Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 07 January 2016
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Parenthood from Biology to Relation. Prevention, Assessment and Interventions for Developmental and Clinical Issues View all 32 articles

Implicit Attitude Toward Caregiving: The Moderating Role of Adult Attachment Styles

  • Department of Psychology, University of Milano-Bicocca, Milan, Italy

Attachment and caregiving are separate motivational systems that share the common evolutionary purpose of favoring child security. In the goal of studying the processes underlying the transmission of attachment styles, this study focused on the role of adult attachment styles in shaping preferences toward particular styles of caregiving. We hypothesized a correspondence between attachment and caregiving styles: we expect an individual to show a preference for a caregiving behavior coherent with his/her own attachment style, in order to increase the chance of passing it on to offspring. We activated different representations of specific caregiving modalities in females, by using three videos in which mothers with different Adult Attachment states of mind played with their infants. Participants' facial expressions while watching were recorded and analyzed with FaceReader software. After each video, participants' attitudes toward the category “mother” were measured, both explicitly (semantic differential) and implicitly (single target-implicit association task, ST-IAT). Participants' adult attachment styles (experiences in close relationships revised) predicted attitudes scores, but only when measured implicitly. Participants scored higher on the ST-IAT after watching a video coherent with their attachment style. No effect was found on the facial expressions of disgust. These findings suggest a role of adult attachment styles in shaping implicit attitudes related to the caregiving system.

Adult Attachment Styles and Implicit Attitudes Toward Caregiving Styles

Attachment Theory

In the first formulations of attachment theory, Bowlby (1969/1982, 1980) postulated that children's need of their caregivers, in terms of proximity seeking, was indispensable for evolutionary adaptation. Children rely on caregivers for safety and resources, but attachment bonds have far-reaching implications since they are involved in shaping development. As Bowlby (1969/1982, p. 64) stated, “Not a single feature of a species' morphology, physiology, or behavior can be understood or even discussed intelligently except in relation to that species' environment of evolutionary adaptness (EEA).” This is consistent with the idea that less than optimal developments should be conceived as a sort of “over-adaptation” to a maladaptive environment rather than as mere failures in adaptation.

The evolutionary pressure that fosters mother-infant bonding lies at the core of adult social behaviors. Baumeister and Leary (1995) highlighted the benefits of forming and maintaining social bonds in terms of survival and reproduction, proposing that the need for interpersonal attachments constitutes a fundamental human motivation. Bowlby introduced the concept of internal working models (IWM), whose function is “to simulate happenings in the real world, thereby enabling the individual to plan his behavior with all the advantages of insight and foresight” (Bowlby, 1988). The IWMs are expectations aimed at the preservation of self-regulation and positive affect even in adverse environmental conditions (Sroufe and Jacobvitz, 1989; Schore, 1994). Main et al. (1985) went beyond the behavioral perspective by introducing a representational framework, in which IWMs are considered to be “a set of conscious and/or unconscious rules,” organizing attachment-related memories, emotions and thought processes. (p. 66).

Social Cognition of Attachment

Westen (1991) proposed that attachment studies are one of the most productive fields in which psychodynamic and cognitive studies can be integrated. In his view, representations that underlie attachment-related processes constitute a key element in linking these two perspectives. In fact, Bowlby always underlined the primary role of beliefs and cognitive schemata in orienting attachment behaviors and expectations, but it is only after the reconceptualization of attachment theory in representational terms that social cognitive models and methods have effectively been implemented in this field (Bartholomew and Horowitz, 1991; Brennan et al., 1998; Brennan and Shaver, 2002; Mikulincer et al., 2005; Shaver and Mikulincer, 2013).

From a social cognition perspective, Baldwin et al. (1996) found that accessible memories of satisfying or unsatisfying attachment experiences play a role in shaping the perception of relationships. Moreover, they showed that mental schemas of attachment can have different accessibility depending on different contexts, in a similar way to the majority of cognitive structures. In particular, they asked the participant to visualize different kinds of relationships and this led them to react in different ways to interpersonal information and to show increased attraction toward potential dating partners with the same attachment orientation. This helped to improve our understanding of the IWMs moving forward from a definition based on personality traits to a more complex perspective based on a hierarchical structure of relationship specific attachment orientations (Bretherton, 1987, 1999). In the same perspective, Bartz and Lydon (2004) found the effect of manipulating close relationships on the working self-concept, in particular on agency and communion. Security priming of attachment leads to positive relationship expectations and affect (Rowe and Carnelley, 2003; Carnelley and Rowe, 2007; Gillath et al., 2008) and the effect seems stable across the life span and for different domains of social information processing (Dykas and Cassidy, 2011).

A social cognition perspective has been applied also to parenting behaviors. Atkinson et al. (2009) studied the interplay between emotional cognitive processes (selective attention in the Stroop task) and disorganized attachment in mothers and their children. They found an interesting interacting role of both attachment and loss of controlled attention that they interpret as a process of “threat tags.” They conclude that IWMs role in risk for psychopathology can be fully appreciated only if research tries to explore different levels of analysis of attachment representations (cognitive and affective). In fact, Okagaki and Bingham (2005) had tried to drive the attention of researchers to the relevance of a better comprehension of the relation between social cognition and behavior in order to develop effective parent intervention programs. For instance, parental stress seems to be associated to both anxiety and avoidance of attachment, because of the difficulties they imply in coping with distress, but in different ways: more avoidant women attribute negative distress to a characteristic of the baby and not situational factors; more anxious women make more mistakes in recognizing fear and attribute distress to physical factors, then they could show an out of sync response to the babies' distress signs (Leerkes and Siepak, 2006; for a complete review of a social cognition approach to parenting processes and behaviors, see: Jones et al., 2015a,b).

Implicit Activation of Attachment Representations

Attachment research studied the role of implicit activation of attachment representations (Gawronski and Payne, 2011), starting from Bowlby's idea of the importance of automatic attachment schemata. For instance, there is evidence of individual differences in expressing avoidance or hypervigilance with respect to attachment threats: fearful avoidant individuals are in fact characterized by cognitive avoidance of all highly emotional stimuli (Dewitte et al., 2007). Baldwin et al. (1993) as well as Mikulincer (1998) presented priming sentences like “If I trust my partner my partner will…” (this example manipulates the variable trust). Subsequently, a word non-word task with attachment related words (representing good or negative outcomes) as priming cues was administered. In general, insecure individuals were quicker in the negative outcome condition while secure were quicker in the good outcome condition. This result can be interpreted as insecure participants expecting less supportive partners in stressful conditions: for them the negative outcome is more naturally consequent to angry feelings.

One of the most widely-used paradigms to study the automatic activation of representations is the implicit association task (IAT; Greenwald et al., 1998, 2003), which assesses implicit dimensions of psychological constructs such as attitudes and stereotypes (for a description, see the Materials paragraph).

From the perspective of attachment research, Zayas and Shoda (2005) found a relation between Mother-IATs and Partner-IATs and an explicit measure of adult attachment (ECR). Low negative correlations were found between the Mother-IAT scores, defined as an association between the dichotomies “pleasant/unpleasant” and “supportive/rejecting” and the ECR-subscale Avoidance. For the Partner-IAT scores, participants were asked to indicate words that were highly related and highly unrelated to their partners. These words were then used as stimulus words to be classified according to a dichotomy “name of the partner/not-name of the partner.” In this case, the correlation with ECR-Avoidance was negative, but considerably higher. This last finding was consistent with Banse and Kowalick's (2007) comparison between groups of women in different stressful conditions. It seems that positive representations of partners constitutes a resource for coping in stressful life situations. Dewitte et al. (2008, p. 282) used the IAT “as an index of the implicit attachment self-concept” and found that self-esteem and relational anxiety on the IAT were, in fact, correlated with attachment style, and able to predict strategies for successfully managing attachment-related stressful circumstances. The aim of the present study is to extend this work on implicit processes related to attachment. We hypothesize an effect of attachment styles in shaping the implicit representations of caregiving related perceptions. Because of the centrality of attachment in affecting individual development, we propose a role of attachment dependent schemata in structuring the implicit activation determined by parenting behavior.

Attachment and Caregiving Systems

The fundamental role of the caregiver in influencing children's attachment behaviors and representations lead to the study of the caregiving behavioral system (George and Solomon, 2008). Bowlby (1969/1982, 1988) proposed the existence of a behavioral structure in caregivers, which is reciprocal to the recipients' attachment behavior. Parenting entails moving from a position characterized by seeking protection and security to one which requires providing protection and care. The connection between the two systems is suggested by their shared evolutionary aim. In fact, Belsky et al. (1991, p. 172) proposed a definition of attachment as an evolved psychological mechanism, through which the parents' experiences during childhood and adolescence are transmitted “probabilistically” to their offspring, shaping their development and reproductive approaches. The strategies (in both behavioral domains and emotion regulation) learnt in infancy, constitute an adaptive advantage because they promote faster and more specific responses within the EEA. As a consequence, such strategies tend to remain relatively stable during the life-span, as confirmed by empirical research (Fraley, 2002; see also Raby et al., 2015, for the transmission of infant attachment). Numerous studies support the strong correspondence between mothers' states of the mind with respect to attachment and their children's attachment (van IJzendoorn, 1995a). This evidence seems stable and constant in different cultures (Grossmann et al., 1988; Sagi et al., 1997; Kazui et al., 2000; Hautamäki et al., 2010) and in different risk populations (Bus and van IJzendoorn, 1997; Tarabulsy et al., 2005; McMahon et al., 2006; Shlafer et al., 2015). In Bowlby's (1969/1982) and Ainsworth's (Ainsworth et al., 1974, 1978) view, the mechanism responsible for this transmission should be the so called “maternal sensitivity,” defined as the ability of being aware of infants' signals and to correctly respond to them. The effect they hypothesized was that an insecure mother's attachment lead to a poor maternal sensitivity and in turn to the transmission of mother's attachment to the infant. Unexpectedly, research identified a “transmission gap” (van IJzendoorn, 1995a,b; van IJzendoorn and Bakermans-Kranenburg, 1997) because the mediating role of maternal sensitivity was significant but modest. Fonagy and Target (2005) proposed the mediating function of mothers' ability to mentalize as a necessary factor for creating a secure base environment for their children, as empirical research confirmed (Grienenberger et al., 2005; Slade et al., 2005). From a developmental perspective, Meins (Meins et al., 2001, 2012) proposed that is mothers' mind-mindedness, i.e., the capacity to understand children's mental states, what enables them to respond to the children's needs adequately. More recently, Bernier et al. (2014) proposed to add the notion of “maternal autonomy support” to describe mothers' ability to enhance children's confidence in exploring the environment. This approach seems to take into account the problem of the transmission gap, although further studies are needed to explore underlying social cognition processes. Scharf and Mayseless (2011) followed 60 men from adolescence to early adulthood and found a continuity between their state of mind with respect to attachment in adolescence and the quality of parenting 9 years later. Other studies used self-reported attachment measures (for reviews, see: Jones et al., 2014, 2015a,b) and found that insecurity is associated with more negative attitudes toward parenting, which is considered more stressful (Nathanson and Manohar, 2012). Secure individuals consider themselves more competent and effective in parenting, compared to less secure ones (Kilmann et al., 2009; Howard, 2010; Caldwell et al., 2011; Kohlhoff and Barnett, 2013). Anxiety was found to be associated to the perception of infants as interfering with parents' romantic relationship (Rholes et al., 2011) and jealousy toward the children (Wilson et al., 2007). Parental avoidance is correlated with less optimistic expectations for child outcomes (Lench et al., 2006).

From an evolutionary perspective, intergenerational transmission seems to foster the maintenance of strategies which are able to enhance the reproduction of the species (Belsky, 2005), but the emotional and cognitive mediators remain unclear. In particular, literature focused primarily on correlational studies, with a less intense focus on experimental manipulations.

The Present Study

Although there is a growing theoretical interest about the attachment transmission gap, few studies focused on the cognitive processes that may affect the individual's relational patterns. The aim of this study was to explore the link between adult attachment styles and attitudes toward different caregiving behavioral modalities. In particular, we investigated if current adult attachment styles make individuals discriminate different ways of caregiving, and if they are associated with a preference for a specific caregiving modality. We hypothesize that the preference for proximity, avoidance, or resistance in adult relationships could predict the attitude toward the perception of a new specific relationship (Brumbaugh and Fraley, 2007), like a caregiving one.

From an evolutionary perspective, the intergenerational transmission should foster the maintenance of strategies able to lead to reproduction of the species (Belsky, 2005). Therefore, if these strategies experienced by an individual led to reproduction, there is no need to change them and they can be transmitted to next generations. Then we expect adult attachment styles to shape the adult perception of caregiving strategies.

In order to explore the association between attachment styles and the perception of different caregiving modalities, we designed a study to elicit a specific caregiving representation in the participants and, then, we assessed the attitude to the semantic category “mother.” Our first hypothesis was that the activation of a caregiving representation in line with the participants' attachment style would entail a more positive attitude, implying a preference for a strategy that is coherent with one's own attachment style. For example, a participant who uses dismissing strategies in his/her adult relationships should show a more positive attitude when he/she is watching a dismissing caregiver than when he/she is watching a preoccupied mother and her baby.

Second, a fundamental question relates to whether the transmission of attachment styles is an automatic or implicit process of identification with an experienced caregiving modality. Do individuals have “script-like representations of secure base experiences” (Waters and Waters, 2006, p. 185) and do they use them as basis for comparison in forming attitudes of different situations? In order to face this issue we used two different measures of attitude, an implicit one and an explicit one. We expect a stronger effect of attachment style on the more implicit measure, because of the importance of automatic processes in IWMs.

Third, in the light of the strong connection between attachment styles and emotion regulation strategies, we are interested in linking the perception of different caregiving styles to a measure of emotion arousal. The process of forming attitudes has also an affective component (Petty et al., 2001; Clore and Schnall, 2005; Malhotra, 2005) so we propose at an explorative level an influence of attachment styles on the emotional processes that contribute to define a specific attitude toward a caregiving style. More specifically, disgust seems to be involved in the process of formation of attitudes, for instance in political orientations (Hibbing et al., 2014; Inbar and Pizarro, 2014). Disgust fosters avoidance not only of dangerous or unhealthy situations but also of unfair or not convenient conditions (Chapman et al., 2009; Chapman and Anderson, 2013; Tybur et al., 2013). The evolutionary purpose is to avoid unhealthy or unfamiliar behaviors, so it is possible that the same process takes place while an individual is selecting between different caregiving behaviors. Magai et al. (2000) found an effect of attachment “Preoccupation” on facial expression of disgust during an emotion elicitation task. For exploratory purposes, we want to test if this role of disgust can be extended during the perception of caregiving modalities. Then we tested whether the expression of disgust during the observation of a specific caregiving modality in adults depends on individual's attachment style. A caregiving modality less coherent with participant's attachment style should elicit more disgust. We are interested in this effect as a first step toward a better understanding of the mechanisms to form this kind of attitudes. This effect could be part of the process that increases the chances of intergenerational transmission of attachment style, influencing the formation of attitudes and driving behaviors. Attachment style would be confirmed as crucial in the psychophysiological process of discerning between different caregiving behaviors and the prominence of low-level information processes in forming these attitudes. The attitudes toward caregiving modalities would directly shape the emotions expression and regulation: this would suggest an explanation of transmission gap in a sort of communication at an implicit level between mother and child.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Seventy-three Italian undergraduate students participated in exchange for course credits at the Psychology Department of the University of Milan-Bicocca. The only exclusion criteria was having children. Twenty-five participants (12 males) were assigned to a preliminary phase; 48 females were assigned to a second step of the present study.

Materials

Experiences in Close Relationship-Revised (ECR-R; Fraley et al., 2000; Busonera et al., 2014)

The ECR-R is a questionnaire used to assess adult romantic attachment style as resulting of two orthogonal dimensions: a subscale of anxiety and a subscale of avoidance. High scores on the first subscale indicate a tendency to preoccupation, jealousy and fear of abandonment, while high scores on the second scale suggest uneasiness with intimacy. The questionnaire is largely used to measure individual differences in romantic adult attachment styles and shows good psychometric properties both for validity and reliability (Sibley et al., 2005; Fairchild, 2006).

Single Target-Implicit Association Task (ST-IAT; Wigboldus et al., 2004)

The ST-IAT is a version of IAT (Greenwald et al., 1998) that measures the level of association between two categories characterized by opposite polarities (e.g., pleasant/unpleasant) and a single target category (in the present study the category was “mother”). It is a computer task developed with Inquisit 4 (Inquisit, 2013): participants are asked to categorize the words that appear in the center of the screen depending on the category they belong to. By pressing different response keys, they associate these words to the categories presented on the top part of the screen: the left key for the left categories or the right key for the right categories. The words can refer to the a polarity (e.g., “wonderful” vs. “awful”) or to a target category (e.g., “care” or “comfort”). In different trials target category can be presented on the right or on the left, that is can be associated with each of the opposite polarities (“good,” “bad”). The differences in reaction times between the conditions when the category is associated with each of the two polarities represent a measure of association between one side of the polarity and the target category. In our task the IAT algorithm (Greenwald et al., 2003) produced the score of the association between the category “good” and the category “mother.” IAT has been shown to have good predictive validity (Greenwald et al., 2009) and validity of the scoring algorithm (Richetin et al., 2015). It is one of the most used task to study implicit attitudes (Teige-Mocigemba et al., 2010) and it has been used also in attachment research (Zayas and Shoda, 2005). The words used in the present task are the following. Positive: Marvelous, Superb, Pleasure, Beautiful, Joyful, Glorious, Lovely, Wonderful. Negative: Tragic, Horrible, Agony, Painful, Terrible, Awful, Humiliate, Nasty. Mother: Care, Attention, Consolation, Support, Help, Bond, Comfort.

Semantic Differential (SD; Osgood et al., 1957)

Semantic differential is an explicit measure of attitude. The participant is asked to think to a category (in our study “mother”) and to rate 10 bipolar adjective on 7-point Likert scales (e.g., “strong/weak,” “good/bad”). It has been recently used to evaluate different kinds of parents (Weed and Nicholson, 2015), showing a good discriminant ability.

FaceReader Software

FaceReader software version 5.0 (FR, Noldus, 2013) automatically analyzed facial expressions to detect the six basic emotions described by Ekman (1992): happy, sad, angry, surprised, scared, disgusted and a neutral state. The software showed a good convergent validity (Den Uyl and Van Kuilenburg, 2005; Terzis et al., 2012) with the manually coded FACS ratings (Ekman and Rosenberg, 1997). FR reduces the time for behavioral coding without compromising accuracy and its use in psychological studies is increasing (Chentsova-Dutton and Tsai, 2010; He et al., 2014).

Stimulus Materials

Three different videos of mother–infant interactions were employed to activate three different representations of caregiving. The videos were chosen based on mothers' Adult Attachment Interview (AAI; George et al., 1985) classification, independently obtained. We selected the videos from a sample of mother infant free play (with standardized games) used in a previous study (Tagini et al., in preparation). The children in the selected videos were females of 23 months of age. Mothers' AAI classification was previously assessed and the mothers were selected to be one secure, one preoccupied and one dismissing. In addition, a further selection criteria was that all ECR-R (which was assessed as well) scores of mothers were consistent with their AAI classification. The secure mother had low anxiety (less than a standard deviation from the mean) and low avoidant scores, the preoccupied one was high in anxiety (more than a standard deviation from the mean) and the dismissing one was high on avoidance. After watching the three videos, 18 attachment experts (researchers and clinicians) answered the following questions for each video: “How much do you consider the caregiving behavior of this mother prototypic of a secure/preoccupied/dismissing mother? One is not prototypic at all and seven is very prototypic.” Results confirm that our stimuli has been chosen to be actually able to represent different caregiving styles and this association seems to be empirically supported.

Procedure

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Declaration oh Helsinki and the approval of the Ethical Committee of University of Milano—Bicocca. All subjects gave written informed consent in order to participate.

The first phase of the experiment consisted in evaluating the association between measures of attitude and participants' attachment style. The implicit and explicit measures were assessed for a first sample of 25 participants (females = 14) and subsequently their attachment style was evaluated with the ECR-R. Because no significant correlations were found, the presence of an effect of attachment style on attitudes during the second phase, could be interpreted a consequence of the experimental manipulation. The second phase consisted in a new, larger sample of female participants (N = 48), who watched the videos of mother-child interactions. In each session, after viewing the mother-child interaction video, the participants' attitude toward the category “mother” was measured, by means of both implicit (ST-IAT) and explicit (SD) measures. Thus, differences in attitudes could be attributed to having watched the different videos. The order of the videos was randomly generated to control learning effects on the ST-IAT. Three days was the minimum interval between one condition and the subsequent one. The choice to select only female participants was due to the characteristics of the videos, that represent mothers playing whit their daughters. This could make very difficult to interpret any possible gender difference in the data.

At the end of the first session, each participant was asked to fill out the ECR-R. The number of observations (N = 138) is slightly less than the maximum possible number (N = 144), because not all participants came three times to perform the task in the three different conditions. Participants who came just once were excluded from the analysis (N = 4).

While participants were watching the videos, their faces were video recorded in order to analyze their emotional expressions.

Data Analysis

We used R software (R Development Core Team, 2013) to analyze data. Correlations between the attachment measure and the ST-IAT and SD in the condition when no caregiving representation was activated (no video watched) were calculated, in order to control the possibility that the attitude toward the category “mother” depended on attachment style.

The effects of the experimental manipulation on ST-IAT and SD was tested with linear mixed models (LMMs). This allowed us to test within subjects experimental effects, considering attachment measures as covariates, and to perform repeated measure analyses, including those participants who were evaluated in only two conditions. The fixed effects were attachment style (anxiety and avoidance of attachment) and the type of condition (the vision of a video with a preoccupied/secure/dismissing mother). The outcome variable was the attitude toward the category “mother,” in one model it was the explicit measure (SD), and in the other model the implicit one (ST-IAT). We report all significant main effects and interactions (up to three way interactions).

The same predictors were used in a different model, with the Disgust measure of FaceReader Software as the outcome variable. Because of the very skewed distribution (skewness = 3.27 and kurtosis = 11.97) the outcome measure was recoded as a binomial variable (median split), in which 0 indicated a low Disgust mean and 1 a high Disgust mean. In this way, we could test a logistic mixed regression model, as part of the generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs).

Linear mixed models and GLMM were performed in R by using the package “lme4” (Bates et al., 2015) and bootstrapping all the confidence intervals. Degrees of freedom and p-values for the LMMs were computed via Kenward–Roger's approximations (F-tests) and Satterthwaite's approximations (t-tests) through the “lmerTest” package (Kuznetsova et al., 2014). The GLMM p values and degrees of freedom for the Chi square tests were computed via the likelihood ratio tests through the package “afex” (Singmann et al., 2014). Plots were built using the package “ggplot2” (Wickham, 2009).

Results

Correlation Between Attachment Style and Attitude Measures Without Manipulation

The first step was to test the correlation between the two dimensions of attachment style (avoidance, anxiety) and the two measures of attitude (ST-IAT, and SD), when no representation of caregiving was activated. This analysis was performed on the first sample (N = 25) that did not watch any video. The results indicate that there was no significant association, both for the ST-IAT (avoidance: r = 0.09; anxiety: r = −0.06) and for the SD (avoidance: r = −0.13; anxiety: r = −0.16). The non-significant correlations obtained allowed us to ignore the “baseline” condition (no experimental manipulation) in subsequent sample, in which it was therefore possible to consider the effect of attachment style as related to the experimental manipulation.

Effects of Attachment Style and Conditions on IAT

The first LMM model (within-subjects factor: type of conditions; covariates: anxiety and avoidance of attachment, centered on the means) showed no effect of the predictors (neither main effects nor interaction effects), on the attitude measured in an explicit way. The random terms were both intercept (var = 0.11) and condition (var = 0.26), because the model with both effects showed a significant improvement of fit [ΔBIC = −44.48, χ2(5) = 54.48, p < 0.001]. The same LMM model was performed with the ST-IAT scores as the outcome variable. The random term was just the intercept (var = 0.04), because the model with intercept and condition effects showed a not significant improvement of fit [ΔBIC = −17.89, χ2(5) = 6.71, p = 0.24]. IAT score was significantly affected by the type of Conditions [F(2, 83.20) = 5.83, p < 0.01] but not by Avoidance [F(1, 42.90) = 0.06, p = 0.80] and Anxiety [F(1, 43.41) = 0.45, p = 0.51]. The three two-ways interaction terms were the following: Conditions × Avoidance, F(2, 82.32) = 8.53, p < 0.001; Conditions × Anxiety, F(2, 82.82) = 7.86, p < 0.001; Avoidance × Anxiety F(1, 42.80) = 0.26, p = 0.61. The three-way interaction Condition × Anxiety × Avoidance was significant, F(2, 82.22) = 3.85, p < 0.05, suggesting that the effect of type of conditions (one of the three videos) was different for different levels of the two attachment style scales. To interpret this interaction we calculated the simple slope analysis: on the left panel of Figure 1 the effects of Avoidance are shown (Anxiety centered on the mean), while on the right panel of Figure 1 the effects of Anxiety are shown (Avoidance centered on the mean).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Results of the simple slope analysis of the ST-IAT model: effects of attachment styles and conditions. The left panel shows the effects of avoidance when Anxiety is centered on the mean, while the right panel refers to anxiety effects when Avoidance is centered on the mean.

When anxiety is centered on the mean, avoidance has a negative effect on implicit attitude after the Secure Video, b = −0.11, SE = 0.05, t(89.83) = −2.26, p < 0.05, 95% CI [−0.20, −0.004], a positive effect after the Dismissing Video, b = 0.10, SE = 0.05, t(90.54) = 2.02, p < 0.05, 95% CI [0.004, 0.189] and a negative not significant effect after the Preoccupied Video, b = −0.02, SE = 0.05, t(90.48) = −0.37, p = 0.71, 95% CI [−0.11, 0.81].

When avoidance is centered on the mean, anxiety has a negative effect on implicit attitude after the Secure Video, b = −0.10, SE = 0.05, t(92.77) = −2.00, p = 0.05, 95% CI [−0.21, −0.001], a negative not significant effect after the Dismissing Video, b = −0.07, SE = 0.05, t(90.85) = −1.42, p = 0.15, 95% CI [−0.17, 0.04] and a positive not significant effect after the Preoccupied Video, b = 0.09, SE = 0.05, t(90.64) = 1.84, p = 0.06, 95% CI [−0.0009, 0.01988].

To sum up, we found a positive effect of avoidance on IAT after the dismissing video and negative effects of both anxiety and avoidance after the secure video.

Effects of Attachment Style and Conditions on Emotion Expression

There was no significant association between Disgust variable and the explicit or implicit measures of attitude.

A third model with the same predictors and random effects was performed with the FaceReader measure of disgust as dichotomous outcome variable. The random term was just the intercept (var = 0.00) because the model with intercept and condition effects was not able to converge. The results showed a main effect of avoidance [χ2(13) = 4.46, p = 0.03] and no other significant main or interaction effect. The effect is positive, as it can be seen in Figure 2 that represents the effect of the three different conditions even if no one of them is significant if taken singularly. Secure Video: b = 0.43, SE = 0.36, z = 1.19, p = 0.23, 95% CI [−0.25, 1.20]; Dismissing Video: b = 0.33, SE = 0.31, z = 1.04, p = 0.30, 95% CI [−0.27, 0.99]; Preoccupied Video: b = 0.47, SE = 0.33, z = 1.42, p = 0.15, 95% CI [−0.14, 1.16].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Results of the simple slope analysis of the Disgust model: effects of attachment styles and conditions.

Discussion

The results of this study confirmed the adequacy of the experimental paradigm in activating a specific caregiving representation, and measuring the process of forming an attitude. We found a role of adult attachment style in shaping the implicit attitude, but non the explicit one, toward the category “mother.” The explicit attitude seems not to be influenced neither by the manipulation neither by participants' attachment style. This can be due to social desirability because the perception of the category “mother” is highly expected to be always good, or also to some kind of inability in being aware of a preference for a relational modality. On the contrary, the significance of the implicit attitude model could suggest the importance of the automatic processes of the IWMs. Participants are able to compare their relational expectations with the behavior they see in the videos, but they process this information in a not conscious way.

The results of the implicit attitude model are consistent with our expectations. In fact both Avoidance and Anxiety have a negative effects on IAT scores after the Secure Video (when the other variable is centered on the mean). This means that the more insecure a participant, the more negative her attitude toward the secure mother. On the contrary, avoidance has a positive effect on IAT scores after the Dismissing Video, but no effect after the Preoccupied one. This seems consistent with our hypotheses: very avoidant participants tend to prefer the Dismissing Video, while we had no predictions of a role of Avoidance on implicit attitude after the Preoccupied Video. Anxiety plays a similar role because it has a positive marginally significant effect on attitude after the Preoccupied Video and a negative not significant effect after the Dismissing Video.

The link between adult attachment style and the perception of different ways of caregiving can contribute to a better understanding of the mechanisms underlying the transmission of attachment. Avoidance or seeking of proximity in adult attachment seem to impact implicit processes of evaluation of caregiving modalities. The continuity between representations of partners and responses in new social encounters (Brumbaugh and Fraley, 2007) has a specific feature for what concerns caregiving relationships. From an evolutionary perspective, attachment schemata are influenced by the caregiving style of the parents. Thus, if an individual reaches the goal of reproduction, it is implied that the caregiving strategies have succeeded in that specific environment. In this sense it seems inexpensive to maintain a continuity between generations. It is consistent with the definition of IWMs that they regulate the access to information relevant to attachment, and our findings underline that the information relevant for the caregiving system is also closely related to attachment styles.

The explorative hypothesis of the effect of the experimental manipulation and attachment style on the facial expression of disgust while participants were watching the different videos was not confirmed. The only significant effect was a positive one of avoidance that was also unexpected because Magai et al. (2000) found an association between facial expression of disgust and “Preoccupation” during an emotion elicitation task. Anyway it seems reasonable that a specific kind of human relationship like the mother infant one elicits more negative physiological reaction in participant with higher levels of avoidance, although this was not our hypothesis. Finally, we found no association between disgust and the two measures of attitude, so we can claim that the process of forming attitudes is not explained by the elicitation of this specific emotion.

Conclusion

Adult attachment styles seem to play a moderating role on high level implicit attitudes toward caregiving but not on explicit attitudes or on low level regulation and expression of emotion. These findings underline the fundamental role of adult attachment style in predicting attitudes related to the caregiving system. Our hypotheses are only partially confirmed because we found a correspondence only between attachment styles and the IAT. Anyway, this specific result is in line with Bowlby's statements on the complementarity of the caregiving and attachment motivational systems. Furthermore, our study confirms his belief in the complexity of the effects involved. In fact, Bowlby (1988) wrote: “Parenting behavior, as I see it, has strong biological roots, which accounts for the very strong emotions associated with it; but the detailed form that the behavior takes in each of us turns on our experiences – experiences during childhood especially, experiences during adolescence, experiences before and during marriage, and experiences with each individual child” (p. 5). From a clinical point of view, these issues are of great interest since they may contribute to the process of the intergenerational transmission of attachment, and the passing on of disorders, considering that an insecure attachment style can become a risk factor for psychopathology (Mikulincer and Shaver, 2012). It has in fact been associated to internalizing and externalizing problems in adolescence (Sarracino et al., 2011) clinical disorders such as depression (Roberts et al., 1996; Cantazaro and Wei, 2010; Santona et al., 2015), anxiety disorders (Warren et al., 1997), and personality disorders (Meyer and Pilkonis, 2005; Crawford et al., 2007).

The limitations of this study must be acknowledged. In fact, research on adult attachment reports a “trivial to small” correspondence between adult attachment style questionnaires and state of mind with respect to attachment (Roisman et al., 2007). Then a limitation of our study is the use of different models of measure between stimuli and participants' assessment. AAI is indeed considered the “gold standard” for adult attachment assessment (Hesse, 2008) and differs from adult attachment styles questionnaires because of its implicit nature. AAI relies on different processes and measures coherence of mind with respect to attachment, which, by definition, differs from the explicit thoughts about attachment style. Finally the correspondence between an individual's attachment style and his preference for the matching caregiving modality could however not be directly tested, because of the attachment model underlying the measurement used, which differed from the stimulus categorization. On one hand the continuous measure of attachment is more effective (Fraley et al., 2015), on the other hand led to a more difficult interpretation of the results because the videos had to be categorized.

A future perspective could explore the moderating role of gender in evaluating caregiving representations. Although differences in attachment style usually do not emerge from studies based on AAI (Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn, 2009), it seems reasonable that males and females could have different attitudes toward caregiving behaviors. In an evolutionary view, males tend to be more facultative investors (Del Giudice, 2009) than females, probably because a low parental investment can be more adaptive in order to save resources for additional mating. Thus, regardless of the effect of attachment style, we could expected higher levels of positive attitudes when representations were activated in females, due to evolutionary differences.

Approaching the transmission gap issue, the evidence of the non-linear effect of maternal sensitivity in shaping children attachment led to focusing on the role of other mediating constructs, such as Reflective Function (Fonagy and Target, 1997; Slade et al., 2005). This study attempted to investigate, at a more basic level, the link between attachment style and caregiving representations and we found an effect that may be a starting point for further research and for interventions on parenting skills in risk situations. Our results confirm that cognition and behavior linked to caregiving rely on automatic processes (Bowlby, 1969/1982; Soltis, 2004; Bos et al., 2010; Riem et al., 2011; Swain et al., 2014). In this regard, a key research area may be the study of the maternal representations, in order to understand how this caregiving attitude emerge and develop in pregnancy and early motherhood (Stern, 1995; Innamorati et al., 2010). It seems that every kind of intervention should face these implicit processes that do not reach a conscious level of elaboration. Finally, the results may be relevant to psychotherapy, considering that the interconnection between representations and the perception of relational contexts is a key concept in those contexts that focus on pointing out specific non-adaptive representations and reflecting on which specific environment allowed its development.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S. M., and Stayton, D. (1974). “Infant-mother attachment and social development,” in The Introduction of the Child into a Social World, ed. M. P. Richards (London: Cambridge University Press), 99–135.

PubMed Abstract

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Atkinson, L., Leung, E., Goldberg, S., Benoit, D., Poulton, L., Myhal, N., et al. (2009). Attachment and selective attention: disorganization and emotional Stroop reaction time. Dev. Psychopathol. 21, 99–126. doi: 10.1017/S0954579409000078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2009). No reliable gender differences in attachment across the lifespan. Behav. Brain Sci. 32, 22–23. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0900003X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin, M. W., Fehr, B., Keedian, E., Seidel, M., and Thomson, D. W. (1993). An exploration of the relational schemata underlying attachment styles: self-report and lexical decision approaches. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 19, 746–754. doi: 10.1177/0146167293196010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin, M. W., Keelan, J. P. R., Fehr, B., Enns, V., and Koh-Rangarajoo, E. (1996). Social-cognitive conceptualization of attachment working models: availability and accessibility effects. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71, 94. doi: 10.1037/00223514.71.1.94

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banse, R., and Kowalick, C. (2007). Implicit attitudes towards romantic partners predict well-being in stressful life conditions: evidence from the antenatal maternity ward. Int. J. Psychol. 42, 149–157. doi: 10.1080/00207590601067037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartholomew, K., and Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: a test of a four-category model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 61, 226–244. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.61.2.226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartz, J. A., and Lydon, J. E. (2004). Close relationships and the working self-concept: implicit and explicit effects of priming attachment on agency and communion. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30, 1389–1401. doi: 10.1177/0146167204264245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumeister, R. F., and Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J. (2005). “The developmental and evolutionary psychology of intergenerational transmission of attachment,” in Attachment and Bonding: A New Synthesis, eds C. S. Carter, L. Ahnert, K. E. Grossmann, S. B. Hrdy, M. E. Lamb, S. W. Porges, and N. Sachser (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 169–198.

Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., and Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: an evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Dev. 62, 647–670. doi: 10.2307/1131166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernier, A., Matte-Gagné, C., Bélanger, M.-É., and Whipple, N. (2014). Taking stock of two decades of attachment transmission gap: broadening the assessment of maternal behavior. Child Dev. 85, 1852–1865. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bos, P. A., Hermans, E. J., Montoya, E. R., Ramsey, N. F., and van Honk, J. (2010). Testosterone administration modulates neural responses to crying infants in young females. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 114–121. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment Loss, Vol. 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.

Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and Loss, Vol. 3: Loss, Sadness, and Depression. New York: Basic Books.

Bowlby, J. (1988). A Secure Base: Clinical Applications of Attachment Theory, Vol. 393. London: Routledge.

Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., and Shaver, P. R. (1998). “Self-report measurement of adult attachment: an integrative overview,” in Attachment Theory and Close Relationships, eds J. A. Simpson and W. S. Rholes (New York: Guilford Press), 46–76.

PubMed Abstract

Brennan, K. A., and Shaver, P. R. (2002). Attachment styles and personality disorders: their connections to each other and to parental divorce, parental death, and perceptions of parental caregiving. J. Pers. 66, 835–878. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bretherton, I. (1987). “New perspectives on attachment relations: security, communication, and internal working models,” in Handbook of Infant Development, 2nd Edn, eds I. Bretherton and J. D. Osofsky (Oxford: John Wiley & Sons), 1061–1100.

Bretherton, I. (1999). Updating the “internal working model” construct: some reflections. Attach. Hum. Dev. 1, 343–357. doi: 10.1080/14616739900134191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brumbaugh, C. C., and Fraley, R. C. (2007). Transference of attachment patterns: how important relationships influence feelings toward novel people. Pers. Relationsh. 14, 513–530. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2007.00169.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bus, A. G., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Affective dimension of mother-infant picturebook reading. J. School Psychol. 35, 47–60. doi: 10.1016/S0022-4405(96)00030-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Busonera, A., Martini, P. S., Zavattini, G. C., and Santona, A. (2014). Psychometric properties of an italian version of the Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised (ECR-R) Scale. Psychol. Rep. 114, 785–801. doi: 10.2466/03.21.PR0.114k23w9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldwell, J. G., Shaver, P. R., Li, C.-S., and Minzenberg, M. J. (2011). Childhood maltreatment, adult attachment, and depression as predictors of parental self-efficacy in at-risk mothers. J. Aggress. Maltreat. Trauma 20, 595–616. doi: 10.1080/10926771.2011.595763

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cantazaro, A., and Wei, M. (2010). Adult attachment, dependence, self criticism, and depressive symptoms: a test of a mediational model. J. Pers. 78, 1135–1162. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00645.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carnelley, K. B., and Rowe, A. (2007). Repeated priming of attachment security influences later views of self and relationships. Pers. Relationsh. 14, 307–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2007.00156.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, H. A., and Anderson, A. K. (2013). Things rank and gross in nature: a review and synthesis of moral disgust. Psychol. Bull. 139, 300–327. doi: 10.1037/a0030964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, H. A., Kim, D. A., Susskind, J. M., and Anderson, A. K. (2009). In bad taste: evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science 323, 1222–1226. doi: 10.1126/science.1165565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chentsova-Dutton, Y. E., and Tsai, J. L. (2010). Self-focused attention and emotional reactivity: the role of culture. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 507–519. doi: 10.1037/a0018534

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Clore, G. L., and Schnall, S. (2005). “The influence of affect on attitude,” in The Handbook of Attitudes, eds D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, and M. P., Zanna (Mahwah: Erlbaum), 437–489.

Crawford, T. N., Livesley, W. J., Jang, K. L., Shaver, P. R., Cohen, P., and Ganiban, J. (2007). Insecure attachment and personality disorder: a twin study of adults. Eur. J. Pers. 21, 191–208. doi: 10.1002/per.602

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Giudice, M. (2009). Sex, attachment, and the development of reproductive strategies. Behav. Brain Sci. 32, 1–21; discussion 21–67. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x09000016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Den Uyl, M. J., and Van Kuilenburg, H. (2005). “The facereader: online facial expression recognition,” in Proceedings of Measuring Behavior 5th International Conference on Methods and Techniques in Behavioural Research, Wageningen, 589–590.

Google Scholar

Dewitte, M., De Houwer, J., and Buysse, A. (2008). On the role of the implicit self-concept in adult attachment. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 24, 282–289. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.24.4.282

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewitte, M., Koster, E. H. W., De Houwer, J., and Buysse, A. (2007). Attentive processing of threat and adult attachment: a dot-probe study. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 1307–1317. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dykas, M. J., and Cassidy, J. (2011). Attachment and the processing of social information across the life span: theory and evidence. Psychol. Bull. 137, 19–46. doi: 10.1037/a0021367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P. (1992). Facial expressions of emotion: an old controversy and new findings. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 335, 63–69. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1992.0008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., and Rosenberg, E. L. (1997). What the Face Reveals: Basic and Applied Studies of Spontaneous Expression Using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Fairchild, A. J. (2006). Investigating validity evidence for the experiences in close relationships-revised questionnaire. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 66, 116–135. doi: 10.1177/0013164405278564

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., and Target, M. (1997). Attachment and reflective function: their role in self-organization. Dev. Psychopathol. 9, 679–700. doi: 10.1017/S0954579497001399

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., and Target, M. (2005). Bridging the transmission gap: an end to an important mystery of attachment research? Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 333–343. doi: 10.1080/14616730500269278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraley, R. C. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adulthood: meta-analysis and dynamic modeling of developmental mechanisms. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6, 123–151. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0602_03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraley, R. C., Hudson, N. W., Heffernan, M. E., and Segal, N. (2015). Are adult attachment styles categorical or dimensional? A taxometric analysis of general and relationship-specific attachment orientations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 109, 354–368. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., and Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item response theory analysis of self-report measures of adult attachment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 350–365. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.350

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gawronski, B., and Payne, B. K. (2011). Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

George, C., Kaplan, N., and Main, M. (1985). Adult Attachment Interview. Berkeley: University of Caligfornia Press, Berkeley.

George, C., and Solomon, J. (2008). “The caregiving system: a behavioral systems approach to parenting,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 2nd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York: Guilford Press), 833–856.

Gillath, O., Selcuk, E., and Shaver, P. R. (2008). Moving toward a secure attachment style: can repeated security priming help? Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 2, 1651–1666. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00120.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., and Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1464–1480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., and Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 197–216. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., and Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 17–41. doi: 10.1037/a0015575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grienenberger, J. F., Kelly, K., and Slade, A. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning, mother-infant affective communication, and infant attachment: exploring the link between mental states and observed caregiving behavior in the intergenerational transmission of attachment. Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 299–311. doi: 10.1080/14616730500245963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossmann, K., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Rudolph, J., and Grossmann, K. E. (1988). “Maternal attachment representations as related to patterns of infant-mother attachment and maternal care during the first year,” in Relationships Within Families, eds R. A. Hinde and J. Stevenson-Hinde (Oxford: Oxford Science Publications), 241–260.

Hautamäki, A., Hautamäki, L., Neuvonen, L., and Maliniemi-Piispanen, S. (2010). Transmission of attachment across three generations: continuity and reversal. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 15, 347–354. doi: 10.1177/1359104510365451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, W., Boesveldt, S., de Graaf, C., and de Wijk, R. A. (2014). Dynamics of autonomic nervous system responses and facial expressions to odors. Front. Psychol. 5:110. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hesse, E. (2008). “The adult attachment interview: protocol, method of analysis, and empirical studies,” in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 2nd Edn, eds J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (New York: Guilford Press), 552–598.

Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., and Alford, J. R. (2014). Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology. Behav. Brain Sci. 37, 297–307. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13001192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, K. S. (2010). Paternal attachment, parenting beliefs and children's attachment. Early Child Dev. Care 180, 157–171. doi: 10.1080/03004430903415031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inbar, Y., and Pizarro, D. (2014). Disgust, politics, and responses to threat. Behav. Brain Sci. 37, 315–316. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13002598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Innamorati, M., Sarracino, D., and Dazzi, N. (2010). Motherhood constellation and representational change in pregnancy. Infant. Mental Health J. 31, 379–396. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inquisit (2013). Inquisit 3.0 [Computer Software]. Seattle, WA: Millisecond Software.

Jones, J. D., Brett, B. E., Ehrlich, K. B., Lejuez, C. W., and Cassidy, J. (2014). Maternal attachment style and responses to adolescents' negative emotions: the mediating role of maternal emotion regulation. Parent. Sci. Pract. 14, 235–257. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2014.972760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, J. D., Cassidy, J., and Shaver, P. R. (2015a). “Adult attachment style and parenting,” in Attachment Theory and Research: New Directions and Emerging Themes, eds J. A. Simpson and W. S. Rholes (New York, NY: Guilford Publications), 234.

Jones, J. D., Cassidy, J., and Shaver, P. R. (2015b). Parents' self-reported attachment styles: a review of links with parenting behaviors, emotions, and cognitions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 19, 44–76. doi: 10.1177/1088868314541858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kazui, M., Endo, T., Tanaka, A., Sakagami, H., and Suganuma, M. (2000). Intergenerational transmission of attachment: Japanese mother–child dyads. Jap. J. Educ. Psychol. 48, 323–332. doi: 10.5926/jjep1953.48.3_323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilmann, P. R., Vendemia, J. M. C., Parnell, M. M., and Urbaniak, G. C. (2009). Parent characteristics linked with daughters' attachment styles. Adolescence 44, 557–568. Available online at: https://scholar.google.it/scholar?hl=it&q=Parent+characteristics+linked+with+daughters%E2%80%99+attachment+styles&btnG=&lr=

PubMed Abstract

Kohlhoff, J., and Barnett, B. (2013). Parenting self-efficacy: links with maternal depression, infant behaviour and adult attachment. Early. Hum. Dev. 89, 249–256. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2013.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., and Christensen, R. H. B. (2014). lmerTest: tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models (lmer objects of lme4 package). R package version 2.0-6. Available online at: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmerTest

Leerkes, E. M., and Siepak, K. J. (2006). Attachment linked predictors of women's emotional and cognitive responses to infant distress. Attach. Hum. Dev. 8, 11–32. doi: 10.1080/14616730600594450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lench, H. C., Quas, J. A., and Edelstein, R. S. (2006). My child is better than average: the extension and restriction of unrealistic optimism. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 36, 2963–2979. doi: 10.1111/j.0021-9029.2006.00137.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magai, C., Hunziker, J., Mesias, W., and Culver, L. C. (2000). Adult attachment styles and emotional biases. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 24, 301–309. doi: 10.1080/01650250050118286

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Main, M., Kaplan, N., and Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy, childhood, and adulthood: a move to the level of representation. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 50, 66. doi: 10.2307/3333827

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malhotra, N. K. (2005). Attitude and affect: new frontiers of research in the 21st century. J. Bus. Res. 58, 477–482. doi: 10.1016/S0148-2963(03)00146-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMahon, C. A., Barnett, B., Kowalenko, N. M., and Tennant, C. C. (2006). Maternal attachment state of mind moderates the impact of postnatal depression on infant attachment. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 660–669. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01547.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., de Rosnay, M., Arnott, B., Leekam, S. R., and Turner, M. (2012). Mind-mindedness as a multidimensional construct: appropriate and nonattuned mind-related comments independently predict infant-mother attachment in a socially diverse sample. Infancy 17, 393–415. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00087.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Fradley, E., and Tuckey, M. (2001). Rethinking maternal sensitivity: mothers' comments on infants' mental processes predict security of attachment at 12 months. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 637–648. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, B., and Pilkonis, P. A. (2005). “An attachment model of personality disorders,” in Major Theories of Personality Disorder, eds M. F. Lenzenweger and J. F. Clarkin (New York, NY: Guilford), 231–281.

Mikulincer, M. (1998). Adult attachment style and individual differences in functional versus dysfunctional experiences of anger. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 513. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikulincer, M., and Shaver, P. R. (2012). An attachment perspective on psychopathology. World Psychiatry 11, 11–15. doi: 10.1016/j.wpsyc.2012.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Gillath, O., and Nitzberg, R. A. (2005). Attachment, caregiving, and altruism: boosting attachment security increases compassion and helping. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 89, 817. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.5.817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nathanson, A. I., and Manohar, U. (2012). Attachment, working models of parenting, and expectations for using television in childrearing. Family Relations 61, 441–454. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2012.00701.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noldus. (2013). FaceReader: Tool for Automatic Analysis of Facial Expression: Version 5.0.15. Wageningen: Noldus Information Technology B. V.

Okagaki, L., and Bingham, G. E. (2005). “Parents' social cognitions and their parenting behaviors,” in Parenting: An Ecological Perspective, Vol. 2, eds T. Luster and L. Okagaki (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 3–33.

Google Scholar

Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., and Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The Measurement of Meaning, Vol. 47. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.

Google Scholar

Petty, R. E., DeSteno, D., and Rucker, D. D. (2001). “The role of affect in attitude change,” in Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition, ed. J. P. Forgas (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 212–233.

R Development Core Team (ed.). (2013). The R Project for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Raby, K. L., Steele, R. D., Carlson, E. A., and Sroufe, L. A. (2015). Continuities and changes in infant attachment patterns across two generations. Attach. Hum. Dev. 17, 414–428. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2015.1067824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J. A., Kohn, J. L., Wilson, C. L., Martin, A. M., Tran, S., et al. (2011). Attachment orientations and depression: a longitudinal study of new parents. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 567–586. doi: 10.1037/a0022802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richetin, J., Costantini, G., Perugini, M., and Schönbrodt, F. (2015). Should we stop looking for a better scoring algorithm for handling implicit association test data? Test of the role of errors, extreme latencies treatment, scoring formula, and practice trials on reliability and validity. PLoS ONE 10:e0129601. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riem, M. M. E., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Pieper, S., Tops, M., Boksem, M. A. S., Vermeiren, R. R. J. M., et al. (2011). Oxytocin modulates amygdala, insula, and inferior frontal gyrus responses to infant crying: a randomized controlled trial. Biol. Psychiatry 70, 291–297. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, J. E., Gotlib, I. H., and Kassel, J. D. (1996). Adult attachment security and symptoms of depression: the mediating roles of dysfunctional attitudes and low self-esteem. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 310–320. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.2.310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roisman, G. I., Holland, A., Fortuna, K., Fraley, R. C., Clausell, E., and Clarke, A. (2007). The Adult Attachment Interview and self-reports of attachment style: an empirical rapprochement. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 92, 678–697. doi: 10.1037/00223514.92.4.678

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, A., and Carnelley, K. B. (2003). Attachment style differences in the processing of attachment-relevant information: primed-style effects on recall, interpersonal expectations, and affect. Per. Relationsh. 10, 59–75. doi: 10.1111/14756811.00036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sagi, A., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Scharf, M., Joels, T., Koren-Karie, N., and Aviezer, O. M. O. (1997). Ecological constraints for intergenerational transmission of attachment. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 20, 287–299. doi: 10.1080/016502597385342

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santona, A., De Carli, P., Pace, C. S., Parolin, L., Sarracino, D., Tagini, A., et al. (2015). Maternal depression and attachment: the evaluation of mother–child interactions. Front. Psychol. 6:1235. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarracino, D., Presaghi, F., Degni, S., and Innamorati, M. (2011). Sex-specific relationships among attachment security, social values, and sensation seeking in early adolescence: implications for adolescents' externalizing problem behaviour. J. Adolesc. 34, 541–554. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scharf, M., and Mayseless, O. (2011). Buds of parenting in emerging adult males: what we learned from our parents. J. Adolesc. Res. 26, 479–505. doi: 10.1177/0743558411402339

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect Regulation and the Origin of the Self: The Neurobiology of Emotional Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Shaver, P. R., and Mikulincer, M. (2013). “Patterns of relating and of thinking about relationships,” in Human Bonding: The Science of Affectional Ties, eds C. Hazan and M. I. Campa (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 251.

Shlafer, R. J., Raby, K. L., Lawler, J. M., Hesemeyer, P. S., and Roisman, G. I. (2015). Longitudinal associations between adult attachment states of mind and parenting quality. Attach. Hum. Dev. 17, 83–95. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2014.962064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sibley, C. G., Fischer, R., and Liu, J. H. (2005). Reliability and validity of the revised experiences in close relationships (ECR-R) self-report measure of adult romantic attachment. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 31, 1524–1536. doi: 10.1177/0146167205276865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singmann, H., Bolker, B., Højsgaard, S., Fox, J., Lawrence, M. A., Mertens, U., et al. (2014). afex: Analysis of Factorial Experiments. R package version 0.15-2. Available online at: ftp://www.r-project.org/pub/R/web/packages/afex/

Slade, A., Grienenberger, J., Bernbach, E., Levy, D., and Locker, A. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning, attachment, and the transmission gap: a preliminary study. Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 283–298. doi: 10.1080/14616730500245880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soltis, J. (2004). The signal functions of early infant crying. Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 443–458. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x0400010x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sroufe, L. A., and Jacobvitz, D. (1989). Diverging pathways, developmental transformations, multiple etiologies and the problem of continuity in development. Hum. Dev. 32, 196–203. doi: 10.1159/000276468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, D. N. (1995). The Motherhood Constellation. New York: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Swain, J. E., Kim, P., Spicer, J., Ho, S. S., Dayton, C. J., Elmadih, A., et al. (2014). Approaching the biology of human parental attachment: brain imaging, Oxytocin and coordinated assessments of mothers and fathers. Brain Res. 1580, 78–101. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarabulsy, G. M., Bernier, A., Provost, M. A., Maranda, J., Larose, S., Moss, E., et al. (2005). Another look inside the gap: ecological contributions to the transmission of attachment in a sample of adolescent mother-infant dyads. Dev. Psychol. 41, 212–224. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teige-Mocigemba, S., Klauer, K. C., and Sherman, J. W. (2010). “A practical guide to implicit association tests and related tasks,” in Handbook of Implicit Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications, Vol. 2, eds B. Gawronski and B. K. Payne (New York, NY: Guilford), 117–139.

Terzis, V., Moridis, C. N., and Economides, A. A. (2012). The effect of emotional feedback on behavioral intention to use computer based assessment. Comput. Educ. 59, 710–721. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2012.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., and DeScioli, P. (2013). Disgust: evolved function and structure. Psychol. Rev. 120, 65–84. doi: 10.1037/a0030778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1995a). Adult attachment representations, parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: a meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the Adult Attachment Interview. Psychol. Bull. 117, 387–403. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1995b). Of the way we were: on temperament, attachment, and the transmission gap: a rejoinder to Fox (1995). Psychol. Bull. 117, 411–415. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van IJzendoorn, M. H., and Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1997). “Intergenerational transmission of attachment: a move to the contextual level,” in Attachment and Psychopathology, eds L. Atkinson and K. J. Zucker (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 135–170.

Warren, S. L., Huston, L., Egeland, B., and Sroufe, L. (1997). Child and adolescent anxiety disorders and early attachment. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36, 637–644. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199705000-00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waters, H. S., and Waters, E. (2006). The attachment working models concept: among other things, we build script-like representations of secure base experiences. Attach. Hum. Dev. 8, 185–197. doi: 10.1080/14616730600856016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weed, K., and Nicholson, J. S. (2015). Differential social evaluation of pregnant teens, teen mothers and teen fathers by university students. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 20, 1–16. doi: 10.1080/02673843.2014.963630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westen, D. (1991). Social cognition and object relations. Psychol. Bull. 109, 429–455. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.3.429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickham, H. (2009). ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York, NY: Springer Science & Business Media. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-98141-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wigboldus, D. H. J., Holland, R. W., and Van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Single target implicit associations. Unpublished Manuscript.

PubMed Abstract

Wilson, C. L., Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J. A., and Tran, S. (2007). Labor, delivery, and early parenthood: an attachment theory perspective. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 33, 505–518. doi: 10.1177/0146167206296952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zayas, V., and Shoda, Y. (2005). Do automatic reactions elicited by thoughts of romantic partner, mother, and self relate to adult romantic attachment? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 31, 1011–1025. doi: 10.1177/0146167204274100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: adult attachment, implicit measure, caregiving, intergenerational transmission, internal working model

Citation: De Carli P, Tagini A, Sarracino D, Santona A and Parolin L (2016) Implicit Attitude Toward Caregiving: The Moderating Role of Adult Attachment Styles. Front. Psychol. 6:1906. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01906

Received: 26 May 2015; Accepted: 25 November 2015;
Published: 07 January 2016.

Edited by:

Silvia Salcuni, Universitá degli Studi di Padova, Italy

Reviewed by:

Paola Miano, Università degli Studi di Palermo, Italy
Alessandro Talia, University of Copenhagen, Denmark

Copyright © 2016 De Carli, Tagini, Sarracino, Santona and Parolin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Laura Parolin, laura.parolin@unimib.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.