Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol., 05 January 2016
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Dynamic of Sensori-motor Interactions in Embodied Cognition View all 19 articles

Editorial: Dynamics of Sensorimotor Interactions in Embodied Cognition

  • 1Department of Psychology, Centre de Recherche de l'Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal, Montreal, Canada
  • 2Laboratoire EPSYLON, Department of Psychology, Université Paul-Valéry Montpellier III, Montpellier, France
  • 3Laboratoire d'Etude des Mécanismes Cognitifs, Department of Psychology, Université Lyon 2, Lyon, France
  • 4Psychological Sciences Research Institute, Université Catholique de Louvain, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium

The concept of incorporating the current situation and the body state within cognitive processes, referred to as embodiment, has revolutionized cognitive research (Glenberg et al., 2013). Interest in this approach has grown substantially in the last few decades. Embodied cognition has now been demonstrated across a wide range of topics, from babies (e.g., Smith and Gasser, 2005) to elderly adults (e.g., Vallet et al., 2013a), from normal cognition to neuropsychology (e.g., Vallet et al., 2013b) as well as in emotion (e.g., Vermeulen et al., 2007), and in neuroscience as a whole (e.g., Pulvermüller, 2013). Nevertheless, there is yet much to discover in order to better understand embodiment.

One of the striking arguments of embodiment is that sensori-motor features are at the core of mental processes (Pecher and Zwaan, 2005; Barsalou, 2010). Their relationship should also be considered to be dynamic. In the embodied cognition approaches, perception, memory, and action are no longer regarded as (relatively) independent functions, but rather as closely interacting components. Some authors even argue for an overlap within these processes (Brunel et al., 2009, 2015; Vermeulen et al., 2009) that would rely on the same neural code (e.g., Hommel). One key direction in embodiment that needs to be further explored is the dynamic interaction between the sensory and motor components across the different cognitive functions. The present Research Topic aims to shed new light on this issue.

The importance of studying this problematic is well presented in Dijkstra and Post's critical review. Their article highlights the crucial role of sensorimotor simulation across several cognitive activities (e.g., reasoning, evaluating) and how the interaction between the current situation and the simulation of past sensorimotor states mediates the emergence of adaptive behavior. This is possible since perception and memory interact closely and share processes and resources (e.g., Vermeulen et al., 2008; Riou et al., 2011; Rey et al., 2014). In other words, “direct” perception of an object and the mental simulation of this object involve common sensorimotor units. Heurley and Ferrier argue in their perspective that these interactions serve to plan and control actions to interact with well-known objects in non-optimal perceptual conditions, thus producing adaptive behavior.

The idea that cognitive activities are strongly influenced by the given social and physical environment is referred to as situated cognition. Rey et al. demonstrate that motor simulation relies on the relevance of the object as a function of the task. Further evidence also supports the hypothesis that sensorimotor simulation is heavily influenced by situational aspects (Kapoula et al.). For instance, Amorim et al. have demonstrated that the visual angle and screen orientation along with contextual information modulates the resulting movement.

The reverse is also applicable, since sensorimotor simulation shapes our perception of, as well as our interaction with, our environment. Grade et al. found that similar action simulation underlies both reachability and egocentric distance perception. This effect can be generalized across individuals and possibly incorporate the role of social environment in cognition. In accordance, Quesque and Coello show that during reach-to-grasp actions, participants unconsciously modify their trajectory curvature based on their partner's eye level. This adaptation to biological motion is one the key component of social interaction, but human movements are so complex that they might be described as mathematically chaotic. Nonetheless, children quickly develop the ability to coordinate gaze, and in some respect posture, in response to complex chaotic motion structures (Haworth et al.).

The dynamics of sensorimotor interactions allows for situational behavioral adaptation in reaching and grasping, but also in more complex evaluative processes. For instance, associations between handedness and valence have previously been found (e.g., Casasanto, 2011). de la Vega et al. extend this finding by showing that strong right-footers respond to positive words faster with the dominant foot. The association between valence and laterality, however, becomes less clear when motor fluency is taken into account. Brouillet et al. observe that most participants prefer choosing stable supports for “good” items, regardless of side and handedness. The space-valence association could also be reported for the vertical axis. Xie et al. demonstrate that the processing of affective valence concepts activated the vertical spatial axis (positive in the up position).

The association between cognition and space in the context of emotion is well documented, but it is not restricted to this cognitive domain. Hartmann et al. report that simple arithmetics are associated with gaze shifting along the vertical axis. Interestingly, operand magnitude partly modulated horizontal gaze position as well. The reciprocal influence of motor components on numerical cognition may also provide the opportunity to assess numerical cognition with methods such as mouse tracking. In their perspective, Fischer and Hartmann point out important insight and methodological considerations on conceptual aspects related to numeric cognition. This perspective has been further elaborated by Faulkenberry and Rey.

These recent reports offer a new perspective on cognitive functioning, one that combines sensorimotor dynamics with contextual and body information. These new concepts provide new opportunities to explore related domains such as motivation (Shalev) and anticipation (Raab) and offer a new framework to interpret well-known and sometimes contradictory results in fields such as short-term memory (Macken et al.) or healthy cognitive aging (Vallet).

Author Contributions

Every author participates to the writing of the editorial article. All of them also significantly contribute to the edition of the articles of the research topic.

Funding

GV is supported by a postdoctoral grant from the Fonds Québécois de la Recherche en Santé (FRQS). LB is supported by ACCEPT (“Assistance Tools and Cognitive Contribution: Embodied Potential of Technology”), French research ministerial mission (MiRe-DREES and CNSA). BR is supported by FS-CH (Formation Universitaire à distance, Suisse) and this work by the LabEx Cortex (“Construction, Function, and Cognitive Function and Rehabilitation of the Cortex,” ANR-10-LABX-0042) of Université de Lyon, within the program “Investissements d'Avenir” (ANR-11-IDEX-0007) operated by the French National Research Agency (ANR).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Kristina Aurousseau for her precious help and meticulous proofreading as well as all authors and reviewers involved in the present research topic.

References

Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded cognition: past, present, and future. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2, 716–724. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunel, L., Carvalho, P. F., and Goldstone, R. L. (2015). It does belong together: cross-modal correspondences influence cross-modal integration during perceptual learning. Front. Psychol. 6:358. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brunel, L., Labeye, E., Lesourd, M., and Versace, R. (2009). The sensory nature of episodic memory: sensory priming effects due to memory trace activation. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 35, 1081–1088. doi: 10.1037/a0015537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casasanto, D. (2011). Different bodies, different minds: the body specificity of language and thought. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20, 378–383. doi: 10.1177/0963721411422058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glenberg, A. M., Witt, J. K., and Metcalfe, J. (2013). From the revolution to embodiment: 25 years of cognitive psychology. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 573–585. doi: 10.1177/1745691613498098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pecher, D., and Zwaan, R. A. (2005). Grounding Cognition: The Role of Perception and Action in Memory, Language, and Thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 458–470. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rey, A. E., Riou, B., and Versace, R. (2014). Demonstration of an Ebbinghaus illusion at a memory level: manipulation of the memory size and not the perceptual size. Exp. Psychol. 61, 378–384. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riou, B., Lesourd, M., Brunel, L., and Versace, R. (2011). Visual memory and visual perception: when memory increase visual search. Mem. Cognit. 39, 1094–1102. doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0075-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, L., and Gasser, M. (2005). The development of embodied cognition: six lessons from babies. Artif. Life 11, 13–29. doi: 10.1162/1064546053278973

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallet, G. T., Hudon, C., Simard, M., and Versace, R. (2013b). The disconnection syndrome in the Alzheimer's disease: the cross-modal priming example. Cortex 49, 2402–2415. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallet, G. T., Simard, M., Versace, R., and Mazza, S. (2013a). The perceptual nature of audiovisual interactions for semantic knowledge in young and elderly adults. Acta Psychol. 143, 253–260. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermeulen, N., Niedenthal, P. M., and Luminet, O. (2007). Switching between sensory and affective systems incurs processing costs. Cogn. Sci. 31, 183–192. doi: 10.1080/03640210709336990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermeulen, N., Corneille, O., and Niedenthal, P. M. (2008). Sensory load incurs conceptual processing costs. Cognition 109, 287–294. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermeulen, N., Mermillod, M., Godefroid, J., and Corneille, O. (2009). Unintended embodiment of concepts into percepts: Sensory activation boosts attention for same-modality concepts in the attentional blink paradigm. Cognition 112, 467–472. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: embodied cognition, grounded cognition, situated cognition, sensorimotor interactions, memory, action, perception, emotion

Citation: Vallet GT, Brunel L, Riou B and Vermeulen N (2016) Editorial: Dynamics of Sensorimotor Interactions in Embodied Cognition. Front. Psychol. 6:1929. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01929

Received: 27 November 2015; Accepted: 30 November 2015;
Published: 05 January 2016.

Edited and reviewed by: Bernhard Hommel, Leiden University, Netherlands

Copyright © 2016 Vallet, Brunel, Riou and Vermeulen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Guillaume T. Vallet, gtvallet@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.