Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 07 May 2015
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

Somatic, but not cognitive–affective, symptoms are associated with reduced heart rate variability in individuals with dysphoria

\r\nSimone Messerotti Benvenuti*Simone Messerotti Benvenuti1*Giulia Buodo,Giulia Buodo1,2Rocco MennellaRocco Mennella1Daniela Palomba,Daniela Palomba1,2
  • 1 Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 2 Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, University of Padova, Padova, Italy

Background: Somatic, but not cognitive–affective, symptoms of depression have been associated with reduced heart rate variability (HRV), and with poor prognosis in cardiovascular patients. However, factors concomitant with cardiovascular diseases may confound the relationship between somatic symptoms of depression and reduced HRV. Therefore, this study examined whether reduced HRV was differentially associated with cognitive–affective and somatic symptoms of depression in medically healthy individuals with and without dysphoria.

Methods: Self-reported cognitive–affective and somatic symptoms as measured with the Beck Depression Inventory-II questionnaire and time and frequency domain parameters of HRV were collected in 62 medically healthy individuals, of whom 25 with and 37 without dysphoria.

Results: Somatic, but not cognitive–affective, symptoms of depression were inversely associated with SD of NN intervals (β = -0.476, p < 0.05), number of interval differences of successive NN intervals greater than 50 ms (NN50; β = -0.498, p < 0.03), and HRV total power (β = -0.494, p < 0.04) in the group with dysphoria, after controlling for sex, anxiety, and lifestyle factors. Cognitive–affective and somatic symptoms were not related to any of the HRV parameters in the group without dysphoria (all ps > 0.24).

Conclusion: By showing that the relationship between somatic depressive symptoms and reduced HRV extends to medically healthy individuals with dysphoria, the present findings suggest that this association is independent of factors concomitant with cardiovascular diseases. The present study also suggests that individuals with somatic rather than cognitive–affective subsets of depressive symptoms may be at greater risk for developing cardiovascular diseases.

Introduction

Depression is an important and independent risk factor for the onset of coronary artery disease (CAD) as well as for the adverse course and outcomes in patients with CAD (Wulsin and Singal, 2003; Barth et al., 2004) or following myocardial infarction (MI; Van Melle et al., 2004). Specifically, there is evidence that depression is associated with a 60% greater likelihood of having CAD (Wulsin and Singal, 2003). Patients with depression are also more likely to have a major cardiac event within a year of the diagnosis of CAD (Carney et al., 1988), and/or to die in the years following the diagnosis (Barefoot et al., 1996). Moreover, the presence of depression in post-MI patients is associated with a 2- to 2.5-fold increased risk of poor cardiovascular outcome (Van Melle et al., 2004). It has been also reported that depression is a risk factor for cardiac morbidity and/or mortality in patients who have undergone cardiac surgery (Lespérance et al., 2000; Blumenthal et al., 2003).

The autonomic nervous system (ANS) has been identified as a pivotal site of dysregulation, with reduced parasympathetic and/or increased sympathetic activity leading to arrhythmias and sudden cardiac death (Schwartz and Vanoli, 1981; Podrid et al., 1990; Musselman et al., 1998). Given that depression itself is associated with reduced parasympathetic and/or increased sympathetic activity (Kemp et al., 2010), the presence of depression is likely to potentiate the impaired parasympathetic control and increased sympathetic activity observed in patients with CAD. In turn, these depression-related ANS abnormalities may further predispose depressed patients with CAD to ventricular tachycardia, ventricular fibrillation, myocardial ischemia, and sudden cardiac death (Carney et al., 2002, 2005).

In line with these findings, reduced heart rate variability (HRV), which reflects excessive sympathetic modulation and/or inadequate cardiac vagal control, has been reported in psychiatric depressed individuals compared to healthy controls (Imaoka et al., 1985; Dallack and Roose, 1990; Rechlin et al., 1994). Importantly, there is even more evidence that depressed patients with CAD or after MI have reduced HRV compared to non-depressed individuals (Carney et al., 1995, 2001; for reviews, see Carney et al., 2002, 2005; Shaffer et al., 2014). It has been recently shown that an association between depression and reduced HRV extends also to patients who underwent cardiac surgery (Patron et al., 2012, 2014, 2015). Based on these findings, reduced HRV has been suggested as one possible pathophysiological marker of depression-related ANS dysregulation that has been implicated as a risk factor for cardiac morbidity and/or mortality in CAD, post-MI, or post-surgical patients (Carney et al., 2002, 2005; Patron et al., 2012, 2014, 2015).

It should be noted, however, that some studies failed to replicate the association between depression and reduced HRV in patients with CAD (Gehi et al., 2005) or post-MI (Martens et al., 2008). One possible explanation for these inconsistent findings may lie in the heterogeneity of depressive symptoms (Kemp et al., 2014), including cognitive–affective (e.g., negative self-image, guilt) and somatic (e.g., sleep problems, poor appetite, fatigue) symptoms. Indeed, a reanalysis of data from Gehi et al. (2005) showed that somatic, but not cognitive–affective, symptoms of depression were associated with reduced HRV in patients with stable CAD (de Jonge et al., 2007). Consistent with this finding, several lines of evidence have shown that cardiovascular prognosis is associated with somatic, but not cognitive–affective, depressive symptoms (e.g., Watkins et al., 2003; de Jonge et al., 2006; Linke et al., 2009; Roest et al., 2011). Specifically, Watkins et al. (2003) showed that medical comorbidity in patients with acute MI were more strongly associated with somatic than cognitive–affective depressive symptoms. Likewise, de Jonge et al. (2006) reported that somatic, but not cognitive–affective, symptoms of depression were related to poor cardiovascular prognosis in patients 2.5 years after MI, a finding that has been replicated by others (Smolderen et al., 2009; Martens et al., 2010; Roest et al., 2011). Notably, Linke et al. (2009) and Schiffer et al. (2009) have recently extended these findings to women with suspected myocardial ischemia and patients with chronic heart failure, respectively.

Despite the intriguing evidence suggesting that somatic, but not cognitive–affective, symptoms of depression may differentially impact cardiovascular risk by reducing HRV (de Jonge et al., 2007), research on depression and HRV has been typically conducted in cardiovascular patients (Kemp et al., 2010). Therefore, many factors concomitant with cardiovascular diseases (e.g., disease severity, the presence of comorbid medical conditions) may confound the observed association between somatic symptoms of depression and reduced HRV (Carney and Freedland, 2012). Another important condition that may impact the relationship between somatic symptoms and HRV in cardiovascular patients with depression is the presence of anxiety symptoms. Some studies have suggested that anxiety – a comorbid condition reported in more than 60% of patients with major depression disorder (MDD; Kessler et al., 2005) – rather than depression is associated with reduced HRV (Thayer and Lane, 2000; Cohen and Benjamin, 2006; Friedman, 2007; Rottenberg, 2007; Miu et al., 2009), and with the risk of cardiovascular diseases (Cohen and Benjamin, 2006; Vogelzangs et al., 2010). Consistent with these findings, Martens et al. (2008) have shown that anxiety, but not depression, was associated with reduced HRV. Surprisingly, despite this converging evidence, the majority of studies examining the relationship between depression and reduced HRV in cardiovascular patients did not control for the effects of anxiety (e.g., Carney et al., 1995, 2001; Stein et al., 2000; de Jonge et al., 2007).

In light of these considerations, it is crucial to examine whether HRV is differentially associated with subsets of depressive symptoms in medically healthy individuals, while controlling for anxiety symptoms. At present, only one study to our knowledge has examined whether symptom dimensions of depression were differentially related to HRV in medically healthy preadolescents (Bosch et al., 2009). Specifically, consistent with findings observed in patients with cardiovascular disease, Bosch et al. (2009) reported that somatic, but not cognitive–affective, depressive symptoms were inversely associated with HRV and baroreflex sensitivity. However, the results by Bosch et al. (2009) were limited to the preadolescent population, aged between 10 and 12 years. This limitation is even more important when one considers that HRV parameters reach their maximum at about 20 and 30 years of age (Korkushko et al., 1991), and differ between healthy individuals aged 10 years and those aged >16 years (Silvetti et al., 2001). In addition, Bosch et al. (2009) did not control for the potential effect of anxiety on the relationship between somatic depressive symptoms and reduced HRV.

Therefore, the aim of the present study was to examine whether somatic and cognitive–affective symptoms of depression would be differentially associated with time and frequency domain parameters of HRV in medically healthy individuals aged 18–30 years, while controlling for sex, anxiety, and lifestyle factors. Based on previous findings (de Jonge et al., 2007; Bosch et al., 2009), it was hypothesized that HRV parameters would be inversely related to somatic, but not cognitive–affective, symptoms of depression. Given that somatic, but not cognitive–affective, subsets of depressive symptoms are distributed taxonomically (Beach and Amir, 2003), it is possible that depression may influence HRV parameters only when somatic depressive symptoms exceed a certain threshold of severity. Therefore, it was also examined whether somatic or cognitive–affective symptoms of depression would be differentially correlated with HRV parameters in individuals with or without dysphoria. To this end, we divided the whole sample into two subgroups depending on the presence/absence of dysphoria. It was expected that somatic depressive symptoms would be inversely related to HRV parameters in individuals with, but not without, dysphoria.

Materials and Methods

Participants

After local Ethics Committee approval, 62 undergraduate students from the University of Padova were enrolled in the present study. Participants were mostly females (n = 54; 87%), with a mean (SD) age of 21.9 (2.0). None of the participants were taking antidepressants or medications known to affect ANS functioning or had a history of neurological or cardiovascular disease. The descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Characteristics of the participants enrolled in the study.

Assessment of Depressive and Anxiety Symptoms

Depressive symptoms were assessed using the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck et al., 1996; Ghisi et al., 2006). The BDI-II is a reliable and valid self-report questionnaire that evaluates the severity of symptoms of depression in the past 2 weeks. Answers are given on a four-point (0–3) Likert scale and scores range from 0 to 63, with higher scores indicating more severe depressive symptoms. Of note, there is large evidence that the BDI-II is a reliable and valid measure of the severity of depressive symptoms in community samples (Lasa et al., 2000) and college populations (Sprinkle et al., 2002). In the Italian version, a score of 12 has been identified as an optimal cut-off to discriminate individuals with and without problems of depression in the Italian population (Ghisi et al., 2006).

The structure of the BDI-II has been examined in several studies using factor analysis (Ward, 2006). A two-factor solution has been frequently reported in non-clinical (Whisman et al., 2000; Storch et al., 2004) and clinical samples (Beck et al., 1996; Steer et al., 1998, 1999). Specifically, the two-factor solution usually comprises a cognitive–affective factor and a somatic factor in non-clinical individuals (mostly students; Beck et al., 1996; Whisman et al., 2000; Storch et al., 2004; Ward, 2006) as well as in psychiatric patients (Beck et al., 1996; Steer et al., 1998, 1999). Separate scores for each factor were calculated for each participant.

The module A of the Structured Clinical Interview (SCID-I) for the DSM-IV Axis I (First et al., 1997; Italian version by Mazzi et al., 2000) was administered in order to confirm the presence of dysphoria (i.e., at least two current symptoms of depression; Judd et al., 1994; Maier et al., 1997) and to exclude individuals with major depression, dysthymia, or bipolar disorder.

Self-reported trait anxiety symptoms were assessed using the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory, form Y2 (STAI-Y2; Spielberger et al., 1996). The STAI form Y1 and Y2 is composed by two 20-item four-point Likert scales that measure state and trait anxiety, respectively. State anxiety represents the individual’s fluctuant and transitory anxiety, whereas trait anxiety indicates long-lasting and persistent anxiety. Accordingly, test-retest reliability within a 1-month interval is very high for STAI-Y2 (r = 0.82), which evaluates the stable trait of anxiety, whereas it is moderate for STAI Y1 (r = 0.49), which, in turn, evaluates current and time-dependent anxiety. Raw scores range between 20 and 80 for both scales, with higher scores reflecting higher levels of trait or state anxiety. Given that reduced HRV is more likely to be associated with trait rather than state anxiety, we included only trait anxiety in the statistical analyses.

HRV Recordings

The electrocardiogram (ECG) was recorded using Ag/AgCl surface electrodes positioned on the participant’s chest in a modified lead II configuration. The ECG signal was amplified with a gain of 150, bandpass filtered (0.3–100 Hz, to avoid distortions of the T-wave signal), digitized at 500 Hz (16 bit A/D converter; resolution 0.559 μV/LSB), and stored on to a Pentium IV computer. Impedance was kept below 5 kΩ. The ECG signal was amplified and filtered by a SynAmps unit amplifier (Neuroscan, Inc., Compumedics, Ltd, El Paso, TX, USA). A digital trigger detecting R-waves was applied to the ECG signal to obtain RR intervals, corresponding to the inverse of heart rate (HR). All ECG data were visually inspected and artifacts were corrected with a piecewise cubic spline interpolation method that generates missing or corrupted values into RR intervals. Then, time domain parameters of HRV were calculated by Kubios HRV Analysis Software 2.0 (The Biomedical Signal Analysis Group, Department of Applied Physics, University of Kuopio, Finland). Time domain parameters, which are measures of the total variation of HR, were calculated as follows:

  1. Standard deviation of NN intervals (SDNN) expressed in ms, which reflects the cyclic components responsible for the variability of HR (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, and the North American Society for pacing, and electrophysiology, 1996);

  2. Root mean square of successive difference of NN intervals (rMSSD) expressed in ms, which reflects estimates of short-term variability of HR. rMSSD is highly sensitive to the fluctuation of high frequency (HF) of HRV and is an index of cardiac vagal control (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, and the North American Society for pacing, and electrophysiology, 1996);

  3. Number of interval differences of successive NN intervals greater than 50 ms in the entire recording (NN50), which reflects estimates of short-term variability of HR and is an index of cardiac vagal control (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, and the North American Society for pacing, and electrophysiology, 1996).

Fast Fourier spectral analysis was then applied on the NN series to compute frequency domain indexes, which, in turn, were calculated as follows:

  1. HRV total power in ms2, which reflects the variance of all NN intervals (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, and the North American Society for pacing, and electrophysiology, 1996);

  2. HF power (0.15–0.40 Hz) in ms2, which primarily reflects cardiac parasympathetic tone (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, and the North American Society for pacing, and electrophysiology, 1996).

All frequency domain indexes were logarithmically transformed to normalize their distribution.

Procedure

Upon arrival at the laboratory, the participants read and signed an informed consent form and completed the BDI-II and STAI-Y2 questionnaires. In order to confirm the presence of dysphoria and to exclude the presence of major depression, dysthymia, or bipolar disorder, all the participants were administered the SCID-I for the DSM-IV by a trained psychologist. The participants completed an ad hoc interview that provided information on their age, health status, smoking habits, and regular alcohol use. Then, each participant was seated in a comfortable armchair in a sound-attenuated, dimly-lit room. After the electrodes were attached, the ECG was continuously recorded at rest over a 5-min period. In order to avoid movement artifacts, participants were instructed to stay still and not to talk during the ECG recordings.

Statistical Analysis

According to the two-factor structure by Carvalho Bos et al. (2009), cognitive–affective symptoms were calculated by summing the scores of 12 BDI-II items for each individual: indecisiveness, worthlessness, loss of pleasure, punishment feelings, past failure, pessimism, self-criticalness, sadness, loss of interest, self-dislike, guilty feelings, and suicidal thoughts. Somatic symptoms were calculated by summing the scores of the remaining nine BDI-II items: agitation, changes in appetite, crying, tiredness or fatigue, loss of energy, irritability, changes in sleeping pattern, concentration difficulty, and loss of interest in sex. Internal consistency measured by Cronbach’s alpha was high for both the cognitive–affective factor (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.88) and the somatic factor (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.78).

As our first step, a linear regression model was used to predict HR, SDNN, rMSSD, NN50, HRV total power, and HF power from the overall depressive symptoms (i.e., the total BDI-II score) with the group as a whole. Then, linear regression models were used to examine whether cognitive–affective and/or somatic symptoms were associated with HR, SDNN, rMSSD, NN50, HRV total power, and HF power.

As our second step, the whole sample was divided in two subgroups depending on the presence of dysphoria. In line with recommendations for the use of the BDI-II in non-clinical samples, and for categorization of research participants (see Kendall et al., 1987; Haaga and Solomon, 1993; Tennen et al., 1995; Frewen and Dozois, 2005), individuals who scored equal to or greater than 12 on the BDI-II, had at least two current depressive symptoms, at least 2 weeks in duration, and did not meet the diagnostic criteria for major depression, dysthymia, or bipolar disorder were assigned to the group with dysphoria (n = 25). The participants who scored lower than 12 on the BDI-II and had no current symptoms of depression or previous diagnosis of major depression, dysthymia, or bipolar depression were assigned to the group without dysphoria (n = 37). Chi-square or Fisher’s exact test or analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were used to compare the groups in terms of sex, age, smoking habits, regular alcohol use, cognitive–affective, and somatic symptoms, and anxiety symptoms. Linear regression models were used to examine whether overall depressive symptoms were associated with HR, SDNN, rMSSD, NN50, HRV total power, and HF power separately in the group with vs. without dysphoria. Then, linear regression models were used to predict HR, SDNN, rMSSD, NN50, HRV total power, and HF power from cognitive-affective, and somatic symptoms in both groups separately. Sex, anxiety symptoms, smoking habits, and regular alcohol use were included as covariates in each regression model. Age was not included as a covariate in the regression models due to the small age range of participants (SDs ≤ 2.0). In addition, all the participants were between 19 and 27 years of age, when HRV parameters reach their maximum and become stable (Korkushko et al., 1991; Silvetti et al., 2001). A p-value of <0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Somatic vs. Cognitive–Affective Symptoms and HRV

When considering the group as a whole, overall depressive symptoms were not associated with any of the autonomic measures (all ps > 0.31). In addition, HR, SDNN, rMSSD, NN50, HRV total power, and HF power were not predicted by cognitive–affective symptoms of depression (all ps > 0.67). Similarly, somatic symptoms of depression were unrelated to HR, SDNN, rMSSD, NN50, HRV total power, and HF power (all ps > 0.33).

Somatic vs. Cognitive–Affective Symptoms and HRV in Individuals with vs. without Dysphoria

The groups did not differ in terms of sex (p = 0.46), with a predominance of female students both in the group with dysphoria (n = 23, 92%) and in the group without dysphoria (n = 31, 84%). The groups did not differ in terms of smoking habits: the current smokers were 6 (24%) and 18 (49%) in the group with and without dysphoria (p = 0.05), respectively. Similarly, the groups did not differ in terms of regular alcohol use (p = 0.39), with one (4%) and five (14%) regular alcohol drinkers in the group with and without dysphoria, respectively. The mean age was slightly higher in the group without dysphoria (M = 22.4, SD = 2.0) compared to the group with dysphoria (M = 21.2, SD = 1.8; p < 0.03). Overall depressive symptoms as well as cognitive–affective and somatic symptoms were significantly higher in the group with dysphoria (total BDI-II score, M = 15.8, SD = 4.0; cognitive–affective symptoms, M = 9.0, SD = 3.9; somatic symptoms, M = 6.8, SD = 1.8) than without dysphoria (total BDI-II score, M = 3.3, SD = 3.0; cognitive–affective symptoms, M = 1.2, SD = 1.4; somatic symptoms, M = 2.1, SD = 2.3; all ps < 0.001). Likewise, anxiety symptoms, as assessed with the STAI-Y2, were higher in the group with dysphoria (M = 53.6, SD = 9.4) than without dysphoria (M = 34.6, SD = 7.5, p < 0.001).

With respect to the group with dysphoria, the total BDI-II scores were not associated with any of the autonomic measures (all ps > 0.07). Linear regressions showed that somatic symptoms were significantly associated with SDNN, NN50, and HRV total power. Specifically, higher somatic symptoms were associated with lower SDNN, NN50, and HRV total power in the group with dysphoria. It is worth noting that these significant associations were independent of sex, anxiety symptoms, smoking habits, and regular alcohol use. By contrast, cognitive–affective symptoms were unrelated to SDNN, NN50, and HRV total power in the group with dysphoria. Both cognitive–affective and somatic symptoms were not associated with HR, rMSSD, and HF power. All the statistical details of the regression analysis in the group with dysphoria are reported in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Results of linear regression analysis with cognitive–affective or somatic symptoms predicting HRV parameters in the group with dysphoria.

With respect to the group without dysphoria, the total BDI-II scores were not associated with any of the autonomic measures (all ps > 0.12). Similarly, the cognitive–affective and somatic symptoms did not predict any of the HRV parameters. All the statistical details of the regression analysis in the group without dysphoria are reported in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Results of linear regression analysis with cognitive–affective or somatic symptoms predicting HRV parameters in the group without dysphoria.

Discussion

The present study examined whether cognitive–affective and somatic symptoms of depression were differentially associated with time domain parameters of HRV in healthy individuals. Overall, self-reported depressive symptoms (i.e., the total BDI-II score) were not associated with any of the HRV parameters either when the group was considered as a whole or when participants were divided into a group with and a group without dysphoria. Similarly, no significant effects of cognitive–affective or somatic symptoms on any of the HRV parameters in the group as a whole or in individuals without dysphoria were noted. By contrast, SDNN, NN50, and HRV total power were significantly predicted by somatic, but not cognitive–affective, symptoms of depression in the group with dysphoria, while controlling for sex, anxiety symptoms, smoking habits, and regular alcohol use. Specifically, the somatic symptoms were inversely associated with SDNN, NN50, and HRV total power. Both somatic and cognitive–affective symptoms were unrelated to HR, rMSSD, and HF power in the group with dysphoria.

Our results are in line with previous findings showing an inverse association between somatic, but not cognitive–affective, symptoms of depression and HRV in patients with stable CAD (de Jonge et al., 2007). These novel findings add to the literature on the physiological mechanisms underlying the association between depression and cardiovascular diseases by showing that an inverse relationship between somatic depressive symptoms and HRV is present even in healthy individuals. Accordingly, the present data provide preliminary evidence that the relationship between somatic symptoms of depression and reduced HRV is independent of factors concomitant with cardiovascular diseases such as disease severity or the presence of comorbid medical conditions. Of note, the present study also showed that the relationship between somatic depressive symptoms and reduced HRV was independent of anxiety symptoms. The latter result is consistent with previous studies showing that depression, but not anxiety, is associated with reduced HRV (e.g., Pitzalis et al., 2001) and cardiovascular risk (e.g., Jiang et al., 2004). However, it should be noted that anxiety is comorbid with depression in more than 60% of cases (Kessler et al., 2005), and that anxiety rather than depression has been associated with reduced HRV in psychiatric or cardiovascular patients (e.g., Friedman, 2007; Martens et al., 2008). Clearly, future studies are warranted to examine the relative contribution of depression and anxiety to reduced HRV and cardiovascular risk in both clinical and non-clinical samples (Carney et al., 2000).

Our results are likely to explain why some studies did not find any association between overall depressive symptoms and reduced HRV in patients with CAD (Gehi et al., 2005) or post-MI (Martens et al., 2008). Likewise, the present results also provide an explanation for the inconsistent findings concerning the association between depressive symptoms and reduced HRV in psychiatric patients (e.g., Dallack and Roose, 1990; Yeragani et al., 1991; Rechlin et al., 1994; Moser et al., 1998). More specifically, the present findings are consistent with previous studies suggesting that depression is a heterogeneous disorder and that only specific subsets of symptoms may be related to poor cardiovascular health in individuals with dysphoria. Importantly, the finding that low HRV is associated with somatic symptoms of depression suggests that somatic rather than cognitive–affective symptoms are more likely to be implicated in the development of cardiovascular disease in medically healthy individuals with dysphoria. It is well-established, indeed, that reduced HRV reflects ANS dysfunctions, which, in turn, may predispose individuals with depression to ventricular tachycardia, ventricular fibrillation, myocardial ischemia, and sudden cardiac death (Carney et al., 2002, 2005). In line with this suggestion, somatic, but not cognitive–affective, symptoms of depression have been associated with low HRV (de Jonge et al., 2007), and with poor prognosis in cardiovascular patients (e.g., Watkins et al., 2003; de Jonge et al., 2006; Linke et al., 2009; Roest et al., 2011).

Along the same line of reasoning, it is important to note that somatic symptoms of depression were inversely related to SDNN of HRV. Among time domain parameters of HRV, the SDNN has been considered as one of the most accurate predictors of mortality in post-MI patients (Kleiger et al., 1987; Stein et al., 2000). Specifically, Kleiger et al. (1987) have reported that the risk of mortality is 5.3 higher in post-MI patients with SDNN < 50 ms compared to patients with SDNN > 100 ms. Clearly, longitudinal studies are warranted to examine whether SDNN may be a marker of the risk of cardiovascular diseases associated with somatic symptoms of depression.

Our results are also in line with previous findings by Bosch et al. (2009) showing that somatic symptoms of depression were related with low HRV in healthy preadolescents. However, some differences also exist between findings by Bosch et al. (2009) and those reported in the present study. First, while Bosch et al. (2009) found an inverse association between somatic symptoms of depression and HRV in the whole sample, we observed this relationship only in individuals with dysphoria (i.e., with a BDI-II score equal to or greater than 12 and at least two current depressive symptoms, at least 2 weeks in duration). An explanation that reconciles our findings with those by Bosch et al. (2009) is that the inverse relationship between somatic symptoms and HRV becomes much stronger when somatic symptoms exceed a threshold of severity. Second, in line with de Jonge et al.’s (2007), we observed that cognitive–affective symptoms were unrelated to HRV parameters, whereas Bosch et al. (2009) found an unexpected positive association between cognitive–affective symptoms and HRV parameters. This discrepant finding may be due to the different measurement instruments used. Specifically, while the BDI-II questionnaire reflects the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994), and accurately delineates cognitive and somatic depressive symptoms (Dozois et al., 1998), the Youth Self-Report (YSR; Achenbach and Rescorla, 2001) used by Bosch et al. (2009) was not developed to evaluate depressive symptoms according to the DSM-IV criteria. In addition, given that low internal consistency for both subscales of the YSR has been reported by Bosch et al. (2009), it is possible that the cognitive–affective scale of the YSR was heterogeneous, potentially biasing the relationship between cognitive–affective symptoms and HRV.

Although there is consistent evidence that increased sympathetic and/or parasympathetic cardiac control may represent one of the pathophysiological mechanisms linking depression to cardiovascular diseases, the relative contribution of the sympathetic and parasympathetic ANS to depression as a risk factor for cardiovascular diseases is still debated (Carney et al., 2001, 2005). Given that parasympathetic control exceeds sympathetic influence on the variability of resting HR time series (i.e., the HRV), the SDNN, as an estimate the overall HRV, is more likely to reflect cardiac vagal rather than sympathetic control (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, and the North American Society for pacing, and electrophysiology, 1996). In line with the de Jonge et al.’s findings (2007), our data suggest that somatic symptoms of depression may be associated with poor cardiac vagal control in medically healthy individuals with depressive symptoms. Consistent with our hypothesis, the NN50 count, which estimates HF variation in HR (Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, and the North American Society for pacing, and electrophysiology, 1996), was also inversely associated with somatic symptoms of depression. However, given that rMSSD and HF power were unrelated to somatic symptoms in individuals with dysphoria, it remains unclear whether the inverse relationship between somatic symptoms and HRV is actually mediated by reduced cardiac vagal control (Bosch et al., 2009).

The present study has strengths and limitations. Individuals with cardiovascular illness were excluded from the present study, thus avoiding the potential confounding effects of cardiovascular disease on the association between somatic symptoms and reduced HRV. Also, this is the first study to our knowledge that examined the differential association between subsets of depressive symptoms and HRV while controlling for anxiety symptoms.

Some limitations to be noted include the relatively small sample size and, therefore, the fact that we did not conduct a factor analysis on our factor structure. However, the two-factor solution (cognitive–affective vs. somatic symptoms of depression) has been widely used in non-clinical (e.g., Whisman et al., 2000; Storch et al., 2004; Bosch et al., 2009) and clinical samples (e.g., Beck et al., 1996; Steer et al., 1998, 1999; de Jonge et al., 2007). Specifically, the two-factor solution in non-clinical samples (mostly students) usually consists of a first, cognitive–affective factor (Factor 1), and of a second, somatic factor (Factor 2; Beck et al., 1996; Whisman et al., 2000; Storch et al., 2004). In psychiatric patients, the somatic–affective factor is usually the first factor obtained and the cognitive factor is the second factor identified (Beck et al., 1996; Steer et al., 1998, 1999). Given that our study was focused on undergraduates with and without dysphoria, we adopted the two-factor solution with a cognitive–affective factor and a somatic factor (Carvalho Bos et al., 2009). In addition, in order to balance the number of items included in the two factors, we assigned each item either to cognitive–affective or somatic dimensions according to the two-factor structure obtained by Carvalho Bos et al. (2009). The high internal consistency of the cognitive–affective and somatic dimensions confirms the adequacy of the two-factor structure used in the present study. Second, most somatic symptoms of depression (e.g., fatigue, change in appetite, sleep problems) overlap with characteristics of vital exhaustion, a condition known to be associated with the risk for future coronary diseases (Appels et al., 1987). In addition, fatigue, which can be considered as one of the key symptoms of both depression and vital exhaustion or as a symptom by itself (Kop, 2012), has been consistently reported as a risk factor for cardiac diseases (e.g., McSweeney et al., 2003). Therefore, the association between somatic symptoms of depression and cardiac risk, as reflected by reduced HRV, may be confounded by vital exhaustion and/or fatigue. However, it should be noted that somatic depressive symptoms have been shown to predict mortality in patients with heart failure even after controlling for exertion fatigue (Smith et al., 2012). Targeted treatment studies and experimental designs are needed to examine the relative contribution of somatic depressive symptoms, vital exhaustion and fatigue to cardiac risk, as indicated by reduced HRV. Third, although we controlled for the effect of smoking status and alcohol use, other unhealthy life-style factors, such as poor dietary intake or physical inactivity, may confound the relationship between somatic depressive symptoms and reduced HRV (Kop, 2012). Future research should examine whether the association between somatic symptoms of depression and reduced HRV is independent of poor dietary intake and physical inactivity, which have been previously associated with reduced HRV (Puig et al., 1993; Mozaffarian et al., 2008). Finally, given that this study used cross-sectional data, causality could not be inferred. However, the present study aimed at examining whether time domain HRV parameters were differentially associated with somatic rather than cognitive–affective symptoms of depression, but we do not claim that reduced HRV is either the cause or the result of cognitive–affective and/or somatic depressive symptoms. Clearly, further research should specifically address this issue.

Based on the present findings, future studies aimed at examining the relationship between depression and reduced HRV in cardiovascular patients should take into account the differential impact of cognitive–affective and somatic symptoms on ANS functioning. From a clinical point of view, the present study suggests that somatic rather than cognitive–affective symptoms of depression are more likely to be implicated in the depression-related risk of developing cardiovascular diseases in medically healthy individuals with dysphoria. The longitudinal evaluation of subsets of depressive symptoms, HRV parameters, and cardiovascular health is crucial to determine whether a reduced SDNN associated with somatic symptoms may predict the onset of cardiovascular disease in medically healthy individuals with depressed mood.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Achenbach, T. M., and Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for ASEBA School-Age Forms and Profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Appels, A., Hoppener, P., and Mulder, P. (1987). A questionnaire to assess premonitory symptoms of myocardial infarction. Int. J. Cardiol. 17, 15–24. doi: 10.1016/0167-5273(87)90029-5

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barefoot, J. C., Helms, M. J., Mark, D. B., Blumenthal, J. A., Califf, R. M., Haney, T. L., et al. (1996). Depression and long-term mortality risk in patients with coronary artery disease. Am. J. Cardiol. 78, 613–617. doi: 10.1016/S0002-9149(96)00380-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barth, J., Schumacher, M., and Herrmann-Lingen, C. (2004). Depression as a risk factor for mortality in patients with coronary heart disease: a meta-analysis. Psychosom. Med. 66, 802–813. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000146332.53619.b2

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beach, S. R., and Amir, N. (2003). Is depression taxonic, dimensional, or both? J. Abnorm. Psychol. 112, 228–236. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.2.228

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., and Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory, 2nd Edn Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation Harcourt Brace & Company.

Google Scholar

Blumenthal, J. A., Lett, H. S., Babyak, M. A., White, W., Smith, P. K., Mark, D. B., et al. (2003). Depression as a risk factor for mortality after coronary artery bypass surgery. Lancet 362, 604–609. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)14190-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosch, N. M., Riese, H., Dietrich, A., Ormel, J., Verhulst, F. C., and Oldehinkel, A. J. (2009). Preadolescents’ somatic and cognitive-affective depressive symptoms are differentially related to cardiac autonomic function and cortisol: the TRAILS study. Psychosom. Med. 71, 944–950. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181bc756b

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carney, R. M., Blumenthal, J. A., Stein, P. K., Watkins, L., Catellier, D., Berkman, L. F., et al. (2001). Depression, heart rate variability, and acute myocardial infarction. Circulation 104, 2024–2028. doi: 10.1161/hc4201.097834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carney, R. M., and Freedland, K. E. (2012). Are somatic symptoms of depression better predictors of cardiac events than cognitive symptoms in coronary heart disease? Psychosom. Med. 74, 33–38. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182405ac4

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carney, R. M., Freedland, K. E., Miller, G. E., and Jaffe, A. S. (2002). Depression as a risk factor for cardiac mortality and morbidity: a review of potential mechanisms. J. Psychosom. Res. 53, 897–902. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00311-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carney, R. M., Freedland, K. E., and Stein, P. K. (2000). Anxiety, depression, and heart rate variability. Psychosom. Med. 62, 84–86. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200001000-00013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carney, R. M., Freedland, K. E., and Veith, R. C. (2005). Depression, the autonomic nervous system, and coronary heart disease. Psychosom. Med. 67, S29–S33. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000162254.61556.d5

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carney, R. M., Rich, M. W., Freedland, K. E., teVelde, A., Saini, J., Simeone, C., et al. (1988). Major depressive disorder predicts cardiac events in patients with coronary artery disease. Psychosom. Med. 50, 627–633. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198811000-00009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carney, R. M., Saunders, R. D., Freedland, K. E., Stein, P., Rich, M. W., and Jaffe, A. S. (1995). Association of depression with reduced heart rate variability in coronary artery disease. Am. J. Cardiol. 76, 562–564. doi: 10.1016/S0002-9149(99)80155-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carvalho Bos, S., Pereira, A. T., Marques, M., Maia, B., Soares, M. J., Valente, J., et al. (2009). The BDI-II factor structure in pregnancy and postpartum: two or three factors? Eur. Psychiatry 24, 334–340. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.10.003

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, H., and Benjamin, J. (2006). Power spectrum analysis and cardiovascular morbidity in anxiety disorders. Auton. Neurosci. 128, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2005.06.007

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dallack, G. W., and Roose, S. P. (1990). Perspectives on the relationship between cardiovascular disease and affective disorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 51, 4–9.

Google Scholar

de Jonge, P., Mangano, D., and Whooley, M. A. (2007). Differential association of cognitive and somatic depressive symptoms with heart rate variability in patients with stable coronary heart disease: findings from the Heart and Soul Study. Psychosom. Med. 69, 735–739. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815743ca

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Jonge, P., Ormel, J., van den Brink, R. H., van Melle, J. P., Spijkerman, T. A., Kuijper, A., et al. (2006). Symptom dimensions of depression following myocardial infarction and their relationship with somatic health status and cardiovascular prognosis. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 138–144. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.1.138

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dozois, D. J. A., Dobson, K. S., and Ahnberg, J. L. (1998). A psychometric evaluation of the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychol. Assess. 10, 83–89. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.83

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., and Williams, J. B. W. (1997). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID I). Clinical Version. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, Inc.

Google Scholar

Frewen, P. A., and Dozois, D. J. A. (2005). Recognition and interpretation of facial expressions in dysphoric women. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 27, 305–315. doi: 10.1007/s10862-005-2410-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, B. H. (2007). An autonomic flexibility-neurovisceral integration model of anxiety and cardiac vagal tone. Biol. Psychol. 74, 185–199. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.009

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gehi, A., Mangano, D., Pipkin, S., Browner, W. S., and Whooley, M. A. (2005). Depression and heart rate variability in patients with stable coronary heart disease: findings from the Heart and Soul Study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 661–666. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.661

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghisi, M., Flebus, G. B., Montano, A., Sanavio, E., and Sica, C. (2006). Beck Depression Inventory-II. BDI-II. Manuale. Florence: O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali.

Google Scholar

Haaga, D. A. F., and Solomon, A. (1993). Impact of Kendall, Hollon, Beck, Hammen, and Ingram (1987) on treatment of the continuity issue in “depression” research. Cogn. Ther. Res. 17, 313–324. doi: 10.1007/BF01177657

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imaoka, K., Inoue, H., Inoue, Y., Hazama, H., Tanaka, T., and Yamane, N. (1985). R-R intervals of ECG in depression. Folia Psychiatr. Neurol. Jpn. 39, 485–488. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.1985.tb00801.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, W., Kuchibhatla, M., Cuffe, M. S., Christopher, E. J., Alexander, J. D., Clary, G. L., et al. (2004). Prognostic value of anxiety and depression in patients with chronic heart failure. Circulation 110, 3452–3456. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000148138.25157.F9

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Judd, L. L., Rapaport, M. H., Paulus, M. P., and Brown, J. L. (1994). Subsyndromal symptomatic depression: a new mood disorder? J. Clin. Psychiatry 55, 18–28. doi: 10.2165/00023210-199401060-00001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemp, A. H., Quintana, D. S., Gray, M. A., Felmingham, K. L., Brown, K., and Gatt, J. M. (2010). Impact of depression and antidepressant treatment on heart rate variability: a review and meta-analysis. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 1067–1074. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.012

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kemp, A. H., Quintana, D. S., Quinn, C. R., Hopkinson, P., and Harris, A. W. (2014). Major depressive disorder with melancholia displays robust alterations in resting state heart rate and its variability: implications for future morbidity and mortality. Front. Psychol. 5:1387. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01387

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., Beck, A. T., Hammen, C. L., and Ingrain, R. E. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding use of the Beck Depression Inventory. Cogn. Ther. Res. 11, 289–299. doi: 10.1007/BF01186280

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Chiu, W. T., Demler, O., Merikangas, K. R., and Walters, E. E. (2005). Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 617–627. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleiger, R. E., Miller, J. P., Bigger, J. T. Jr., and Moss, A. J. (1987). Decreased heart rate variability and its association with mortality after myocardial infarction. Am. J. Cardiol. 113, 256–262. doi: 10.1016/0002-9149(87)90795-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kop, W. J. (2012). Somatic depressive symptoms, vital exhaustion, and fatigue: divergent validity of overlapping constructs. Psychosom. Med. 74, 442–445. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31825f30c7

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korkushko, O. V., Shatilo, V. B., Plachinda, Y., and Shatilo, T. V. (1991). Autonomic control of cardiac chronotropic function in man as a function of age: assessment by power spectral analysis of heart rate variability. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 32, 191–198. doi: 10.1016/0165-1838(91)90113-H

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lasa, L., Ayuso-Mateos, J. L., Vázquez-Barquero, J. L., Díez-Manrique, F. J., and Dowrick, C. F. (2000). The use of the Beck Depression Inventory to screen for depression in the general population: a preliminary analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 57, 261–265. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(99)00088-9

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lespérance, F., Frasure-Smith, N., Juneau, M., and Théroux, P. (2000). Depression and 1-year prognosis in unstable angina. Arch. Intern. Med. 160, 1354–1360. doi: 10.1001/archinte.160.9.1354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linke, S. E., Rutledge, T., Johnson, B. D., Vaccarino, V., Bittner, V., Cornell, C. E., et al. (2009). Depressive symptom dimensions and cardiovascular prognosis among women with suspected myocardial ischemia: a report from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute-sponsored Women’s Ischemia Syndrome Evaluation. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 66, 499–507. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.27

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maier, W., Gänsicke, M., and Weiffenbach, O. (1997). The relationship between major and subthreshold variants of unipolar depression. J. Affect. Disord. 45, 41–51. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(97)00058-X

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martens, E. J., Hoen, P. W., Mittelhaeuser, M., de Jonge, P., and Denollet, J. (2010). Symptom dimensions of post-myocardial infarction depression, disease severity and cardiac prognosis. Psychol. Med. 40, 807–814. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709990997

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martens, E. J., Nyklícek, I., Szabó, B. M., and Kupper, N. (2008). Depression and anxiety as predictors of heart rate variability after myocardial infarction. Psychol. Med. 38, 375–383. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707002097

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazzi, F., Morosini, P., De Girolamo, G., Lussetti, M., and Guaraldi, G. P. (2000). SCID-I Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders. Clinical Version. Firenze: O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali.

Google Scholar

McSweeney, J. C., Cody, M., O’Sullivan, P., Elberson, K., Moser, D. K., and Garvin, B. J. (2003). Women’s early warning symptoms of acute myocardial infarction. Circulation 108, 2619–2623. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000097116.29625.7C

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miu, A. C., Heilman, R. M., and Miclea, M. (2009). Reduced heart rate variability and vagal tone in anxiety: trait versus state, and the effects of autogenic training. Auton. Neurosci. 145, 99–103. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2008.11.010

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moser, M., Lehofer, M., Hoehn-Saric, R., McLeod, D. R., Hildebrandt, G., Steinbrenner, B., et al. (1998). Increased heart rate in depressed subjects in spite of unchanged autonomic balance? J. Affect. Disord. 48, 115–124. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(97)00164-X

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mozaffarian, D., Stein, P. K., Prineas, R. J., and Siscovick, D. S. (2008). Dietary fish and omega-3 fatty acid consumption and heart rate variability in US adults. Circulation 117, 1130–1137. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.107.732826

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Musselman, D. L., Evans, D. L., and Nemeroff, C. B. (1998). The relationship of depression to cardiovascular disease: epidemiology, biology, and treatment. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 55, 580–592. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.7.580

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patron, E., Messerotti Benvenuti, S., Favretto, G., Gasparotto, R., and Palomba, D. (2014). Depression and reduced heart rate variability after cardiac surgery: the mediating role of emotion regulation. Auton. Neurosci. 180, 53–58. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2013.11.004

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patron, E., Messerotti Benvenuti, S., Favretto, G., Gasparotto, R., and Palomba, D. (2015). Depression is associated with increased vagal withdrawal during unpleasant emotional imagery after cardiac surgery. Auton. Neurosci. 189, 75–82. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2015.02.002

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patron, E., Messerotti Benvenuti, S., Favretto, G., Valfrè, C., Bonfà, C., Gasparotto, R., et al. (2012). Association between depression and heart rate variability in patients after cardiac surgery: a pilot study. J. Psychosom. Res. 73, 42–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.04.013

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitzalis, M. V., Iacoviello, M., Todarello, O., Fioretti, A., Guida, P., Massari, F., et al. (2001). Depression but not anxiety influences the autonomic control of heart rate after myocardial infarction. Am. Heart J. 141, 765–771. doi: 10.1067/mhj.2001.114806

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Podrid, P. J., Fuchs, T., and Candinas, R. (1990). Role of the sympathetic nervous system in the genesis of ventricular arrhythmia. Circulation 82, I103–I113.

Google Scholar

Puig, J., Freitas, J., Carvalho, M. J., Puga, N., Ramos, J., Fernandes, P., et al. (1993). Spectral analysis of heart rate variability in athletes. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness 33, 44–48.

Google Scholar

Rechlin, T., Weis, M., Spitzer, A., and Kaschka, W. F. (1994). Are affective disorders associated with alterations of heart rate variability? J. Affect. Disord. 32, 271–275. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(94)90091-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roest, A. M., Thombs, B. D., Grace, S. L., Stewart, D. E., Abbey, S. E., and de Jonge, P. (2011). Somatic/affective symptoms, but not cognitive/affective symptoms, of depression after acute coronary syndrome are associated with 12-month all-cause mortality. J. Affect. Disord. 131, 158–163. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.018

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rottenberg, J. (2007). Cardiac vagal control in depression: a critical analysis. Biol. Psychol. 74, 200–211. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.010

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiffer, A. A., Pelle, A. J., Smith, O. R., Widdershoven, J. W., Hendriks, E. H., and Pedersen, S. S. (2009). Somatic versus cognitive symptoms of depression as predictors of all-cause mortality and health status in chronic heart failure. J. Clin. Psychiatry 70, 1667–1673. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04609

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwartz, P. J., and Vanoli, E. (1981). Cardiac arrhythmias elicited by interaction between acute myocardial ischemia and sympathetic hyperactivity: a new experimental model for the study of antiarrhythmic drugs. J. Cardiovasc. Pharmacol. 3, 1251–1259. doi: 10.1097/00005344-198111000-00012

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaffer, F., McCraty, R., and Zerr, C. L. (2014). A healthy heart is not a metronome: an integrative review of the heart’s anatomy and heart rate variability. Front. Psychol. 5:1040. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01040

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvetti, M. S., Drago, F., and Ragonese, P. (2001). Heart rate variability in healthy children and adolescents is partially related to age and gender. Int. J. Cardiol. 81, 169–174. doi: 10.1016/S0167-5273(01)00537-X

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, O. R., Kupper, N., Schiffer, A. A., and Denollet, J. (2012). Somatic depression predicts mortality in chronic heart failure: can this be explained by covarying symptoms of fatigue? Psychosom. Med. 74, 459–463. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31824ef2f4

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smolderen, K. G., Spertus, J. A., Reid, K. J., Buchanan, D. M., Krumholz, H. M., Denollet, J., et al. (2009). The association of cognitive and somatic depressive symptoms with depression recognition and outcomes after myocardial infarction. Circ. Cardiovasc. Qual. Outcomes 2, 328–337. doi: 10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.109.868588

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Pedrabissi, L., and Santinello, M. (1996).STAI, State-trait Anxiety Inventory, Forma Y: Manuale. Florence: O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali.

Google Scholar

Sprinkle, S. D., Lurie, D., Insko, S. L., Atkinson, G., Jones, G. L., Logan, A. R., et al. (2002). Criterion validity, severity cut scores, and test-retest reliability of the Beck Depression Inventory-II in a university counseling center sample. J. Counsel. Psychol. 49, 381–385. doi: 10.1037//0022-0167.49.3.381

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steer, R. A., Ball, R., Ranieri, W. F., and Beck, A. T. (1999). Dimensions of the Beck Depression Inventory-II in clinically depressed outpatients. J. Clin. Psychol. 55, 117–128. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(199901)55:1<117::AID-JCLP12>3.0.CO;2-A

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steer, R. A., Kumar, G., Ranieri, W. F., and Beck, A. T. (1998). Use of the Beck Depression Inventory-II with adolescent psychiatric outpatients. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 20, 127–137. doi: 10.1023/A:1023091529735

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, P. E., Carney, R. M., Freedland, K. E., Skala, J. A., Kleiger, R. E., Rottman, J. N., et al. (2000). Severe depression is associated with markedly reduced heart rate variability in patients with stable coronary heart disease. J. Psychosom. Res. 48, 493–500. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(99)00085-9

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Storch, E. A., Roberti, J. W., and Roth, D. A. (2004). Factor structure, concurrent validity, and internal consistency of the Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition in a sample of college students. Depress. Anxiety 19, 187–189. doi: 10.1002/da.20002

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology, and the North American Society for pacing, and electrophysiology. (1996). Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Circulation 93, 1043–1065. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tennen, H., Hall, J. A., and Affleck, G. (1995). Depression research methodologies in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology: a review and critique. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 870–884. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.870

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., and Lane, R. D. (2000). A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J. Affect. Disord. 61, 201–216. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Melle, J. P., de Jonge, P., Spijkerman, T. A., Tijssen, J. G., Ormel, J., Van Veldhuisen, D. J., et al. (2004). Prognostic association of depression following myocardial infarction with mortality and cardiovascular events: a meta-analysis. Psychosom. Med. 66, 814–822. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000146294.82810.9c

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogelzangs, N., Seldenrijk, A., Beekman, A. T., van Hout, H. P., de Jonge, P., and Penninx, B. W. (2010). Cardiovascular disease in persons with depressive and anxiety disorders. J. Affect. Disord. 125, 241–248. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.02.112

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, L. C. (2006). Comparison of factor structure models for the Beck Depression Inventory–II. Psychol. Assess. 18, 81–88. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.1.81

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, L. L., Schneiderman, N., Blumenthal, J. A., Sheps, D. S., Catellier, D., Taylor, C. B., et al. (2003). Cognitive and somatic symptoms of depression are associated with medical comorbidity in patients after acute myocardial infarction. Am. Heart J. 146, 48–54. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8703(03)00083-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whisman, M. A., Perez, J. E., and Ramel, W. (2000). Factor structure of the Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-II) in a student sample. J. Clin. Psychol. 56, 545–551. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679(200004)56:4<545::AID-JCLP7>3.0.CO;2-U

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wulsin, L. R., and Singal, B. M. (2003). Do depressive symptoms increase the risk for the onset of coronary disease? A systematic quantitative review. Psychosom. Med. 65, 201–210. doi: 10.1097/01.PSY.0000058371.50240.E3

PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yeragani, V. K., Pohl, R., Balon, R., Ramesh, C., Glitz, D., Jung, I., et al. (1991). Heart rate variability in patients with major depression. Psychiatry Res. 37, 35–46. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(91)90104-W

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autonomic nervous system, cardiovascular risk, cognitive–affective symptoms, dysphoria, heart rate variability, somatic symptoms

Citation: Messerotti Benvenuti S, Buodo G, Mennella R and Palomba D (2015) Somatic, but not cognitive–affective, symptoms are associated with reduced heart rate variability in individuals with dysphoria. Front. Psychol. 6:599. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00599

Received: 16 February 2015; Accepted: 22 April 2015;
Published online: 07 May 2015.

Edited by:

J. P. Ginsberg, William Jennings Bryan Dorn Veterans Affairs Medical Center, USA

Reviewed by:

Guido Edoardo D’Aniello, Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Italy
Brandon Gillie, The Ohio State University, USA
Maria A. Zayas, Brenau University, USA

Copyright © 2015 Messerotti Benvenuti, Buodo, Mennella and Palomba. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Simone Messerotti Benvenuti, Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, Via Venezia, 8, 35131 Padova, Italy, simone.messerotti@unipd.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.