Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 21 March 2016
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

The Mad Genius Stereotype: Still Alive and Well

  • Institute of Psychology, Educational and Psychological Assessment, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany

Scientists and laypeople agree on high ability as a defining feature of giftedness. Yet their views on gifted people's socioemotional characteristics diverge. Most studies find the gifted to be similar or slightly superior to average-ability persons in these domains (“harmony hypothesis”). However, subjective conceptions and media representations, most of which have focused on gifted children and youth, stress the socioemotional downsides of giftedness (“disharmony hypothesis”), affecting highly able individuals and those around them, thus hampering individual development. To date, most studies on gifted stereotypes have examined selective samples, mostly teachers. The present study is the first to provide representative data on conceptions of gifted individuals in general. A brief survey of 1029 German adults assessed quality and prevalence of stereotypes about gifted individuals, without an explicit focus on children and/or adolescents. Latent class analysis (LCA) revealed two conceptions of giftedness, with twice as many “disharmonious” than “harmonious” raters. Male gender, single parenthood, unemployment, higher income or negative attitudes toward the gifted predicted disharmonious ratings. However, effects were small, suggesting future studies look deeper into the processes of stereotype formation and maintenance.

Introduction

Imagine two kids: one popular and well-adjusted, the other one uncomfortable around people and often unhappy. If you knew one of them were gifted—who would it rather be?

If you picked the sad loner, you are in good company. Individuals ascribe negative socioemotional characteristics to students described as gifted, such as isolation (Solano, 1987), lower agreeableness, higher introversion, and neuroticism (Baudson and Preckel, 2013). Although such stereotypes have little to do with actual characteristics of the gifted, they form a reality in people's minds and shape how they perceive and behave toward gifted individuals.

Dimensions and Conceptions of Giftedness

When talking about giftedness and gifted individuals, we should keep in mind that there is no universally acknowledged definition of “gifted.” Giftedness is a social construct and thus depends on the cultural, historical, and social context it is used in. Furthermore, we should be aware that its definition is inseparable from the reason why we define it. Different dimensions of and approaches to defining giftedness will be outlined in the following.

Potential vs. Achievement

In line with investment theory (e.g., Cattell, 1971), people differ in terms of their innate intellectual potential, or “fluid intelligence.” Similar to seed capital, potential can be “invested” into learning and the acquisition of so-called “crystallized intelligence,” e.g., knowledge (Schweizer and Koch, 2002). Because investment opportunities are related to age, children's giftedness is more likely to be defined in terms of potential, whereas gifted adults are rather judged in terms of their achievements.

Status vs. Development

Time is but one factor of influence in talent development; and although potential and achievement are positively related, their correlation is by no means perfect. For instance, intelligence and grades correlate to about 0.50 (Jensen, 1998). Whether potential is fully developed into achievement depends on both intrapersonal and environmental catalysts, i.e., the personality characteristics and circumstances that support talent development (e.g., Gagné, 2004). Whereas static conceptions of giftedness pursue a “once gifted, always gifted” approach, developmental conceptions assume that giftedness may change over the lifespan—for the better or for the worse.

Uni- vs. Multidimensionality

Giftedness may be defined by outstanding intellectual abilities only or include further dimensions (e.g., creative, psychomotor, or social abilities). However, most models of giftedness and talent agree on high intellectual ability as a characteristic feature of giftedness. Whereas early conceptions considered IQ measures to be sufficient to identify gifted children (e.g., Terman, 1925), more recent models of giftedness include other dimensions besides intellectual ability, too (e.g., Gagné, 2004).

Qualitative or Quantitative Differences

Empirical research suggests that the difference between “gifted” and “non-gifted” is quantitative rather than qualitative. Development of gifted students is accelerated, but follows similar trajectories as in average-ability students (e.g., Threlfall and Hargreaves, 2008). With respect to intellectual ability, common cut-off criteria such as “two standard deviations above the mean” are motivated statistically and not due to qualitative differences. In contrast, lay theories seem to perceive “the gifted” as a social group, thus as a distinct category, although this does justice neither to the rather quantitative differences between gifted and average-ability individuals nor to the heterogeneity of the gifted as a group (e.g., Achter et al., 1996).

In sum, the definition of giftedness depends on the purpose of this very definition. Whereas scientific theories strive to identify measurable characteristics of the gifted and integrate them into coherent theoretical frameworks, laypeople usually rely on subjective theories that are based on personal and media experiences with gifted individuals and may or may not be in line with scientific findings.

Gifted Stereotypes

The term “gifted” evokes stereotypes—images in our heads, based on beliefs about this group. In line with the above paragraph on qualitative vs. quantitative differences, Dai (2010) argues that rather than using sets of abstract characteristics to define and identify gifted individuals, people represent the gifted as prototypes. These include, but are not limited to, “high flyers” like Hermione Granger, who succeed at virtually anything; “mad geniuses” like Camille Claudel, whose sanity was the price she paid for being an outstanding sculptor; misunderstood “brilliant rebels” like Good Will Hunting, who do not actualize their potential (cf. Cox, 2000); or “geeks/nerds” like Sherlock Holmes, who are characterized by intellectual brilliance and complete lack (or disregard) of social abilities alike (see Cross, 2005, for a discussion of changing conceptions of geeks/nerds).

Across media types, the gifted are portrayed rather negatively, too. On television, they are underrepresented; generally, TV seems to provides few adequate role models for gifted children and adolescents (Abelman, 1992). A classic example from print is the “Sidis fallacy,” (Kett, 1978) “the wrongheaded, but widespread, idea that child prodigies grow into unproductive adults” (Bates, 2011, p. 375). Popular culture often portrays gifted students as nonathletic, unpopular, studying rather than having fun, and often female (Vialle, 2007). Contrarily, an analysis of the diachronic construction of giftedness in Estonian media showed that since the 2000s, giftedness is constructed as “the property of a product, a basis for success,” with a focus on individuals (particularly children) and sports, politics, or pop music (Põlda, 2015, p. 234). Apparently, Cox's (2000) recommendation to assume a critical stance of gifted students' media representations is perfectly justified.

Research on gifted stereotypes runs along two lines: the disharmony hypothesis (rooted in the “mad genius” myth; Becker, 1978; Gallagher, 1990; Neihart, 1999)—high ability implying deficits especially in socioemotional domains; and the harmony hypothesis, assuming in its strong form that gifted people excel at virtually anything (e.g., Terman, 1925; Mönks, 1963; Persson, 1998). Basically, these hypotheses can be broken down to two fundamental dimensions: one pertaining to potential, achievement, and related constructs, and one referring to socioemotional characteristics. Indeed, these two dimensions have been found to be crucial in intergroup perception. The Stereotype Content Model has termed these dimensions “warmth” and “competence” (Fiske et al., 2002, 2007; see Abele and Wojciszke, 2007, for further labels of this fundamental distinction). Warmth refers to people's intent, which can be either positive or negative, whereas competence refers to their ability to pursue this intent. Both dimensions are assumed to have developed in response to evolutionary pressures (Fiske et al., 2007). As the authors further point out, the two dimensions are likely to correlate moderately positively when individuals are judged, but may be independent in the judgment of groups. This corresponds well to the disharmony hypothesis, where the gifted are considered high in competence but low in warmth, thus representing an “envious stereotype” in terms of Fiske et al. (2002).

In sum, both harmony and disharmony hypothesis agree that gifted individuals are characterized by high potential and achievement (“competence”). Their distinguishing feature is their view on socioemotional abilities (“warmth”) of the gifted. Whereas the harmony hypothesis assumes similarity between gifted and average-ability individuals or even superiority of the gifted, the disharmony hypothesis conceives of the gifted as socially and emotionally inferior.

Gifted stereotypes are problematic because they shape gifted individuals self-perception. “Stigma of giftedness” theory posits that stereotyping may lead gifted students to either hide their potential to avoid identification with the stigma, or to overidentify with the label by adopting stereotypical characteristics (Coleman and Cross, 1988; Cross, 2005). However, stereotypes also affect others' perceptions and actions. For instance, Pajares (1992) showed that teachers' conceptions of gifted students shape expectations and, subsequently, students' classroom behavior and educational goals. Negative gifted stereotypes may therefore lead to potential remaining uncovered, underdeveloped, and misunderstood.

Empirical Findings about the Gifted

But what does the empirical literature on actual differences between gifted and average-ability individuals say? As often, the truth lies somewhere in the middle, with a slant toward “harmony.” Regarding the “competence” dimension, we find that on average, gifted individuals are indeed superior in intellectual potential, achievement, and related characteristics. This includes better grades (Roznowski et al., 2000), greater openness to experience (DeYoung, 2011), higher academic self-concept and self-esteem (Roznowski et al., 2000), higher adaptive perfectionism (Parker, 1997; LoCicero and Ashby, 1999), lower performance anxiety (Richards et al., 2003), and higher educational aspirations (Roznowski et al., 2000). For the “warmth” dimension describing socioemotional characteristics, evidence favors great similarity between gifted and average-ability individuals. The gifted are no more prone to depression, anxiety, or suicide (Reis and Renzulli, 2004; Martin et al., 2010), show similar levels of wellbeing and stress (Zeidner and Shani-Zinovich, 2011), and are as agreeable as average-ability persons (Schilling, 2009; DeYoung, 2011), conscientiousness (Ackerman and Heggestad, 1997), and social abilities (Schilling, 2009; overview: Neihart et al., 2002).

Summary and Rationale for the Present Study

In sum, the gifted are characterized by (1) higher intellectual potential and (2), to a lesser extent, higher achievement. Differences favoring the gifted are usually too weak to suggest (3) their general superiority (e.g., Neihart et al., 2002). Contrary to media allegations, the gifted differ neither in (4) emotional stability nor (5) social relationships.

Research on gifted stereotypes suffers from nonrepresentative samples, often teachers (e.g., Endepohls-Ulpe and Ruf, 2005; Geake and Gross, 2008; Baudson and Preckel, 2013; Preckel et al., 2015). The “stigma of giftedness” (Coleman and Cross, 1988) is largely unexplored in gifted adults. Most of what we know about gifted stereotypes thus relates to teachers and students; the quality and prevalence of gifted stereotypes in representative samples are still unknown. Also, characteristics of people holding different stereotypes (e.g., “giftedness = socioemotional difficulties”) have not been identified, though they represent relevant starting points for changing misconceptions. One should also keep in mind that not only the vast majority of research, but also most of the counseling literature and most representations of the gifted in the popular media have focused on gifted children and adolescents, which may likely influence people's conceptions of giftedness.

With these questions in mind, I surveyed a representative German sample (1029 adults) about their ideas about the gifted. A brief questionnaire included five core aspects of gifted stereotypes: (1) higher potential, (2) higher achievement, (3) general superiority, (4) emotional problems, and (5) social issues. Latent class analysis (LCA) differentiated groups with characteristic “rater profiles” by maximizing both homogeneity within and heterogeneity between groups. Theory suggested two profiles: “harmonious” vs. “disharmonious,” differing in ratings of emotional and social problems, but comparable in potential, achievement, and general superiority ratings, with a possible third “extremely harmonious” profile assuming general superiority in the gifted. Logistic regression identified rater characteristics (i.e., statistical predictors of latent class membership), examined exploratorily for lack of relevant research.

Methods

Sample

The sample was representative with respect to age, gender, and regional distribution for German adults between 18 and 69 years of age. Data were collected with the help of INNOFACT AG, a provider specialized in representative surveys. Altogether, 1029 adults (50.5% female; mean age = 43.85 years, SD = 14.31) took part in the study.

Materials

The questionnaire included 10 demographical items about the participants' background: gender, age, federal country of Germany the participants lived in, level of education, current occupational status, monthly net income, family status, and household size, including information about children under 18. The remaining nine questions addressed opinions about gifted persons along the five core dimensions described above: intellectual potential, achievement, social difficulties, emotional issues, and superiority in other domains beside intellect (5 items, rated on 5-point Likert scales from “do not agree at all” to “absolutely agree”). Furthermore, participants rated their own intelligence (5-point scale from “substantially below average” to “substantially above average”), the feelings the term “giftedness” evoked in them (4-point scale from “very negative” to “very positive”), their interest in giftedness (4-point scale from “not interested at all” to “very interested”), and whether they knew any gifted persons (1 item each). The option “cannot/do not want to answer” was provided with all items.

Procedure

The 10 demographical items were a standard part of the omnibus survey. The nine giftedness-related questions were compiled by the author and the strategic development team of the high-IQ society Mensa in Germany. Data were collected as part of the weekly online omnibus survey of a marketing research institute specialized in these services. The survey was funded by Mensa in Germany as part of their strategic development and their efforts to support scientific research on giftedness. The author analyzed the data using SPSS 22.0.0.1 (IBM Corp., 2013; descriptives) and Mplus 7.11 (Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2012; latent class analyses).

Because no clear-cut criteria have yet been proposed to decide on the number of latent classes, a combination of indices was used (Nylund et al., 2007; Geiser, 2011). Criteria included (1) theoretical soundness, the simplest theoretically sound solution being two classes, namely, the “harmonious” vs. the “disharmonious” view; (2) parsimony, which would exclude solutions with highly similar classes; (3) average classification probability, which should exceed 0.80 (Geiser, 2011); (4) entropy (a global measure of how reliable the classification is, 1.00 being the maximum), (5) information criteria such as AIC, BIC, and sample-size adjusted BIC; and (6) statistical tests assessing whether increasing the number of classes improves fit. Here, the Vuong-Lo-Mendell-Rubin Likelihood Ratio Test (VLMR LRT), the Lo-Mendell-Rubin adjusted Likelihood Ratio Test (LMR LRT) and the Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Test (BLRT) were used, which allow for a direct comparison between neighboring solutions (k vs k-1 classes).

Results

Descriptives

Descriptives are given in Table 1. Overall, participants rated gifted persons as somewhat more positive than negative. However, correlations (Table 2) showed that positively (potential, achievement) and negatively connoted characteristics (social and emotional issues) are positively (though weakly) correlated.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Gifted ratings and attitudes toward giftedness.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Intercorrelations amongst respondents' ratings of the gifted (single-item measures).

Latent-Class Analysis

To examine whether this pattern held across the entire sample, or whether different subgroups of raters should be differentiated, an LCA was performed. Table 3 provides an overview of the fit indices for solutions with different numbers of classes. With respect to theoretical foundation (harmony vs. disharmony hypothesis), parsimony and distinctness of classes, average probability of correct classification, and results of the statistical tests (VLMR, LMR, and BLRT), a two-class solution was deemed to fit the data best. Also, the decline in relative fit indices (AIC, BIC, adjusted BIC) was sharpest between the one- and the two-class solution. Entropy was the only criterion disfavoring the two-class solution. Figure 1 provides a graphical representation of the average ratings for the two-class solution.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Latent class analysis results.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. “Harmonious” and “disharmonious” stereotypes. Rater profiles of the “harmonious” class (dotted line), which ascribes high potential, high achievement, no pronounced superiority and average ratings on social and emotional issues to gifted persons, and the “disharmonious” class (solid line), which rates gifted persons similar in potential, achievement, and superiority, but high in negative social and emotional characteristics.

Predictors of Class Membership

I further examined which variables predict class membership using binary logistic regression (Table 4). Men were 1.39 times more likely to attribute a “disharmonious” profile (i.e., greater social and emotional issues) to gifted individuals. While age had no influence, family status did: Compared to singles without kids, single parents were more than three times more likely to perceive the gifted in line with the disharmony hypothesis. With respect to the raters' educational and professional situation, unemployed individuals were 2.81 times more likely to associate giftedness with the disharmonious profile, compared to raters employed fulltime. Except for the highest income group (≥3800 €), the likelihood of rating gifted persons as disharmonious increased with income, but only reached significance in the 2500–3800 € group. In contrast, educational level had a marginal influence only. Of the giftedness-related variables, only positive emotions toward giftedness (about 21% lower probability of disharmonious ratings per step in the more “positive” direction) predicted class membership, whereas interest in giftedness was tendentially related to a greater probability of disharmonious ratings (OR = 1.23, p = 0.06). Neither knowing a gifted person nor self-rated intelligence level had any significant influence.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Binary logistic regression results—predictors of group membership (0 = “harmonious,” 1 = “disharmonious” raters).

Classification accuracy improved little through stepwise inclusion of the predictors. The null model classified 67.5% of all cases correctly, which did not change when demographical characteristics were included, and increased only by 0.3% after inclusion of professional background variables. Giftedness-related variables did not impact the absolute classification accuracy, yet led to more “harmonious” raters being classified correctly. Absolute figures were still small, though. The changes in percentage with each regression step were 0/0/3.9/7.0% for the “harmonious” group, compared to 100/100/98.5/97% for the “disharmonious” group. The Hosmer-Lemeshow test exceeded the 10% probability level in all cases, suggesting that the null hypothesis (i.e., the model fits the data) should not be discarded.

Discussion

Summary of the Findings

Conceptions of giftedness come in two shapes: harmony, characterized by high potential and achievement ratings, and disharmony, comparable in potential, achievement, and general superiority ratings, but ascribing more social and emotional problems to the gifted. This study provides the first representative picture of how people in a Western society conceive of the gifted. The survey covered five crucial aspects of gifted stereotypes. Partially, results align with research revealing disharmonious conceptions of giftedness in teachers (Baudson and Preckel, 2013; Preckel et al., 2015). However, this is the first study to quantify both negative and positive stereotypes, showing that 2/3 of the respondents hold a negative stereotype. Though some demographic and psychological predictors were identified, much variance remained unexplained.

Bakan's “Duality of Human Existence” Revisited

It seems surprising that gifted stereotypes are no more complex than this. However, their underlying structure aligns perfectly with Fiske et al.'s (2007) warmth vs. competence dimensions of intergroup perception. As mentioned above, the disharmonious gifted stereotype would represent an “envious” stereotype, comparable to other highly able, but cold groups who have the capacity to pursue their intent, but whose intent is not necessarily in line with the community's (e.g., career women or Germans; Fiske et al., 2002). In contrast, the harmonious gifted stereotype, which is characterized by high competence and at least “normal” warmth, might be considered a reference group, a “normative standard for social comparison and most often, social aspiration” (Cuddy et al., 2009, p. 6). In individualistic societies like Germany, these reference groups are usually located in the “competent and warm” quadrant, which would fit the results presented here.

As participants were supposed to rate the gifted on five characteristics relating to gifted stereotypes, the findings can also be related to the “Big Two” of personality: agency (here: potential/achievement) and communion (here: socioemotional issues, or lack thereof; Bakan, 1966). The Big Five forming these two “superfactors” are also reflected directly in the items characterizing the gifted. For agency, openness (the dimension most strongly associated with intelligence) may be considered part of “higher potential,” whereas “higher achievement” comprises conscientiousness, success usually requiring effort. For community, neuroticism and agreeableness/extraversion are represented by proneness to emotional issues and social difficulties, respectively1.

A “strongly harmonious” gifted stereotype including general superiority (Terman, 1925) was not identified, suggesting that a realistic image of high competence and at least average warmth is as good as it can get for the gifted. Because the gifted are indeed superior in some agentic aspects (potential, achievement), Paulhus and John's (1998) “superhero bias” (i.e., exaggerated self-ascribed agency) is not reflected in gifted stereotypes. There is no evidence of any “saint” bias either, as neither of the two gifted stereotypes comprises superior community.

Understanding Predictors of Gifted Stereotypes

The low variance explained suggests that future studies should look deeper into the mechanisms of stereotyping. E.g., people intimidated by the gifted's undisputably higher potential might devalue their (imaginary) competitors by ascribing them negative socioemotional characteristics, thus coping with perceived threat and inferiority, which might explain why unemployment predicts disharmonious ratings. That the same relationship was identified for males and higher income groups corroborates this interpretation: Genders respond differently to intergroup threat and competition (Van Vugt et al., 2007; Sutter and Rützler, 2010). This fits well into the two-dimensional frameworks of self- and other-perception outlined above. Competitiveness, intergroup/ego threat, perceived injustice, envy, and sense of entitlement are therefore promising predictors to examine in future studies. Considering that the “envious” stereotype may also be a consequence of perceiving others as both highly competent and cold, it might also be interesting to examine possible reciprocal effects between stereotyping of and emotions toward certain groups longitudinally, hereby also differentiating different emotions beyond the rather coarse positive/negative associations with the term used here.

Knowing a gifted person did not influence whether a participant rated the gifted as harmonious or disharmonious. This is notable with respect to the slight positive halo effect Fiske et al. (2007) report for the judgment of individuals and allows for diverse interpretations. It is possible that respondents who know “harmonious” and “disharmonious” gifted individuals are about evenly distributed, thus canceling each other out. An alternative explanation is that despite the greater visibility of disharmonious gifted individuals, raters are nevertheless able to make a balanced judgment and not overgeneralize their personal experiences. Future studies may want to look deeper into respondents' actual experiences with gifted individuals to identify differential effects.

Single parents more readily ascribe socioemotional difficulties to the gifted. Children of divorce are less adjusted (meta-analyses: Amato and Keith, 1991; Amato, 2001), probably due to conflicts preceding divorce (Hetherington, 1999). Additionally, parents overrate their offspring's intelligence (e.g., Miller et al., 1991). Speculatively, parents might misattribute socioemotional problems to their children's (assumed) giftedness, a question worth pursuing.

Self-rated intelligence was unrelated to latent class membership, suggesting highly intelligent persons (= gifted by IQ) are no more likely to see their ingroup as overproportionally positive. In line with the stereotype content model, this allows for at least two interpretations. First, harmonious giftedness might be an ideal to aspire to, but the harmoniously gifted may not be considered part of one's ingroup. Second, it is conceivable that though high intelligence is a characteristic of both implicit and explicit theories of giftedness (e.g., Baudson and Preckel, 2013), it is not sufficient to define giftedness, such that even highly intelligent raters would not necessarily identify themselves as gifted. Possibly, this is due to the stigma of giftedness: Highly intelligent individuals might refuse the label “gifted” because a large majority associates the term with negative characteristics. This has interesting implications for identity development in minorities (e.g., Frable, 1997), a topic yet underexamined in giftedness research.

Though largely representative, our data span Germany only, which likely impacts the content of stereotypes (e.g., Cuddy et al., 2009). Also, the value of effort vs. innate ability (Dweck, 2007) or the representation of gifted programs in the media (Karnes and Lewis, 1995) may affect both quality and quantity of gifted stereotypes internationally. Research should address these questions, also to develop culture-sensitive interventions.

Calling for Action

As all countries scarce in natural resources, Germany depends on brainpower. Yet many Western countries fail to cater for their most able students' educational needs (Colangelo et al., 2004; Assouline et al., 2015). One reason may be that two thirds of the population perceive the gifted as socioemotionally problematic, possibly even dangerous to the community when low social orientation meets high intelligence (Geake and Gross, 2008). Possibly, fostering the gifted is perceived as unfair in two ways: because the gifted would be able to help themselves (high ascribed potential and achievement); and because later benefits for society are uncertain (low ascribed community). This question touches on fundamental social values—who deserves support, and who does not?—, which should be understood and clarified interdisciplinarily before taking action.

Stereotypes shape the perception of reality. That negative gifted stereotypes are twice as common as realistic ones makes the disharmonious “outgroup” perspective (likely acquired through others passing on the stereotype) as comprehensible as the “ingroup”'s reluctance to label themselves as gifted. The negative gifted stereotype's ambivalence (amalgamating positive and negative ascriptions) should therefore be covered when addressing the gifted's socioemotional needs (e.g., Peterson, 2008).

Stereotypes may also shape actions. Misconceptions of giftedness should therefore be remedied (which is hard, but not impossible; e.g., Schack and Starko, 1990; Hoogeveen et al., 2005). Mere exposure is unlikely to succeed: Knowing gifted persons did not impact people's conceptions of giftedness here. A deeper involvement tackling the underlying dynamics of stereotyping thus seems more promising.

Popular media creating and perpetuating stereotypes should use their power responsibly, even if this clashes with economic interests. Scientists should enter the public discourse to counterbalance misrepresentations critically.

Conclusion

Accurate knowledge about giftedness may translate into greater awareness, hint at possible remedies, and eventually improve conditions for our most talented citizens. The challenge to convey this knowledge should be accepted concertedly by researchers, practitioners, and the media. Keeping high ability and socioemotional deficits apart would help gifted people accept and actualize their potential—for their own sake and for that of a caring and supportive society.

Author Contributions

TGB provided the theoretical background, developed major parts of the questionnaire, analyzed the data and wrote the paper.

Funding

Data collection was funded by Mensa in Germany, e. V.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The author wishes to thank Mensa in Germany e. V. for funding the present study and for granting the author full liberty in analyzing the data. Special thanks is due to the Mensa Strategy Team for their input in developing the questionnaire. Furthermore, the author is indebted to the two reviewers for their positive and constructive comments which certainly improved the paper.

Footnotes

1. ^Considering the generality of these two core dimensions, it is conceivable that the incompatibility between high potential and success on the one hand and socioemotional difficulties on the other is not limited to the gifted, but possibly extends to high achievers in general, as one reviewer pointed out. As such, the stereotype might be interpreted as a general human desire for fairness. The design of our study does not allow to rule out this possibility. However, I would like to offer a few speculative thoughts on why giftedness may more than mere high ability in intergroup perception. First, not all high abilities are created equal. As Geake and Gross (2008) point out, psychomotor prowess and musical abilities are valued because of their direct usefulness to the group. In contrast, high verbal ability has the potential to destroy the existing order, especially when coupled with low regard for others. But even if we limit “high ability” to “high intellectual ability,”, the qualifier “high” suggests that a highly able person is to be found somewhere in the upper tail of the ability continuum. One would rather speak of low achievement or intelligence than of *non-achievement or *non-intelligence. In contrast, “gifted” versus “non-gifted” or “average-ability” persons would rather suggest a categorical distinction.

References

Abele, A. E., and Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of the self versus others. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 751–763. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abelman, R. (1992). Some Children Under Some Conditions: TV and the High Potential Kid. Storrs, CT: The National Research Center on the Gifted and Talented. Available online at: http://nrcgt.uconn.edu/wp-content/uploads/sites/953/2015/03/rbdm9206.pdf

Achter, J. A., Lubinski, D., and Benbow, C. P. (1996). Multipotentiality among the intellectually gifted: “It was never there and already it's vanishing.” J. Couns. Psychol. 43, 65–76. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.43.1.65

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ackerman, P. L., and Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and interests: evidence for overlapping traits. Psychol. Bull. 121, 219–245. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.2.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amato, P. R. (2001). Children of divorce in the 1990s: an update of the Amato and Keith (1990) meta-analysis. J. Family Psychol. 15, 355–370. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.15.3.355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amato, P. R., and Keith, B. (1991). Parental divorce and the well-being of children: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 110, 26–46. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.26

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Assouline, S. G., Colangelo, N., VanTassel-Baska, J., and Lupkowski Shoplik, A. (eds.). (2015). A Nation Empowered, Vol. 2. Iowa City, IA: The Connie Belin & Jacqueline N. Blank International Center for Gifted Education and Talent Development.

Bakan, D. (1966). The Duality of Human Existence. Isolation and Community in Western Man. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.

Bates, S. (2011). The prodigy and the press: William James Sidis, anti-intellectualism, and standards of success. J. Mass Commun. Q. 88, 374–397. doi: 10.1177/107769901108800209

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baudson, T. G., and Preckel, F. (2013). Teachers' implicit personality theories about the gifted. School Psychol. Q. 28, 37–46. doi: 10.1037/spq0000011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, G. (1978). The Mad Genius Debate. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their Structure, Growth, And Action. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Google Scholar

Colangelo, N., Assouline, S., and Gross, M. (2004). A Nation Deceived. Iowa City, IA: The Connie Belin & Jacqueline N. Blank International Center for Gifted Education and Talent Development.

Google Scholar

Coleman, L. J., and Cross, T. L. (1988). Is being gifted a social handicap? J. Educ. Gift. 11, 41–56. doi: 10.1177/0162353214521486

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, J. (2000). Amadeus to young Einstein: modern cinema and its portrayal of gifted learners. Gift. Child Today 23, 14–19. doi: 10.4219/gct-2000-712

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross, T. L. (2005). Nerds and geeks: society's evolving stereotypes of our students with gifts and talents. Gift. Child Today 28, 26–27, 65. doi: 10.1177/107621750502800406

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., Kwan, V. S. Y., Glick, P., Demoulin, S., Leyens, J. P., et al. (2009). Stereotype content model across cultures: towards universal similarities and some differences. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 48, 1–33. doi: 10.1348/014466608X314935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dai, D. Y. (2010). The Nature and Nurture of Giftedness. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.

Google Scholar

DeYoung, C. G. (2011). “Intelligence and personality,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence, eds R. J. Sternberg and S. B. Kaufman (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 711–737.

Dweck, C. S. (2007). Mindset. The New Psychology of Success. New York, NY: Ballantine Books.

Endepohls-Ulpe, M., and Ruf, H. (2005). Primary school teachers' criteria for the identification of gifted pupils. High Abil. Stud. 16, 219–228. doi: 10.1080/13598130600618140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., and Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: warmth and competence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 77–83. doi: 10.1016/j-tics.2006.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., and Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 878–902. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.82.6.878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frable, D. E. S. (1997). Gender, racial, ethnic, sexual, and class identities. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 48, 139–162. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagné, F. (2004). Transforming gifts into talents: the DMGT as a developmental theory. High Abil. Stud. 15, 119–147. doi: 10.1080/1359813042000314682

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, J. J. (1990). Editorial: the public and professional perception of the social and emotional status of gifted children. J. Educ. Gift. 13, 202–211. doi: 10.1177/016235329001300302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geake, J. G., and Gross, M. U. M. (2008). Teachers' negative affect towards academically gifted students: an evolutionary psychological study. Gift. Child Q. 52, 217–231. doi: 10.1177/0016986208319704

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geiser, C. (2011). Datenanalyse Mit Mplus [Data analysis using Mplus]. Wiesbaden: Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften.

Hetherington, E. M. (1999). “Should we stay together for the sake of thec hildren?” in Coping with Divorce, Single Parenting, and Remarriage, ed E. M. Hetherington (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 93–116.

Google Scholar

Hoogeveen, L., van Hell, J. G., and Verhoeven, L. (2005). Teacher attitudes toward academic acceleration and accelerated students in the Netherlands. J. Educ. Gift. 29, 30–59. doi: 10.1177/016235320502900103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IBM Corp. (2013). IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh, Version 22.0.0.1. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g Factor. Westport, CT: Praeger.

Google Scholar

Karnes, F. A., and Lewis, J. (1995). Examining the media coverage of gifted education. Gift. Child Today 18, 28–30.

Google Scholar

Kett, J. F. (1978). Curing the disease of precocity. Am. J. Sociol. 84, S183–S211. doi: 10.1086/649240

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LoCicero, K. A., and Ashby, J. S. (1999). Multidimensional perfectionism in middle school age gifted students: a comparison to peers from the general cohort. Roeper Rev. 22, 182–185. doi: 10.1080/02783190009554030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, L. T., Burns, R. M., and Schonlau, M. (2010). Mental disorders among gifted and nongifted youth: a selected review of the epidemiologic literature. Gift. Child Q. 54, 31–41. doi: 10.1177/0016986209352684

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, S. A., Manhal, M., and Mee, L. L. (1991). Parental beliefs, parental accuracy, and children's cognitive performance: a search for causal relations. Dev. Psychol. 27, 267–276. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mönks, F. J. (1963). Beiträge zur Begabtenforschung im Kindes- und Jugendalter [Contributions to the research on giftedness in childhood and adolescence]. Arch. Gesamte Psychol. 115, 362–382.

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (1998–2012). Mplus User's Guide, 7th Edn., Los Angeles, CA: Muthén Muthén.

Neihart, M. (1999). The importance of giftedness and psychological wellbeing: what does the empirical literature say? Roeper Rev. 22, 10–17. doi: 10.1080/02783199909553991

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neihart, M., Reis, S. M., Robinson, N. M., and Moon, S. M. (eds.). (2002). The Social and Emotional Development of Gifted Children: What Do We Know? Waco, TX: Prufrock Press.

Google Scholar

Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., and Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: a Monte-Carlo simulation study. Struct. Eqn. Model. 14, 535–569. doi: 10.1080/10705510701575396

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pajares, M. F. (1992). Teachers' beliefs and educational research: cleaning up a messy construct. Rev. Educ. Res. 62, 307–322. doi: 10.2307/1170741

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, W. D. (1997). An empirical typology of perfectionism in academically talented children. Am. Educ. Res. J. 34, 545–562. doi: 10.3102/00028312034003545

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulhus, D. L., and John, O. P. (1998). Egoistic and moralistic biases in self-perception: the interplay of self-deceptive styles with basic traits and motives. J. Pers. 66, 1025–1060. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Persson, R. S. (1998). Paragons of virtue: teachers' conceptual understanding of high ability in an egalitarian school system. High Abil. Stud. 9, 181–196. doi: 10.1080/1359813980090204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, J. S. (2008). The Essential Guide to Talking with Gifted Teens. Minneapolis, MN: Free Spirit.

Google Scholar

Põlda, H. (2015). The linguistic construction of the giftedness discourse in the media texts of historical and digital times. Eston. Pap. Appl. Linguist. 11, 223–239. doi: 10.5128/ERYa11.14

CrossRef Full Text

Preckel, F., Baudson, T. G., Krolak-Schwerdt, S., and Glock, S. (2015). Gifted and maladjusted? Implicit attitudes and automatic associations related to gifted children. Am. Educ. Res. J. 52, 1160–1184. doi: 10.3102/0002831215596413

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reis, S. M., and Renzulli, J. S. (2004). Current research on the social and emotional development of gifted and talented students: good news and future possibilities. Psychol. Sch. 41, 119–129. doi: 10.1002/pits.10144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, J., Encel, J., and Shute, R. (2003). The emotional and behavioral adjustment of intellectually gifted adolescents: a multi-dimensional, multi-informant approach. High Abil. Stud. 14, 153–164. doi: 10.1080/1359813032000163889

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roznowski, M., Reith, J., and Hong, S. (2000). A further look at youth intellectual giftedness and its correlates: values, interests, performance, and behavior. Intelligence 28, 87–113. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00032-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schack, G. D., and Starko, A. J. (1990). Identification of gifted students: an analysis of criteria preferred by preservice teachers, classroom teachers, and teachers of the gifted. J. Educ. Gift. 13, 346–363. doi: 10.1177/016235319001300405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schilling, S. (2009). “Peer-beziehungen [Peer relationships],” in Hochbegabte und Hochleistende Jugendliche, 2nd Edn. ed D. H. Rost (Münster: Waxmann), 367–421.

Schweizer, K., and Koch, W. (2002). A revision of Cattell's investment theory: cognitive properties influencing learning. Learn. Individ. Differ. 13, 57–82. doi: 10.1016/S1041-6080(02)00062-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solano, C. H. (1987). Stereotypes of social isolation and early burnout in the gifted: do they still exist? J. Youth Adolesc. 16, 527–539. doi: 10.1007/BF02138819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutter, M., and Rützler, D. (2010). “Gender differences in competition emerge early in life,” in IZA Discussion Paper, No. 5015 (Bonn: IZA).

Terman, L. M. (1925). Genetic Studies of Genius. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Threlfall, J., and Hargreaves, M. (2008). The problem-solving methods of mathematically gifted and older average-attaining students. High Abil. Stud. 19, 83–98. doi: 10.1080/13598130801990967

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Vugt, M., De Cremer, D., and Janssen, D. P. (2007). Gender differences in cooperation and competition. Psychol. Sci. 18, 19–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01842.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vialle, W. (2007). Pink or Paris? Giftedness in the popular culture. Aust. J. Gift. Educ. 16, 5–11. Available online at: http://ro.uow.edu.au/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=2222&context=edupapers

PubMed Abstract

Zeidner, M., and Shani-Zinovich, I. (2011). Do gifted and non-gifted students differ on the big-five and adaptive status? Some recent data and conclusions. Personal. Indiv. Diff. 51, 566–570. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: giftedness, harmony hypothesis, disharmony hypothesis, gifted stereotypes, social perception, big two, warmth and competence, stigma of giftedness

Citation: Baudson TG (2016) The Mad Genius Stereotype: Still Alive and Well. Front. Psychol. 7:368. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00368

Received: 06 January 2016; Accepted: 29 February 2016;
Published: 21 March 2016.

Edited by:

Jeremy A. Frimer, University of Winnipeg, Canada

Reviewed by:

Kurt Gray, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, USA
Diego Fernandez-Duque, Villanova University, USA

Copyright © 2016 Baudson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tanja G. Baudson, tanja.baudson@uni-due.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.