Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 03 May 2016
Sec. Cognition

Physical Activity, Sleep, and Nutrition Do Not Predict Cognitive Performance in Young and Middle-Aged Adults

  • 1Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Welten Institute—Research Centre for Learning, Teaching and Technology, Open University of the Netherlands, Heerlen, Netherlands
  • 2eLearn Center, Universitat Oberta de Catalunya, Barcelona, Spain
  • 3Department of Complex Genetics, Faculty of Health, Medicine and Life Sciences, School for Nutrition, Toxicology and Metabolism, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands

Biological lifestyle factors (BLFs) such as physical activity, sleep, and nutrition play a role in cognitive functioning. Research concerning the relation between BLFs and cognitive performance is scarce however, especially in young and middle-aged adults. Research has not yet focused on a multidisciplinary approach with respect to this relation in the abovementioned population, where lifestyle habits are more stable. The aim of this study was to examine the contribution of these BLFs to cognitive performance. Path analysis was conducted in an observational study in which 1131 adults were analyzed using a cross-validation approach. Participants provided information on physical activity, sedentary behavior, chronotype, sleep duration, sleep quality, and the consumption of breakfast, fish, and caffeine via a survey. Their cognitive performance was measured using objective digital cognitive tests. Exploration yielded a predictive cohesive model that fitted the data properly, χ2/df = 0.8, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA < 0.001, SRMR = 0.016. Validation of the developed model indicated that the model fitted the data satisfactorily, χ2/df = 2.75, CFI = 0.95, RMSEA < 0.056, SRMR = 0.035. None of the variables within the BLFs were predictive for any of the cognitive performance measures, except for sedentary behavior. Although sedentary behavior was positively predictive for processing speed its contribution was small and unclear. The results indicate that the variables within the BLFs do not predict cognitive performance in young and middle-aged adults.

Introduction

Cognitive performance is influenced, amongst other things, by physical activity (e.g., active vs. passive behavior; Burkhalter and Hillman, 2011; Voss et al., 2011), sleep (e.g., sleep duration and quality; Dean et al., 2010; Philip et al., 2012), and nutrition (e.g., breakfast and poly-unsaturated fatty acids; Burkhalter and Hillman, 2011). A comprehensive perspective of behavior within these three biological lifestyle factors (BLFs) and their relation with cognitive performance in adults is needed for a number of reasons. Healthy behavior concerning these controllable environmental factors has been proven to result in better physical and mental health (Busch et al., 2013), but also leads to better cognitive performance (Small et al., 2006). Previous research has focused mainly on children and adolescents however, and solely on separate domains or even variables within these domains (e.g., only chronotype: Vollmer et al., 2013). To our knowledge there is no research focusing on the combination of these three BLFs and the relation with cognitive performance in young and middle-aged adults, an age group in which lifestyle habits are more stable in comparison to their younger counterparts. Therefore, the goal of the current study was to evaluate whether subjectively measured general habitual behavior on the three BLFs physical activity, sleep, and nutrition was predictive for cognitive performance in young and middle-aged adults using path analysis.

Physical activity, sleep, and nutrition all exert influence on the regulation of the body's physiological parameters. For example, the levels of brain-derived neurotropic factor (BDNF) increase when being physically active (Winter et al., 2007), hereby enhancing the proliferation, synaptic plasticity, growth and survival of neurons (van Praag, 2009). Melatonin, a hormone produced by the pineal gland, regulates sleep onset. Later bed times in these contemporary times means more artificial light exposure later at night, shifting the melatonin onset (Wright et al., 2013). n–3 long chain poly-unsaturated fatty acids (n-3 LCPUFAs), such as omega-3 found in foods like fish and nuts, are a good example of nutritional elements influencing the body. n-3 LCPUFAs are important building blocks for cell membranes, providing the proper permeability and fluidity which is important for signal transduction in neurons (Gómez-Pinilla, 2008). In addition, they also have a direct impact on BDNF levels (i.e., omega-3; van Praag, 2009).

Many of the physiological mechanisms influenced by physical activity, sleep, and nutrition have implications for brain functioning and thus possibly also on cognitive performance. The goal of this paper is not to provide a review of all possible mechanisms related to physical activity, sleep, and nutrition and their effect on brain functioning. Readers that would like an overview of the possible mechanisms can refer to the following sources of literature for physical activity (Barenberg et al., 2011), sleep (Tononi and Cirelli, 2014), and nutrition (Gómez-Pinilla, 2008).

Cognition is a term that is often used to refer to the mental abilities that facilitate processes such as memory, planning, inhibition, and problem-solving. These processes range from simple lower-order processes such as processing speed, to more complex higher-order processes such as task switching. In the cognitive domain, the executive functions (EFs) are considered to be very important for normal adult performance (Salthouse et al., 2003). EFs are top-down controlled mental processes that are needed for concentration and attention; the use of which take effort. When EFs are not used this implies that individuals act habitually, do not change their ways, and give in to temptations (Diamond, 2013). Despite the many definitions for EFs and the components that belong to them, the former description is generally agreed upon (Jurado and Rosselli, 2007). This review provides an in-depth examination of the concept, its components, related brain areas, and related tests (Jurado and Rosselli, 2007). In essence, some researchers believe that EFs have a unifying, central factor (Duncan et al., 1996; de Frias et al., 2006), while other believe that EFs depend on separate processes (Miyake et al., 2000; Salthouse, 2005). It remains to be determined which underlying processes relate to all EFs. For the data gathered and the tests performed in this study, Miyake et al. (2000) provide the best model as compared to others (Fisk and Sharp, 2004; Salthouse, 2005). The three EFs described by this model are often mentioned in literature, all of which are based on Baddeley's model of working memory (Baddeley, 1983) and his later proposal on the functions performed by the “central executive” (Baddeley, 1996). Miyake et al. (2000) described these functions as inhibition, updating, and shifting after statistically having analyzed Baddeley's proposal. To clarify, working memory can be divided into two components, short-term storage and executive processes (Smith and Jonides, 1999). The executive function “updating” manipulates the short-term storage and together with the “storage” forms the working memory.

Physical activity is defined as any movement originating from skeletal muscles that requires energy expenditure and has various subcategories (e.g., exercise or work-related activity) and indicators (e.g., subjective or objective). There is a large amount of research focusing on the positive effects of physical activity on cognitive processes (e.g., the reviews of Colcombe and Kramer, 2003; Hillman et al., 2008; Barenberg et al., 2011). The executive function hypothesis proposed a preferential benefit of physical activity for EFs (Hall et al., 2001). Indeed, such differential effects of physical activity on different cognitive functions exist. EFs benefit most from physical activity, while processing speed seems to benefit least (Colcombe and Kramer, 2003). In their extensive review article, Barenberg et al. (2011) discussed the limitations of the study of Colcombe and Kramer (2003) and investigated whether this preferential benefit of physical activity for EFs was apparent. They concluded that the results point in the direction of a preferential benefit (Barenberg et al., 2011). Important to note is that, according to the three EFs (i.e., updating, shifting, inhibition) as defined by Miyake et al. (2000), updating has never been investigated, consistent positive effects were shown on inhibition, and in the shifting domain positive effects were found occasionally (Barenberg et al., 2011). These research findings focus on direct effects of physical activity and not on overall habitual physical activity behavior. This is important as measures within the construct physical activity are diverse and different measures can be differentially associated with cognitive performance (e.g., Syväoja et al., 2014). In this study we focus on general habitual physical activity and on overall behavior as an indicator of physical activity.

Traditionally seen as part of physical activity, sedentary behavior refers to any behavior in which someone is using >1.5 metabolic equivalents. Generally this occurs when an individual is either sitting or lying down. In recent research the construct sedentary behavior is more often viewed as a separate construct, independent of physical activity, as a large review study found that general sedentary behavior is not related to physical activity (Rhodes et al., 2012). This is not surprising as one can be highly physically active and still sit a large amount of the day, due to a sedentary job, for example, which is a very likely situation in these contemporary times. Furthermore, it is important to evaluate whether this independence also exists with respect to cognitive performance. Research regarding sedentary behavior and its relation to cognitive performance is still very scarce, especially in young and middle-aged adults. There are some signs that lower amounts of sedentary behavior in combination with low-intensity physical activity behavior could be counteractive in age-related cognitive decline and that biological mechanisms underlying sedentary behavior and physical activity could overlap and possibly counteract their effects (Voss et al., 2014). To specify, these signs include the overall activity relation with cognitive performance in which moderate-to-vigorous physical activity barely accounted for the age-related cognitive decline (Smith, 2012).

In children (Dewald et al., 2010), adolescents (Radek and Kaprelian, 2013), and older adults (Nebes et al., 2009) sleep deprivation, due to low sleep quality or simply too little sleep, has been proven to lead to impaired cognitive performance. Besides actual sleep, chronotype also plays a role in cognitive performance. Chronotype, or time-of-day preference, refers to a person's preference for mornings or evenings (for more information, see Roenneberg et al., 2003). As shown in a meta-analytic study, evening oriented people generally perform better than morning oriented people, independent of the time at which the cognitive tests were performed (Preckel et al., 2011). In adults, ideal sleep duration is 7–8 h per night. Shorter as well as longer sleep duration leads to lower cognitive performance (Ferrie et al., 2011; Sternberg et al., 2013). Although sleep quality is a heavily researched domain, research concerning cognitive performance in adults, and in particular middle-aged adults, is scarce. Research in young adults shows poor sleep quality to be associated with decreased executive performance, specifically shifting (Benitez and Gunstad, 2012). In older adults, poor sleep quality has been associated with lower cognitive functioning. These relations were not uniform however across different cognitive functions. Impaired sleep quality has also been associated with decreases in working memory and shifting, but not in processing speed (Nebes et al., 2009). Both these findings align and show that higher order executive functions appear to be more affected by sleep quality than lower order functions.

We will evaluate the nutritional components that were measured in this study: breakfast, fish, and caffeine consumption. Breakfast consumption has been heavily investigated, albeit mainly in children. No generally accepted definition of breakfast is apparent in previous research and definitions range from “the first meal of the day” to “what participants themselves consider breakfast” (Mullan and Singh, 2010). Breakfast, as its name suggests, is mostly viewed as giving the body energy after a night of fasting and effects on cognitive performance are therefore mostly viewed as direct. Theoretically however, consuming breakfast could also have long-term benefits as a result of increased nutrient intake and better nutritional status (Pollitt and Mathews, 1998). Breakfast is a meal that is often skipped (Mullan and Singh, 2010). The general belief that breakfast is “the most important meal of the day” seems however to be far-fetched as this review shows that there is no general “recipe” for breakfast in relation to cognitive performance (Zilberter and Zilberter, 2013). This opinion article describes how seven distinct breakfast types have 16 different cognitive effects on nine populations.

Although research shows that n-3 LCPUFAs—found mainly in fish—are important for normal development, it is unknown whether n-3 LCPUFAs are beneficial for cognition in adults (Stonehouse, 2014). A major reason for this is simply because little research has been done in healthy young and middle-aged adults. Most research focuses on the development of children, adolescents, and older healthy or demented adults (Luchtman and Song, 2013). Research results in these age groups indicate that cognitive performance is enhanced following supplementation of n-3 LCPUFAs, although more research is needed to confirm this (McCann and Ames, 2005; Frensham et al., 2012; Stonehouse, 2014).

Caffeine can be regarded as a nutrient, a drug or a drug of abuse, depending on the way it is used (Pardo Lozano et al., 2007). Since we investigated habitual caffeine use in a healthy adult population, we considered caffeine as a nutrient. Caffeine is known to boost various cognitive functions and habitual caffeine intake is related to better long-term memory (Hameleers et al., 2000), alertness (Owen et al., 2008), reaction time, and short term recall (Ruxton, 2008). It is disputed however whether habitual intake truly enhances cognition and is not just a result of reversal of the withdrawal state. The withdrawal reversal hypothesis states that lower cognitive performance and alertness follows withdrawal and that this is restored by the consumption of caffeine, however no enhancement—separate from restoring the original performance—is apparent (Rogers, 2007). A recent review illustrates that evidence is in favor for the withdrawal reversal hypothesis (Rogers, 2014). Furthermore, consuming caffeine later in the night could also disrupt subsequent sleep processes which, as shown in adolescents (James et al., 2011), could impair cognitive performance.

In the present study, we examined whether variables within the BLFs physical activity, sleep, and nutrition were predictive for cognitive performance in young and middle-aged adults. We set out to investigate the joint contribution of subjectively measured general habitual behavior on these BLFs and expected that indicators of physical activity and sleep would be significant predictors of cognitive performance, while nutrition would not, following the literature review. The focus of this article will be on three cognitive functions: processing speed, shifting, and updating. As stated, more research is especially needed on the EFs shifting and updating (Nebes et al., 2009; Barenberg et al., 2011). In addition, as we measured cognitive performance in young and middle-aged adults, it is imperative to measure processing speed. First, because many cognitive processes are dependent on processing speed. Taking processing speed into account provides more interpretable information on the EFs. Second, aging causes cognitive processes to decline. This effect is independent and it is larger for processing speed than for EFs (Albinet et al., 2012). It is therefore important to account for processing speed as it can show insight in the unique age-related decline in each cognitive process.

The study was executed among young and middle-aged adult students of the Open University of the Netherlands (OUNL), a formal university-level institute providing distance education. As we had three outcome measures which were related to each other, path analysis using structural equation modeling was used. This method provides a measure of overall agreement, also called “fit,” between the model and the data as opposed to traditional path analysis using separate multiple regressions. As research in this adult population regarding BLFs and cognitive performance is scarce, a cross-validation approach was used with an exploratory and confirmatory mode to ensure validity of the analyses. The exploratory mode was used for model development, while the confirmatory mode was used for validation of the developed model.

Materials and Methods

Design

Data from this observational study come from the Adult Learning Open University Determinants (ALOUD) study. The ALOUD study is an investigation of different psychological and biological factors possibly affecting cognitive performance and/or learning performance in students participating in distance education (Neroni et al., 2015). Data from the biological part of this project are available and stored permanently on DANS EASY, a sustainable platform for archiving research data (Gijselaers, 2015). All variables within the BLFs were reported via an online digital survey conducted after registration at the university. Cognitive performance was measured objectively using digital cognitive tests conducted via the participants' computer directly after the survey.

Participants

Throughout 1 year (Sept. 2012–Aug. 2013), all new students of the OUNL who signed up for one or more regular bachelor or master course(s) were invited to participate. At the OUNL, students can register and start throughout the year as the education is modular and self-paced, open to everyone (with an age of at least 18 years old), and the curriculum is not fixed. This means that students can study full or part time. The OUNL mainly delivers online education.

The approached population size was 4945, 57.5% of those approached responded (N = 2842) and 41.27% of those approached (N = 2040) fully participated. From the sample of students that fully participated, the majority of students studied part time as most students had a full or part time paid job (i.e., 85.2%). Most students either lived alone (i.e., 20.4%), with a partner (i.e., 27.6%), or with partner and children (i.e., 34.3%). The age of participants ranged from 18 to 80, with the largest part (i.e., 56.9%) being between 26 and 45 years old. These participants are similar to the general population of students who normally study at the OUNL (Moerkerke, 2014).

Procedures

Participants were invited automatically via the e-mail system of the university 14–21 days after successful registration. This 7 day range is because a bulk mailing was sent weekly. Students received a reminder 2 weeks after the initial invitation and 1 week later a last reminder via e-mail. Four weeks after the initial invitation, a phone call was made (the goal was to reach participants in the three subsequent weeks) in which potential participants were asked whether they were still interested in participating. If so, they received the original invitation once more when needed and a reminder 6.5 weeks after the initial invitation, which was around 1.5 weeks after the phone call. In case the phone call was made in week 6, the reminder was sent 1 week later. Participants only received reminders or a telephone call if no full response was recorded.

The survey was administered online using LimeSurvey®, version 1.92+ (LimeSurvey Project Team/Carsten, Schmitz, 2012). Full participation cost the participants 45–60 min on average and it was possible to stop and continue later, allowing participants more freedom in their participation by spreading the time burden. It was only possible to interrupt and continue later in between questionnaires. Most participants (i.e., more than 92%) concluded the cognitive tests in one go, as advised beforehand. If participants broke a task off before completing it, they had to do the complete task again. Students who fully participated could win (5% chance) a gift voucher of €20. The ALOUD study was ethically approved by the local ethical committee of the OUNL. Each participant signed a digital informed consent form, explicating the use of the personal data gathered, voluntary participation, possibility to withdraw at any time, and finally giving their permission to use the data for the described goals. Participants had to click a checkbox to agree with the terms mentioned; a mandatory action to start the survey.

Materials

Outcome Measures

Cognitive performance was measured by an online digital cognitive test battery which was collected after the survey. Three tests were administered in the following order: (1) the Trail Making Test (TMT) (Army Individual Test Battery, 1944); (2) the Substitution Test (ST), which resembles the symbol digit modalities test (Smith, 1991); (3) and the N-back task (NBT) (Lezak et al., 2004). The TMT consisted of four parts, namely two training sessions and two test sessions. The A-part which involved clicking randomly placed numbers as fast as possible in the correct order (i.e., 1, 2, 3, etc.), and the B-part which involved clicking randomly placed numbers and letters as fast as possible in the correct order in a shifting mode (i.e., 1, A, 2, B, 3, C, etc.). Both parts were preceded by an instruction and a practice session. The ST consisted of two parts, namely one training sessions and one test session. The participants had to match the symbol shown with the correct number from a key on the top of the page. After clicking any number, the next symbol came up. They were instructed to substitute as many items possible in 90 s. The NBT consisted of four parts; three training sessions and one test session. The participants performed a two-back task with 60 items in which they had to indicate whether the number shown was identical to the number shown two trials earlier. For all three tests the participants were instructed to work as accurately and quickly as possible. The TMT resulted in a measure for the executive function shifting, measured via the B-A part, in which the number part was subtracted from the number-letter part. The B-A part provides a relatively pure indicator of task-switching ability that minimizes for working memory and visuoperceptual demands (Sánchez-Cubillo et al., 2009). The outcome measure in the ST was the number of items correctly substituted in 90 s, which is a measure of processing speed. This ST mainly measures perceptual processing, visual search, and involves a motor component (e.g., Shum et al., 1990; Sánchez-Cubillo et al., 2009). In the NBT, the number of correctly remembered items is a measure for working memory and the executive function updating. Updating tasks, such as the NBT, measure general working memory processes as well as unique substitution processes which are independent of working memory (Ecker et al., 2010; Wilhelm et al., 2013).

Predictors

The measures for the variables within each BLF were extracted from various questionnaires. First the sleep related measures were questioned, then physical activity, and then nutrition. All BLF measures were related to habitual behavior over the past months. Physical activity was measured via the Short Questionnaire to ASsess Health-enhancing physical activity (SQUASH), which has a reasonable reliability (r = 0.58) and validity (r = 0.45; validated against an accelerometer) (Wendel-Vos et al., 2003). Participants were asked how many days a week they spent on the activities and how much time on an average day. Physical activity was calculated as a weekly activity score; an accumulated product score of intensity of the activity multiplied by the minutes spent on the activity. The activity score represented habitual physical activity on the basis of an average week over the past months. The questions included information regarding activities related to commuting, leisure time, household, and work. The leisure time activities were walking, cycling, gardening, and four sports to be filled in by the participants. The reported sports were manually inspected and rescored to corresponding metabolic equivalent values reported in the most recent version of the compendium of physical activities (Ainsworth et al., 2011). Sedentary behavior was measured using a questionnaire based on the principle of the SQUASH. Questions on sedentary behavior concerned sedentary behavior during work, transportation, leisure time (i.e., on work and free days) and resting. Participants were asked how many days a week they spent on sedentary behavior during these activities and how much time on an average day. Sedentary behavior was calculated as a total score of minutes of sitting and lying per week. This questionnaire was designed in another study at the OUNL (van Stralen et al., 2009), but has not been validated or reported up till now. Chronotype was measured via reported sleep- and wake-times on work and free days using specific questions from the Munich ChronoType Questionnaire (MCTQ) (Roenneberg et al., 2003). Midsleep (i.e., midpoint of sleep) on free days corrected for sleep debt (MSFSC), was used as the measure for chronotype (Roenneberg et al., 2004). This was calculated using the equation: MSFSC = MSF–0.5*[SDF–(5*SDW + 2*SDF)/7]. SDF is sleep duration on free days and SDW is sleep duration on work days. Sleep quality was measured with the Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), a well-known and well-validated self-report sleep quality measure (Buysse et al., 1989). The PSQI includes items on subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep disturbances, use of sleep medication, and daytime dysfunction. These seven components yield one global score. Sleep duration was derived from specific questions from the MCTQ and was included as a polynomial term as an inverted U-shape is present in the relation with cognitive performance (Ferrie et al., 2011; Sternberg et al., 2013). Sleep duration was investigated separately for work and free days. Breakfast consumption was measured as eating breakfast every day or not. Fish consumption was measured with a questionnaire validated against omega-3 plasma levels (de Groot et al., 2009). Caffeine consumption was measured as average daily caffeine consumption calculated from reported coffee, tea and energy drink consumption using average beverage caffeine values reported in the literature (Ruxton, 2008).

Covariates

Next to the three BLFs discussed above, five covariates were taken into account:

• alcohol use, as it was positively related to cognitive performance in an observational study conducted among middle-aged adults (average age of 56) (Kalmijn et al., 2002). However, another study among younger middle-aged adults (average age of 43) found no associations between alcohol use and cognitive performance (Caspers et al., 2010).

• educational level (measured on an eight-level scale), as it is a predictor for cognitive performance (Van der Elst et al., 2006a,b,c). In addition, educational level is associated with different lifestyle elements. For example, a higher educational level is related to higher levels of physical activity (Droomers, 2001) and predictive for a healthier nutritional pattern (Darmon and Drewnowski, 2008). The coding represents the following levels: 1, Lower general education; 2, Lower vocational education; 3, Average general education; 4, Average vocational education; 5, Secondary general education; 6, Higher vocational education; 7, Higher general/scientific education; 8, Post-higher/Post-university education.

• age, as it is an important predictor for cognitive performance as well, as cognitive functions decrease with age (Albinet et al., 2012).

• body mass index (BMI; computed from self-reported weight and height using the formula: BMI = weight/height2), as it is an important covariate since these three BLFs are associated with BMI. For example, low sleep duration is associated with increased BMI (Taheri et al., 2004).

• computer abilities (measured via a self-developed questionnaire mapping attitude, confidence, and skills toward the use of a computer), as the cognitive tests were conducted via the participants' computer. This could mean that people with lower levels of computer abilities could be slower in responding. This should therefore be only visible in the processing speed measure seeing as the executive measures correct for basic performance (i.e., processes such as motor speed). In the TMT, basis performance was controlled for by subtracting the A-part from the B-part. In the NBT, motor speed plays no role as the number of correctly answered items is the outcome. In the ST no correction for basic performance is included.

Analyses

Pre-processing and the analyses for descriptives were done in SPSS (version 20; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Path analysis was executed using structural equation modeling (i.e., with only manifest variables) performed in AMOS (version 22.0.0; Arbuckle, 2012). Cross-validation was conducted by randomly splitting the final sample into two subsamples, with an almost equal number of cases in each subsample. One subsample was used as a testing sample to develop a model based upon theory and using an exploratory mode. The second subsample was used as a validation sample to test the developed model in a confirmatory mode to check the validity of the developed model.

A covariate model was built, including all covariates mentioned with paths to the dependent measures. Based on the literature, paths from processing speed to both executive variables were drawn. Subsequently, the model fit was evaluated by investigating fit and modification indices using the testing subsample. Other relevant parameters were drawn, if necessary. When the fit indices demonstrated proper fit, the statistically non-significant parameters were trimmed (i.e., P-value higher than 0.05). The parameter with the highest P-value was excluded in a step-by-step mode, re-evaluating the model at each step. This yielded the covariate model. Next, a model was built for the BLFs including all measures mentioned with paths to the dependent measures. The same approach was used as in the development of the covariate model and insignificant parameters were trimmed step-by-step. The last step in the exploratory mode was to control for the relevant covariates. This was done by combining both models developed after which the insignificant parameters were trimmed. Finally, this model was tested using the validation dataset, in order to evaluate the validity of the final model.

The fit measures reported are the: Normed chi-square (χ2/df) (Hair et al., 2014), comparative fit index (CFI: Bentler, 1990), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA: Browne and Cudeck, 1992) and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR: Byrne, 2010). Proper fit is indicated when: χ2/df is less than 3 (Hair et al., 2014), CFI is higher or close to 0.95, RMSEA < 0.06 and SRMR < 0.08 (Hu and Bentler, 1999). Univariate outliers on the three outcome measures were excluded before analyses (i.e., a standardized Z-value higher than the absolute value of 3.29 or via visual inspection) in case the distribution was normal.

Results

Dataset Compilation

The original dataset contained 2842 cases. Participants were excluded if they: (1) did not complete the survey and the cognitive tests (1224 cases); (2) made a remark at the end of the survey that led to exclusion (85 cases); (3) had missing data (346 cases); (4) performed below chance level on the NBT (51 cases); and (5) outliers were excluded as mentioned in the Methods section (five cases). All exclusions mentioned led to the analyses reported below with 1131 people included. This file was then randomly split into a testing sample (N = 565) and a validation sample (N = 566) for cross-validation.

To make sure that the exclusions led to a sample that was representative of the initial sample, comparisons were made regarding age, sex, and educational level. No differences were observed regarding age, t(1853) = −0.31, p = 0.757, as the age of the excluded group with 37.1 years old was almost equal to the included group with 37.3 years old. No differences were observed in terms of sex, χ2(1, N = 2043) > 0.01, p = 0.96, as 38.3% was male in the excluded group, compared with 38.4% in the included group. The apparent sex bias might have influenced the results. Men might outperform women on visual-spatial tasks, which is relevant for the processing speed measurement in the current study. Nevertheless, there is a much greater overlap in the distribution of scores than a difference between them (Weiss et al., 2003). Considering the large sample, this means that analyses can be carried out using the entire sample. In case significant results are found, the sample can be stratified to investigate possible sex differences. Educational level was also equal in both groups, t(2463) = −1.69, p = 0.091, with 5.82 for the excluded group compared to 5.92 for the included group.

There was no difference in cognitive performance between people who executed the task in one trial or those who interrupted the tasks. For the TMT, the difference between the “one-traillers” (M = 20.28; SD = 13.03) and the “interrupters” (M = 22.53; SD = 15.34) was not significant, t(1129) = 1.03, p = 0.306. For the ST, the difference between the “one-traillers” (M = 49.96; SD = 7.78) and the “interrupters” (M = 54.00; SD = 7.60) was not significant, t(1129) = 1.86, p = 0.063. For the NBT, the difference between the “one-traillers” (M = 55.50; SD = 5.57) and the “interrupters” (M = 56.15; SD = 4.96) was not significant, t(1129) = 0.79, p = 0.431.

Descriptives

Each model was tested using the method of asymptotically distribution free (ADF) estimation to correct for the non-normality seen in the results since kurtosis values should be around 1 and multivariate kurtosis should not be higher than 5 (Byrne, 2010). Furthermore, the distribution of the outcome measures updating and shifting were not normal. ADF was employed in the analysis because the sample size was more than 10 times the number of freely estimated parameters (Byrne, 2010).

The descriptives are depicted in Table 1. The assumption of no multicollinearity was met following inspection of the correlations (i.e., below 0.8, data not shown, according to Field, 2009). All variables were included in the evaluation of this assumption. Table A1 shows the zero-order correlations between all model variables. As can be seen in Table 1, the standard deviation differs among the variables, imposing a threat to the reliability of the results as homoscedasticity is an important assumption in linear models. Large differences in variances can distort the estimation of the model fit and parameters. The variables were therefore transformed to align the variances prior to the analyses. All descriptives mentioned in Table 1 were reported over the entire dataset before random splitting.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptives of all included variables.

Path Analyses

The testing sample was used in the exploratory mode for model development. The covariate model which depicted proper fit between the model and the data was built, χ2/df = 0.79, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA < 0.001, SRMR = 0.018 (Figure 1). Next, the model was built for the BLFs which showed satisfactory fit between the model and the data, χ2/df = 0.89, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA < 0.001, SRMR = 0.022 (Figure 2). Clearly, four measures within the BLFs show significant relations with processing speed. There were no direct relations from any measure within the BLFs with one of the executive measures. Model 3, which corrected for the covariates, showed good fit between the model and the data, χ2/df = 0.85, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA < 0.001, SRMR = 0.016 (Figure 3). Model 3 shows that, after controlling for the covariates, only sedentary behavior remains as a significant predictor of processing speed. The relation is positive, meaning that participants who were more sedentary performed better on the processing speed test.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Model 1: Covariate model with only the relevant parameters.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Model 2: Biological lifestyle factor model with only the relevant parameters.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Model 3: Final trimmed model for the biological lifestyle factor measures after controlling for confounding covariates.

The validation sample was used to test the developed model in the confirmatory mode and check the validity of the developed model (i.e., to perform the cross-validation). Validation of the developed model indicated the model was fitting the data properly, χ2/df = 2.75, CFI = 0.95, RMSEA < 0.056, SRMR = 0.035 (Figure 4). All fit indices are within the limits of proper fit indication. The parameter estimates differ from the estimates in model 3, which used the testing sample, but they are comparable (Table 2). In the validation step, exclusion of sedentary behavior indicated that 0.8% of the variance in processing speed was explained by sedentary behavior. This confirmatory step in the cross-validation procedure means the model developed in the exploratory mode was correct.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Validation model: Validation of the developed model (model 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Estimates for model 3 and the validation model.

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to examine whether subjectively measured general habitual behavior on the variables within the BLFs physical activity, sleep, and nutrition were predictive for cognitive performance in young and middle-aged adults. Although a large number of participants needed to be excluded in order to successfully carry out the structural equation modeling, statistical comparison of the included vs. the excluded cases shows no differences in terms of age, sex, and educational level. This means there is no selection bias resulting from these exclusions within the investigated population. In the exploratory mode a model was developed using the testing sample. This yielded a covariate model (Figure 1), a BLF model (Figure 2), and a final BLF model which was corrected for possible confounding by combining model 1 and model 2 (Figure 3). After correcting for the covariates, sedentary behavior remained as a significant predictor of processing speed. The significant variables chronotype, fish consumption, and caffeine consumption in model 2 (Figure 2) turned out to be insignificant after correcting for the covariates (Figure 3). All other measurements within the BLFs were insignificant. This means that none of the variables measured within the BLFs were predictive for either executive functioning or processing speed, except for sedentary behavior. Sedentary behavior explained a very small proportion of the variance in processing speed. Validation of the developed model via the confirmatory step using the validation sample showed that the developed model was valid and that the estimates were comparable to the developed model in the exploratory mode. This indicated the developed model is generalizable to the population. We will discuss these results per BLF in the light of the literature, after the discussion of the study's limitations.

The current study had several important limitations that we would like to emphasize before discussing the results. First, the study is observational and does not allow for causal inferences although the hypotheses were theory-driven and path analysis assumes directional paths implying causation. Second, the cognitive tests were conducted at the participants' home following the survey which lasted around 45 min. Although participants were instructed to conduct the cognitive tests in a well-rested and active state and without possible distraction, and that they had the liberty to postpone the test and thus separate it from the survey, it could well be that participants did the tests directly after the survey when they were probably fatigued. Alternatively, they might have been distracted by their surroundings (e.g., a pet, child, or partner). Both of these points were tackled by the large data set, however, largely smoothing these possibly confounding factors. Third, survey based research is in many cases not the most reliable type of research because of social desirability, faulty reporting due to memory issues, or question interpretation. Fourth, the time of the day at which the cognitive tests took place was not recorded for the cognitive tests specifically. Despite the large dataset, these limitations may have confounded the results and could impair the interpretation of the results. Fifth, the sensitivity of the cognitive tests might be questioned. The measurement for processing speed clearly shows a relation with a number of predictors and covariates, demonstrating its sensitivity. The executive functions measurements (i.e., updating and shifting) show consistent relations with processing speed, as expected. No relation with the predictors was shown, however, which questions the sensitivity of the measurements. For shifting, the measurement is normally distributed. It could, however, be possible that the sensitivity is too low to reveal relations with the predictors. For updating a ceiling effect is present. Some participants were able to answer all 60 items correct. This is an indication of a possible threat for the sensitivity. On the other hand, updating also shows a relation with education, suggesting the measurement is sensitive enough. Especially considering that education is a large-grained measurement (i.e., an eight-point scale). Last, the investigated sample is quite homogeneous in terms of educational level and all participants are studying for a higher degree. Still, the students investigated are diverse in educational background (i.e., disciplines) and age. Nevertheless, this specific subpopulation might be to homogeneous to reveal possible relations that could be present in the general population of young and middle-aged adults.

Regarding physical activity, the findings were not expected as earlier research showed physical activity to be differentially related to specific cognitive functions (Colcombe and Kramer, 2003; Hillman et al., 2008; Barenberg et al., 2011). However, Barenberg et al. (2011) reported that the function updating was never investigated and that the results on the function shifting were mixed. Hence, these findings are interesting as this is probably the first study that investigated updating and its relation with physical activity. For shifting, the current study added to the body of knowledge. Physical activity shows different relationship with cognitive performance, dependent on the physical activity construct measured (Syväoja et al., 2014). It is therefore of importance to further investigate more specific and objective physical activity constructs. The finding that sedentary behavior was positively predictive for processing speed was also unexpected. This means that participants who were more sedentary performed better on the processing speed test. The measurement of specific types of sedentary behavior is recommended more and more often (Rhodes et al., 2012), as research shows that different types of sedentary behavior (e.g., watching TV vs. working on the computer) are differentially associated with cognitive performance (Kesse-Guyot et al., 2012). For example, Kesse-Guyot et al. (2012) showed TV viewing to be negatively related to cognitive functioning, while computer use was positively related to cognitive functioning. The population investigated in the current study is characterized by fulltime working adult students, who often have very limited time to study due to work and social responsibilities (e.g., children and/or partner). It is therefore very likely that the sedentary time measured here reflects more the sedentary time caused by positive sedentary behaviors (e.g., studying) that stimulates the brain, than negative sedentary behaviors (e.g., TV viewing), since overall sedentary time was measured. We recommend future studies to focus on specific sedentary behaviors to evaluate whether these are differentially related to cognitive performance.

The sleep related measures showed no added value in the prediction of cognitive performance. Although Preckel et al. (2011) showed in their meta-analyses that chronotype was related to cognitive performance, this finding was concluded from research in adolescents/young adults (average age range = 15.19-25.04). In addition, chronotype in this meta-analysis was defined as a two-dimensional construct and cognitive performance was measured using various different aggregated tests. In the current study, participants age range was different and chronotype was defined as a continuous variable (i.e., midsleep on free days corrected for sleep debt; Roenneberg et al., 2004). Cognitive performance was measured for three specific constructs and no overall scores were used. Taken together, these differences can explain the different findings. It could well be that at older ages chronotype is not that strongly related anymore to cognitive performance. In addition, our use of a continuous chronotype measure ensured better estimation of chronotype, so a possible relation with cognitive performance should have stood out if present. It could be that other cognitive functions besides processing speed and executive functioning are associated with chronotype. Second, sleep duration was also not predictive for cognitive performance. Sleep duration was included as a polynomial term (i.e., separate for sleep duration on work and free days) since research shows an inverted U-shaped relation with cognitive performance (Ferrie et al., 2011; Sternberg et al., 2013). Sleep duration not being predictive was unexpected, considering these previous studies. However, the cognitive tests executed in the previous research mentioned included different tests measuring an aggregated set of cognitive functions (Ferrie et al., 2011; Sternberg et al., 2013). This could mean that sleep duration is not associated with processing speed or the executive functions updating and shifting, but that it is associated with combined or other cognitive processes. Third, sleep quality was not shown to be predictive for cognitive performance, partly opposed to the expectations from the literature. Sleep quality not being related to processing speed is in accordance with the literature (Nebes et al., 2009). Sleep quality not being related to executive functioning was unexpected as research suggests a relation (Nebes et al., 2009; Benitez and Gunstad, 2012). However, sleep quality is barely investigated, especially in middle-aged adults. It could be that no relation between sleep quality and executive functioning exists at this age. Considering the limitations of this study, this requires further investigation.

When it comes to nutrition, it was less surprising that breakfast, fish, and caffeine consumption were not predictive for cognitive performance, as these variables were measured on a level of habitual use. Breakfast consumption mostly provides direct effects on cognitive performance (i.e., in the morning, after consumption). Considering that most adults in the current study have full-time jobs and family responsibilities (i.e., children and a partner), they study in the evening hours and will most likely have taken the cognitive tests at night or in the weekend. It is therefore understandable that breakfast consumption did not predict cognitive performance. The relation between fish consumption and cognitive performance in adults is still under debate in the literature (Stonehouse, 2014). From the findings of this study, we conclude that fish consumption does not predict cognitive performance in young and middle-aged adults. Habitual use of caffeine is not predictive for cognitive performance. These findings are in line with previously conducted research. As shown by Killgore et al. (2012), executive functioning is not influenced by caffeine use. In addition, Ruxton (2008) shows in an extensive review that caffeine is expected to have short-term effects on cognitive performance. Since timing of cognitive testing and specific consumption of caffeine use prior to timing was not investigated in the current study, this cannot be evaluated here. Lastly, tolerance effects of caffeine are subject to debate and no agreement is apparent on whether caffeine actually stimulates cognitive performance, or just restores it after tolerance effects appear (Ruxton, 2008). Together, the findings discussed above lead us to conclude that habitual use on these nutrition variables is not predictive for cognitive performance.

The strengths of this study are multiple. The large data set provides a high power related to the findings and decreases the risk of contracting a type-1 error. On the other hand, explorative model development increases capitalization on chance. A cross-validation approach was used to control for this. This approach allows us to safely conclude that the model was generalizable to the population. Another strength is that this adult population has rarely been investigated, making these new findings an important starting point for the fields of cognition and individual differences in young and middle-aged adults. Investigating the combination of these three BLFs physical activity, sleep, and nutrition is a new and challenging approach, providing new insights. A major strength is that a number of possible confounders was controlled for, hereby eliminating possible spurious relationships, as shown in the differences between model 2 (Figure 2) and model 3 (Figure 3).

In conclusion, the results presented here indicate that the variables within the BLFs do not predict cognitive performance in young and middle-aged adults. The only exception is sedentary behavior which predicts processing speed, although it predicts only a small proportion of the data. Furthermore, it is unclear which specific sedentary behavior is responsible for this positive relation. Therefore, these results should not promote people to become more sedentary as it is not clear which exact sedentary behaviors are responsible for this effect and research shows sedentary behaviors to be differently associated with cognitive performance (Kesse-Guyot et al., 2012; Rhodes et al., 2012). These results should be interpreted with care and more research is needed to clarify this relation. We suggest future research to investigate the relations between BLFs and their respective relation with, or effect on, cognitive performance as this will create a better understanding of the environmental influences on cognitive performance.

Author Contributions

This study was mainly executed and written by HG, the first author of this research paper. EB supervised HG during his 4-month internship at the Universitat Oberta de Catalunya in Barcelona in 2014. During this time the analyses and writing of the article was done. RD and PK were supervisors of HG during his PhD project and guided him through the process of executing this study and helped him writing the research paper. All co-authors substantially contributed to the work and the subsequent writing of the article.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Carme Viladrich, from de Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, for her voluntary, disinterested, and kind assistance. Carme offered her expertise upon request in a meeting and gave subsequent advice via email. This highly increased the quality and understanding of the use of structural equation modeling for executing path analysis. We are Carme very thankful for her efforts.

References

Ainsworth, B. E., Haskell, W. L., Herrmann, S. D., Meckes, N., Bassett, D. R. Jr., Tudor-Locke, C., et al. (2011). 2011 Compendium of Physical Activities: a second update of codes and MET values. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 43, 1575–1581. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31821ece12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albinet, C. T., Boucard, G., Bouquet, C. A., and Audiffren, M. (2012). Processing speed and executive functions in cognitive aging: how to disentangle their mutual relationship? Brain Cogn. 79, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arbuckle, J. L. (2012). Amos (Version 22.0.0) [Computer Program]. Chicago, IL: IBM SPSS.

Army Individual Test Battery. (1944). Manual of Directions and Scoring. Washington, DC: War Department, Adjutant General's Office.

Baddeley, A. (1983). Working memory. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 302, 311–324. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1983.0057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sec. A 49, 5–28. doi: 10.1080/713755608

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barenberg, J., Berse, T., and Dutke, S. (2011). Executive functions in learning processes: do they benefit from physical activity? Educ. Res. Rev. 6, 208–222. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2011.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benitez, A., and Gunstad, J. (2012). Poor sleep quality diminishes cognitive functioning independent of depression and anxiety in healthy young adults. Clin. Neuropsychol. 26, 214–223. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2012.658439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol. Bull. 107, 238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Soc. Methods Res. 21, 230–258. doi: 10.1177/0049124192021002005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burkhalter, T. M., and Hillman, C. H. (2011). A narrative review of physical activity, nutrition, and obesity to cognition and scholastic performance across the human lifespan. Adv. Nutr. 2, 201S–206S. doi: 10.3945/an.111.000331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Busch, V., Van Stel, H. F., Schrijvers, A. J. P., and de Leeuw, J. R. J. (2013). Clustering of health-related behaviors, health outcomes and demographics in Dutch adolescents: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 13:1118. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-1118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F. III, Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., and Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28, 193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M. (2010). Structural Equation Modeling With AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming, 2nd Edn. London: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Caspers, K., Arndt, S., Yucuis, R., McKirgan, L., and Spinks, R. (2010). Effects of alcohol- and cigarette-use disorders on global and specific measures of cognition in middle-age adults. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 71, 192–200. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2010.71.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colcombe, S., and Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Sci. 14, 125–130. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darmon, N., and Drewnowski, A. (2008). Does social class predict diet quality? Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 87, 1107–1117.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dean, B., Aguilar, D., Shapiro, C., Orr, W. C., Isserman, J. A., Calimlim, B., et al. (2010). Impaired health status, daily functioning, and work productivity in adults with excessive sleepiness. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 52, 144–149. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0b013e3181c99505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Frias, C., Dixon, R. A., and Strauss, E. (2006). Structure of four executive functioning tests in healthy older adults. Neuropsychology 20, 206–214. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.2.206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Groot, R. H. M., van Boxtel, M. P. J., Schiepers, O. J. G., Hornstra, G., and Jolles, J. (2009). Age dependence of plasma phospholipid fatty acid levels: potential role of linoleic acid in the age-associated increase in docosahexaenoic acid and eicosapentaenoic acid concentrations. Br. J. Nutr. 102, 1058–1064. doi: 10.1017/S0007114509359103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dewald, J. F., Meijer, A. M., Oort, F. J., Kerkhof, G. A., and Bögels, S. M. (2010). The influence of sleep quality, sleep duration and sleepiness on school performance in children and adolescents: a meta-analytic review. Sleep Med. Rev. 14, 179–189. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Droomers, M. (2001). Educational level and decreases in leisure time physical activity: predictors from the longitudinal GLOBE study. J. Epidemiol. Commun. Health 55, 562–568. doi: 10.1136/jech.55.8.562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duncan, J., Emslie, H., Williams, P., Johnson, R., and Freer, C. (1996). Intelligence and the frontal lobe: the organization of goal-directed behavior. Cogn. Psychol. 30, 257–303. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1996.0008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ecker, U. K. H., Lewandowsky, S., Oberauer, K., and Chee, A. E. H. (2010). The components of working memory updating: an experimental decomposition and individual differences. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 36, 170–189. doi: 10.1037/a0017891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrie, J. E., Shipley, M. J., Akbaraly, T. N., Marmot, M. G., Kivimäki, M., and Singh-Manoux, A. (2011). Change in sleep duration and cognitive function: findings from the Whitehall II Study. Sleep 34, 565–573.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Field, A. (2009). Discovering Statistics Using SPSS: And Sex and Drugs and Rock “n” Roll, 3rd Edn. London: Sage.

Fisk, J., and Sharp, C. (2004). Age-related impairment in executive functioning: updating, inhibition, shifting, and access. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 26, 874–890. doi: 10.1080/13803390490510680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frensham, L. J., Bryan, J., and Parletta, N. (2012). Influences of micronutrient and omega-3 fatty acid supplementation on cognition, learning, and behavior: methodological considerations and implications for children and adolescents in developed societies. Nutr. Rev. 70, 594–610. doi: 10.1111/j.1753-4887.2012.00516.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gijselaers, H. J. M. (Open Universiteit (NL)) (2015). Adult Learning Open University Determinants (ALOUD) study (biological part). DANS. doi: 10.17026/dans-2zy-xqju

CrossRef Full Text

Gómez-Pinilla, F. (2008). Brain foods: the effects of nutrients on brain function. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 568–578. doi: 10.1038/nrn2421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., and Anderson, R. E. (2014). Multivariate Data Analysis, 7th Edn. Harlow: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Hall, C. D., Smith, A. L., and Keele, S. W. (2001). The impact of aerobic activity on cognitive function in older adults: a new synthesis based on the concept of executive control. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 13, 279–300. doi: 10.1080/09541440126012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hameleers, P. A. H. M., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Hogervorst, E., Riedel, W. J., Houx, P. J., Buntinx, F., et al. (2000). Habitual caffeine consumption and its relation to memory, attention, planning capacity and psychomotor performance across multiple age groups. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 15, 573–581. doi: 10.1002/hup.218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., and Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 58–65. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, J. E., Kristjánsson, A. L., and Sigfúsdóttir, I. D. (2011). Adolescent substance use, sleep, and academic achievement: evidence of harm due to caffeine. J. Adolesc. 34, 665–673. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jurado, M. B., and Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol. Rev. 17, 213–233. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9040-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalmijn, S., Boxtel, M. P. J., Verschuren, M. W. M., Jolles, J., and Launer, L. J. (2002). Cigarette smoking and alcohol consumption in relation to cognitive performance in middle age. Am. J. Epidemiol. 156, 936–944. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kesse-Guyot, E., Charreire, H., Andreeva, V. A., Touvier, M., Hercberg, S., Galan, P., et al. (2012). Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations of different sedentary behaviors with cognitive performance in older adults. PLoS ONE 7:e47831. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Killgore, W. D. S., Grugle, N. L., and Balkin, T. J. (2012). Gambling when sleep deprived: don't bet on stimulants. Chronobiol. Int. 29, 43–54. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2011.635230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., and Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment, 4th Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press Inc.

LimeSurvey Project Team/Carsten, Schmitz. (2012). LimeSurvey: An Open Source Survey Tool. Retrieved from: http://www.limesurvey.org

Luchtman, D. W., and Song, C. (2013). Cognitive enhancement by omega-3 fatty acids from child-hood to old age: findings from animal and clinical studies. Neuropharmacology 64, 550–565. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCann, J. C., and Ames, B. N. (2005). Is docosahexaenoic acid, an n-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid, required for development of normal brain function? An overview of evidence from cognitive and behavioral tests in humans and animals. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 82, 281–295.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moerkerke, G. (2014). Trends in student population and curriculum design for the Open University of the Netherlands. Int. J. Contin. Educ. Lifelong Learn. 7, 71–91.

Google Scholar

Mullan, B. A., and Singh, M. (2010). A systematic review of the quality, content, and context of breakfast consumption. Nutr. Food Sci. 40, 81–114. doi: 10.1108/00346651011015953

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nebes, R. D., Buysse, D. J., Halligan, E. M., Houck, P. R., and Monk, T. H. (2009). Self-reported sleep quality predicts poor cognitive performance in healthy older adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 64, 180–187. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbn037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neroni, J., Gijselaers, H. J. M., Kirschner, P. A., and de Groot, R. H. M. (2015). The Adult Learning Open University Determinants (ALOUD) study: biological and psychological factors associated with learning performance in adult distance education. Br. J. Educ. Technol. 46, 953–960. doi: 10.1111/bjet.12288

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owen, G. N., Parnell, H., De Bruin, E. A., and Rycroft, J. A. (2008). The combined effects of L-theanine and caffeine on cognitive performance and mood. Nutr. Neurosci. 11, 193–198. doi: 10.1179/147683008X301513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pardo Lozano, R., Alvarez García, Y., Barral Tafalla, D., and Farré Albaladejo, M. (2007). Cafeína: un nutriente, un fármaco, o una droga de abuso [Caffeine: a nutrient, a drug or a drug of abuse]. Adicciones 19, 225–238. doi: 10.20882/adicciones.303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Philip, P., Sagaspe, P., Prague, M., Tassi, P., Capelli, A., Bioulac, B., et al. (2012). Acute versus chronic partial sleep deprivation in middle-aged people: differential effect on performance and sleepiness. Sleep 35, 997–1002. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1968

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollitt, E., and Mathews, R. (1998). Breakfast and cognition: an integrative summary. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 67, 804S–813S.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Preckel, F., Lipnevich, A. A., Schneider, S., and Roberts, R. D. (2011). Chronotype, cognitive abilities, and academic achievement: a meta-analytic investigation. Learn. Individ. Differ. 21, 483–492. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radek, K. S., and Kaprelian, J. (2013). Emerging adult sleep quality: health and academic performance factors of assessment. J. Sleep Disord. Ther. 2:112. doi: 10.4172/2167-0277.1000112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhodes, R. E., Mark, R. S., and Temmel, C. P. (2012). Adult sedentary behavior: a systematic review. Am. J. Prev. Med. 42, e3–e28. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2011.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roenneberg, T., Kuehnle, T., Pramstaller, P. P., Ricken, J., Havel, M., Guth, A., et al. (2004). A marker for the end of adolescence. Curr. Biol. 14, R1038–R1039. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.11.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roenneberg, T., Wirz-Justice, A., and Merrow, M. (2003). Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J. Biol. Rhythms 18, 80–90. doi: 10.1177/0748730402239679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, P. J. (2007). Caffeine, mood and mental performance in everyday life. Nutr. Bull. 32, 84–89. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-3010.2007.00607.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, P. J. (2014). Caffeine and alertness: in defense of withdrawal reversal. J. Caffeine Res. 4, 3–8. doi: 10.1089/jcr.2014.0009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruxton, C. H. S. (2008). The impact of caffeine on mood, cognitive function, performance and hydration: a review of benefits and risks. Nutr. Bull. 33, 15–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-3010.2007.00665.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (2005). Relations between cognitive abilities and measures of executive functioning. Neuropsychology 19, 532–545. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., and Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 132, 566–594. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.4.566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sánchez-Cubillo, I., Periáñez, J. A., Adrover-Roig, D., Rodríguez-Sánchez, J. M., Ríos-Lago, M., Tirapu, J., et al. (2009). Construct validity of the Trail Making Test: role of task-switching, working memory, inhibition/interference control, and visuomotor abilities. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 438–450. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shum, D. H. K., McFarland, K. A., and Bain, J. D. (1990). Construct validity of eight tests of attention: comparison of normal and closed head injured samples. Clin. Neuropsychol. 4, 151–162. doi: 10.1080/13854049008401508

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Small, G. W., Silverman, D. H. S., Siddarth, P., Ercoli, L. M., Miller, K. J., Lavretsky, H., et al. (2006). Effects of a 14-day healthy longevity lifestyle program on cognition and brain function. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 14, 538–545. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000219279.72210.ca

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, A. (1991). Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Smith, P. J. (2012). Physical activity, vascular health, and cognitive impairment. Arch. Intern. Med. 172, 83. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2011.615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. E., and Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science 283, 1657–1661. doi: 10.1126/science.283.5408.1657

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sternberg, D. A., Ballard, K., Hardy, J. L., Katz, B., Doraiswamy, P. M., and Scanlon, M. (2013). The largest human cognitive performance dataset reveals insights into the effects of lifestyle factors and aging. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:292. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stonehouse, W. (2014). Does consumption of LC omega-3 PUFA enhance cognitive performance in healthy school-aged children and throughout adulthood? Nutrients 6, 2730–2758. doi: 10.3390/nu6072730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Syväoja, H. J., Tammelin, T. H., Ahonen, T., Kankaanpää, A., and Kantomaa, M. T. (2014). The associations of objectively measured physical activity and sedentary time with cognitive functions in school-aged children. PLoS ONE 9:e103559. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taheri, S., Lin, L., Austin, D., Young, T., and Mignot, E. (2004). Short sleep duration is associated with reduced leptin, elevated ghrelin, and increased body mass index. PLoS Med. 1:e62. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0010062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tononi, G., and Cirelli, C. (2014). Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron 81, 12–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Van Breukelen, G. J. P., and Jolles, J. (2006a). The concept shifting test: adult normative data. Psychol. Assess. 18, 424–432. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.4.424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Van Breukelen, G. J. P., and Jolles, J. (2006b). The letter digit substitution test: normative data for 1,858 healthy participants aged 24-81 from the Maastricht aging study (MAAS): influence of age, education, and sex. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 28, 998–1009. doi: 10.1080/13803390591004428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Elst, W., Van Boxtel, M. P. J., Van Breukelen, G. J. P., and Jolles, J. (2006c). The Stroop color-word test: Influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment 13, 62–79. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Praag, H. (2009). Exercise and the brain: something to chew on. Trends Neurosci. 32, 283–290. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Stralen, M. M., de Vries, H., Mudde, A. N., Bolman, C., and Lechner, L. (2009). The working mechanisms of an environmentally tailored physical activity intervention for older adults: a randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 6:83. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-6-83

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Vollmer, C., Pötsch, F., and Randler, C. (2013). Morningness is associated with better gradings and higher attention in class. Learn. Individ. Differ. 27, 167–173. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, M. W., Carr, L. J., Clark, R., and Weng, T. (2014). Revenge of the “sit” II: does lifestyle impact neuronal and cognitive health through distinct mechanisms associated with sedentary behavior and physical activity? Ment. Health Phys. Act. 7, 9–24. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2014.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, M. W., Nagamatsu, L. S., Liu-Ambrose, T., and Kramer, A. F. (2011). Exercise, brain, and cognition across the life span. J. Appl. Physiol. (1985) 111, 1505–1513. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00210.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, E. M., Kemmler, G., Deisenhammer, E. A., Fleischhacker, W. W., and Delazer, M. (2003). Sex differences in cognitive functions. Pers. Individ. Dif. 35, 863. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00288-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wendel-Vos, G. C., Schuit, A. J., Saris, W. H. M., and Kromhout, D. (2003). Reproducibility and relative validity of the short questionnaire to assess health-enhancing physical activity. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 56, 1163–1169. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(03)00220-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilhelm, O., Hildebrandt, A., and Oberauer, K. (2013). What is working memory capacity, and how can we measure it? Front. Psychol. 4:433. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winter, B., Breitenstein, C., Mooren, F. C., Voelker, K., Fobker, M., Lechtermann, A., et al. (2007). High impact running improves learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 87, 597–609. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2006.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, K. P. Jr., McHill, A. W., Birks, B. R., Griffin, B. R., Rusterholz, T., and Chinoy, E. D. (2013). Entrainment of the human circadian clock to the natural light-dark cycle. Curr. Biol. 23, 1554–1558. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zilberter, T., and Zilberter, E. Y. (2013). Breakfast and cognition: sixteen effects in nine populations, no single recipe. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:631. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

TABLE A1
www.frontiersin.org

Table A1. Correlation table of all variables included in the analyses.

Keywords: biological lifestyle factors, sedentary behavior, the ALOUD study, path analysis, structural equation modeling, trail making test, N-back task, substitution test

Citation: Gijselaers HJM, Barberà E, Kirschner PA and de Groot RHM (2016) Physical Activity, Sleep, and Nutrition Do Not Predict Cognitive Performance in Young and Middle-Aged Adults. Front. Psychol. 7:642. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00642

Received: 09 December 2015; Accepted: 18 April 2016;
Published: 03 May 2016.

Edited by:

Lorenza S. Colzato, Leiden University, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Juan Lupiáñez, University of Granada, Spain
Louise Anne Corben, Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Australia

Copyright © 2016 Gijselaers, Barberà, Kirschner and de Groot. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hieronymus J. M. Gijselaers, Jerome.Gijselaers@ou.nl; h.gijselaers@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.