, S. L., and Heller, W. (2015). Obsessive-compulsive disorder is associated with broad impairments in executive function: a meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 3, 301–330. doi: 10.1177/2167702614534210

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, H. R., Kaiser, R. H., Whisman, M. A., Turner, A. E. J., Guild, R. M., and Munakata, Y. (2014). Opposite effects of anxiety and depressive symptoms on executive function: the case of selecting among competing options. Cogn. Emot. 28, 893–902. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.859568

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, H. R., and Munakata, Y. (2010). Becoming self-directed: abstract representations support endogenous flexibility in children. Cognition 116, 155–167. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.04.007

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, H. R., and Munakata, Y. (2013). So many options, so little control: abstract representations can reduce selection demands to increase children’s self-directed flexibility. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 116, 659–673. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.010

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sprague, J., Verona, E., Kalkhoff, W., and Kilmer, A. (2011). Moderators and mediators of the stress-aggression relationship: executive function and state anger. Emotion 11, 61–73. doi: 10.1037/a0021788

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spronk, D. B., van Wel, J. H. P., Ramaekers, J. G., and Verkes, R. J. (2013). Characterizing the cognitive effects of cocaine: a comprehensive review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 1838–1859. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.003

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stavro, K., Pelletier, J., and Potvin, S. (2012). Widespread and sustained cognitive deficits in alcoholism: a meta-analysis. Addict. Biol. 18, 203–213. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00418.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stefanopoulou, E., Manoharan, A., Landau, S., Geddes, J. R., Goodwin, G., and Frangou, S. (2009). Cognitive functioning in patients with affective disorders and schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 21, 336–356. doi: 10.1080/09540260902962149

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuss, D. T. (2011). Functions of the frontal lobes: relation to executive functions. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 759–765. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000695

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stuss, D. T., and Alexander, M. P. (2007). Is there a dysexecutive syndrome? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 362, 901–915. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2096

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tackett, J. L., Lahey, B. B., van Hulle, C., Waldman, I., Krueger, R. F., and Rathouz, P. J. (2013). Common genetic influences on negative emotionality and a general psychopathology factor in childhood and adolescence. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 122, 1142–1153. doi: 10.1037/a0034151

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teuber, H.-L. (1972). Unity and diversity of frontal lobe functions. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 32, 615–656.

Google Scholar

Tokita, K., Yamaji, T., and Hashimoto, K. (2012). Roles of glutamate signaling in preclinical and/or mechanistic models of depression. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 100, 688–704. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2011.04.016

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., and Stanovich, K. E. (2012). Practitioner review: do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54, 131–143. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12001

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torres, I. J., Boudreau, V. G., and Yatham, L. N. (2007). Neuropsychological functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 116, 17–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01055.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Haugen, R., Thompson, M. S., and Kupfer, A. (2013). Effortful control and impulsivity as concurrent and longitudinal predictors of academic achievement. J. Early Adolesc. 33, 946–972. doi: 10.1177/0272431613477239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Heuvel, O., Veltman, D., Groenewegen, H., Witter, M., Merkelbach, J., and van Dyck, R. (2005). Disorder-specific neuroanatomical correlates of attentional bias in obsessive-compulsive disorder, panic disorder, and hypochondriasis. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 922–933. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.8.922

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Linden, M., Ceschi, G., Zermatten, A., Dunker, D., and Perroud, A. (2005). Investigation of response inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder using the Hayling task. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 11, 776–783. doi: 10.1017/S1355617705050927

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Mourik, R., Oosterlaan, J., and Sergeant, J. A. (2005). The Stroop revisited: a meta-analysis of interference control in AD/HD. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 150–165. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00345.x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaughan, L., and Giovanello, K. (2010). Executive function in daily life: age-related influences of executive processes on instrumental activities of daily living. Psychol. Aging 25, 343–355. doi: 10.1037/a0017729

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verbruggen, F., Logan, G. D., and Stevens, M. A. (2008). STOP-IT: windows executable software for the stop-signal paradigm. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 479–483. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.2.479

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vita, A., De Peri, L., Deste, G., and Sacchetti, E. (2012). Progressive loss of cortical gray matter in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis and meta-regression of longitudinal MRI studies. Transl. Psychiatry 2, e190. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.116

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walshaw, P. D., Alloy, L. B., and Sabb, F. W. (2010). Executive function in pediatric bipolar disorder and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: in search of distinct phenotypic profiles. Neuropsychol. Rev. 20, 103–120. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9126-x

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitmer, A., and Banich, M. T. (2007). Inhibition versus switching deficits in different forms of rumination. Psychol. Sci. 18, 546–553. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01936

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiebe, S. A., Espy, K. A., and Charak, D. (2008). Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children: I. Latent structure. Dev. Psychol. 44, 575–587. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.575

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiebe, S. A., Sheffield, T., Nelson, J. M., Clark, C. A. C., Chevalier, N., and Espy, K. A. (2011). The structure of executive function in 3-year-olds. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108, 436–452. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.008

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S., and Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1336–1346. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., and DeFries, J. C. (2007). Understanding comorbidity: a twin study of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Med. Genet. 144B, 709–714. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30310

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, J. M., Mathews, A., and MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol. Bull. 120, 3–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, S. E., Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Willcutt, E. G., Corley, R. P., Haberstick, B. C.,et al. (2009). Behavioral disinhibition: liability for externalizing spectrum disorders and its genetic and environmental relation to response inhibition across adolescence. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 118, 117–130. doi: 10.1037/a0014657

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zakzanis, K. K., Campbell, Z., and Jovanovski, D. (2007). The neuropsychology of ecstasy (MDMA) use: a quantitative review. Hum. Psychopharmacol. Clin. Exp. 22, 427–435. doi: 10.1002/hup.873

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zetsche, U., D’Avanzato, C., and Joormann, J. (2012). Depression and rumination: relation to components of inhibition. Cogn. Emot. 26, 758–767. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.613919

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: psychopathology, executive function, inhibition, shifting, working memory, methods, transdiagnostic

Citation: Snyder HR, Miyake A and Hankin BL (2015) Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Front. Psychol. 6:328. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00328

Received: 20 December 2014; Accepted: 07 March 2015;
Published online: 26 March 2015

Edited by:

Damien Brevers, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Alexandre Heeren, Université Catholique de Louvain, Belgium
Qinghua He, Southwest University, China

Copyright © 2015 Snyder, Miyake and Hankin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hannah R. Snyder, Department of Psychology, University of Denver, Frontier Hall, 2155 S. Race Street, Denver, CO 80208, USA hsnyders@du.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.