Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 28 July 2016
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

Smartphone-Based Psychotherapeutic Micro-Interventions to Improve Mood in a Real-World Setting

  • 1Division of Clinical Psychology and Epidemiology, Department of Psychology, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
  • 2Faculty of Medicine, Ruhr-University Bochum, Bochum, Germany
  • 3Department of Brain and Cognitive Engineering, Korea University, Seoul, South Korea
  • 4Division of Clinical Psychology and Psychiatry, Department of Psychology, University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland
  • 5Department of Radiology, Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
  • 6Incheon St. Mary's Hospital, The Catholic University of Korea, Incheon, South Korea

Background: Using mobile communication technology as new personalized approach to treat mental disorders or to more generally improve quality of life is highly promising. Knowledge about intervention components that target key psychopathological processes in terms of transdiagnostic psychotherapy approaches is urgently needed. We explored the use of smartphone-based micro-interventions based on psychotherapeutic techniques, guided by short video-clips, to elicit mood changes.

Method: As part of a larger neurofeedback study, all subjects—after being randomly assigned to an experimental or control neurofeedback condition—underwent daily smartphone-based micro-interventions for 13 consecutive days. They were free to choose out of provided techniques, including viscerosensory attention, emotional imagery, facial expression, and contemplative repetition. Changes in mood were assessed in real world using the Multidimensional Mood State Questionnaire (scales: good–bad, GB; awake–tired, AT; and calm–nervous, CN).

Results: Twenty-seven men participated on at least 11 days and were thus included in the analyses. Altogether, they underwent 335, generally well-tolerated, micro-intervention sessions, with viscerosensory attention (178 sessions, 53.13%) and contemplative repetition (68 sessions, 20.30%) being the most frequently applied techniques. Mixed models indicated that subjects showed better mood [GB: b = 0.464, 95%confidence interval (CI) [0.068, 0.860], t(613.3) = 2.298, p = 0.022] and became more awake [AT: b = 0.514, 95%CI [0.103, 0.925], t(612.4) = 2.456, p = 0.014] and calmer [CN: b = 0.685, 95%CI [0.360, 1.010], t(612.3) = 4.137, p < 0.001] from pre- to post-micro-intervention. These mood improvements from pre- to post-micro-intervention were associated with changes in mood from the 1st day until the last day with regard to GB mood (r = 0.614, 95%CI [0.297, 0.809], p < 0.001), but not AT mood (r = 0.279, 95%CI [−0.122, 0.602], p = 0.167) and CN mood (r = 0.277, 95%CI [0.124, 0.601], p = 0.170).

Discussion: Our findings provide evidence for the applicability of smartphone-based micro-interventions eliciting short-term mood changes, based on techniques used in psychotherapeutic approaches, such as mindfulness-based psychotherapy, transcendental meditation, and other contemplative therapies. The results encourage exploring these techniques' capability to improve mood in randomized controlled studies and patients. Smartphone-based micro-interventions are promising to modify mood in real-world settings, complementing other psychotherapeutic interventions, in line with the precision medicine approach. The here presented data were collected within a randomized trial, registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (Identifier: NCT01921088) https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT01921088.

Introduction

Mental disorders are one of the leading global causes of disability (Murray et al., 2012). Besides the personal suffering, their direct and indirect economic costs are tremendous (Wittchen et al., 2011; Olesen et al., 2012). A prominent consortium of researchers, advocates, and clinicians identified key “grand challenges in global mental health” in terms of major research priorities for improving the lives of people with mental illnesses around the world (Collins et al., 2011). Notably, one of the prioritized goals is to improve treatments and expand access to mental health care, with the development of mobile and Internet technologies to increase access to evidence-based care being among the top challenges (Collins et al., 2011). This need is underscored by the fact that in countries, regardless of their economic status, the demand for individual face-to-face psychotherapy is already exceeding or will exceed mental health service supply in the future (Kazdin and Blase, 2011). Therefore, new forms of treatment are required that can complement or expand our current approaches in treating people who suffer from mental disorders (Kazdin and Blase, 2011; Kostkova, 2015).

To this end, Internet-based psychotherapies have received considerable attention during the past decade, lowering the barrier to access mental health service. Most studies indicated that Internet-delivered interventions were efficacious in achieving positive behavioral change or symptom reduction, with no clear evidence of superiority or inferiority as compared to face-to-face interventions (Cuijpers et al., 2010; Griffiths et al., 2010; Richards and Richardson, 2012; Andersson et al., 2014; Riper et al., 2014; Ebert et al., 2015; Richards et al., 2015; Kuester et al., 2016; Melioli et al., 2016; Olthuis et al., 2016; Zachariae et al., 2016).

The advent of mobile information technologies has taken this low-barrier approach to the next level. In the year 2020, 70% of the world's population will use a smartphone (Ericsson, 2015). The core features of smartphones and other mobile devices are that they are running most of the time, are used in a variety of situations during daily life, and ensure a broad reachability of their users beyond calls, e-mails, short messaging, or instant messaging. Unlike the dissemination of many other technologies, the rapid uptake of mobile phones has not been restricted to developed countries (Kay et al., 2011). Furthermore, mobile phones are the preferred means of communication among young people, the age group most unlikely to seek treatment (Oliver et al., 2005). However, some target populations, such as veterans, that experience mental health service gaps may also be more difficult to reach via smartphone-based interventions, as compared to the general population (Klee et al., 2016). Smartphones are increasingly complex, computationally powerful, sensory-rich, and integrated with social networking (Morris and Aguilera, 2012). These factors make them ideal for the delivery of mental health information, digital psychotherapeutic techniques and support anywhere, in real-time and when needed, the latter identified amongst others using sensors integrated in the smartphone (McClernon and Roy Choudhury, 2013). This is in line with the “precision medicine approach,” aiming to provide the right treatment, at the right time, and for the right person (Insel, 2014; Collins and Varmus, 2015). Integrating smartphones in mental healthcare provides a wealth of opportunities, including to overcome the innovation gap by allowing for “disruptive innovation” (Bower and Christensen, 1995), and to provide the basis for new, personalized forms of treatment (Ehrenreich et al., 2011; Zeevi et al., 2015).

Modifying mood or inducing certain mood states in the laboratory, using different approaches in non-clinical samples, has a long-standing history in psychological research (Velten, 1968; Martin, 1990; Schaefer et al., 2010). However, there are only few studies that examined the use of exclusively smartphone-based interventions to modify mood or affective states in healthy populations (e.g., Cipresso et al., 2012); which is in contrast to the large number of studies using smartphones for mood assessment (e.g., Asselbergs et al., 2016). However, a better understanding on how smartphones may be used to modify mood in healthy subjects may provide an important basis for its future application in clinical samples.

Notably, even though an increasing number of mobile applications (apps) that claim to target mental health are available in software repositories (Mani et al., 2015; Nicholas et al., 2015; Shen et al., 2015), as yet, studies that evaluate the effects of applying smartphones as a means of behavior modification are relatively scarce (Donker et al., 2013; Mohr et al., 2013a; Harrison and Goozee, 2014; Mani et al., 2015; Olff, 2015; Torous and Powell, 2015; Bakker et al., 2016). Initial studies provide evidence that smartphone-based interventions have the potential to reduce symptoms of mental disorders, such as anxiety, depression, schizophrenia, and substance use disorders (Watts et al., 2013; Ben-Zeev et al., 2014; Gustafson et al., 2014; Ly et al., 2014; Ahmedani et al., 2015). Further, there is first evidence that mobile technology, including smartphone-based applications, can boost the efficacy of psychotherapy and behavioral interventions (Lindhiem et al., 2015). In sum, further research on smartphone-based interventions in non-clinical samples is highly warranted, and may provide an important basis for future studies and applications, aiming at improving and facilitating prevention and treatment of mental disorders, which has the potential to complement established treatment approaches, serving great clinical and societal relevance.

One particular challenge in the field of mobile mental health research is the mismatch of the paces of research and technology development, with rather long timeframes of classical randomized controlled trials (RCTs), the gold standard of research designs to determine the efficacy of an intervention, with a median duration of more than 5 years from initial enrollment to publication and much longer timeframes until implementation into routine care (Ioannidis, 1998; Riley et al., 2013; Clough and Casey, 2015). This has led to the call for new frameworks and refinement of mobile mental health research (Kumar et al., 2013; Riley et al., 2013; Mohr et al., 2013a,b; Ben-Zeev et al., 2015; Clough and Casey, 2015; Nicholas et al., 2015; Bakker et al., 2016). Classical RCTs evaluate a well-circumscribed intervention; hence modifications of the intervention require conducting a new RCT. One solution to this problem, we believe, is to evaluate core psychotherapeutic components and key features of interventions, which can then guide the assembly of the intervention as a whole, if desired still followed by an RCT. To this end, studies that focus on the evaluation of important elements, characteristics, and principles of smartphone-based interventions, starting with non-clinical samples and later being applied to patients, may be of great importance (Mohr et al., 2014; Alkhaldi et al., 2016; Bakker et al., 2016).

The idea to focus on core intervention components is in line with transdiagnostic treatment approaches, which center on core disease mechanisms to improve the understanding and treatment of mental disorders (Wilamowska et al., 2010; Thompson-Hollands et al., 2014; Newby et al., 2015, 2016). One central target of psychotherapeutic interventions is the improvement of mood, with mood disturbances being the key symptom of a variety of mental disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Furthermore, mood plays a key role in the quality of daily life, and influences personal and social adjustment and physical health, social interactions, and problem solving (Fredrickson, 2004; Shallcross et al., 2010). Hence, the advancement of easily applicable interventions to improve mood is of paramount importance.

Our goal was to explore in a real-world setting, in a non-clinical sample, the use of smartphone-based micro-interventions and related changes in mood. We thereby applied micro-interventions in form of psychotherapeutic techniques that have already been used as components of face-to-face psychotherapy (see Paredes et al., 2014), guided by short video-clips of <5 min duration. More specifically, we aimed at estimating changes in mood and hypothesized that mood would improve from pre- to post-micro-intervention. Furthermore, we evaluated whether these changes were related to changes in mood from the first to the last micro-intervention day, and finally, whether they varied over time and between techniques. The analyzed data were collected from 13 daily micro-intervention sessions, as part of a larger neurofeedback study, in which two real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging neurofeedback (RT-fMRI NF) sessions were conducted, one before all daily micro-intervention sessions and one after, separated by 14 days.

Materials and Methods

Outline of the Study Procedure

Overall Study Procedure

The data presented here were collected within a randomized trial, registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (Identifier: NCT01921088) https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT01921088. The aim of this larger study was to assess the application of real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging neurofeedback (RT-fMRI NF) to modulate the response to an acute stressor in form of the Stroop color word interference task. RT-fMRI NF is a type of self-regulation technique that provides an individual with feedback about specific brain activity using functional magnetic resonance imaging in connection with a related behavior; The underlying assumption at the core of this practice is that through RT-fMRI NF a subject can learn to regulate neural activity and related mental functions (see Thibault et al., 2016).

The institutional review board of Korea University approved the study protocol. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was conducted between August and October 2013 at the facilities of Korea University, Seoul, Republic of Korea [Resource Identifier (RRID): SCR_004095].

The whole study consisted of three laboratory visits and 13 days of ambulatory smartphone-based micro-interventions using psychotherapeutic strategies, the latter following the second laboratory visit on which the RT-fMRI NF procedure was applied for the first time (see Figure 1; for a brief overview of the whole study, please refer to Supplementary Material Data Sheet 1). The data presented here were collected during the preliminary testing day and the smartphone-based micro-interventions, with the exception of the feedback on the micro-interventions that was collected before the RT-fMRI NF procedure on experiment day 2. First, we screened subjects interested in study participation during a telephone interview for any history of neurological or mental disorders and invited those eligible to a laboratory visit, the preliminary testing day, on which we verified whether subjects met all eligibility criteria (see below). Subjects fulfilling eligibility criteria and interested in study participation were asked to provide additional data via questionnaires, were instructed in psychotherapeutic techniques (see section below) to be practiced in different phases of the study, and invited to two further laboratory visits (14 days apart from each other) for a RT-fMRI NF experiment. Between these two experiment days, subjects participated in smartphone-based micro-interventions, during which they practiced the psychotherapeutic techniques they had previously learned on the preliminary testing day and experiment day 1, respectively.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Outline of the larger study. The data presented here were collected during the preliminary testing day and the smartphone-based micro-interventions, with the exception of the feedback on the micro-interventions that was collected before the RT-fMRI NF procedure on experiment day 2. *We instructed participants in four psychotherapeutic techniques, first at the preliminary testing day, then at the beginning of the session at experiment day 1, and briefly reiterated these instructions at the end of this session; RT-fMRI NF, real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging neurofeedback.

Preliminary Testing Day

At the preliminary testing day, we first outlined the whole study procedure to the subjects and collected their written informed consent. Then, we had them practice four psychotherapeutic techniques (for details, see below), which they later applied during the RT-fMRI NF experiment and the micro-interventions. In this first introduction to the techniques, we used detailed instructions and handed out copies with the written instructions to the participants, so that they could follow the text while we explained the techniques. Next, we explained them all other tasks relevant for the RT-fMRI NF experiment procedure (details available from the authors on request). We then asked the participants to fill in a set of questionnaires and checklists to verify their eligibility to the experiment and gather additional information (e.g., sociodemographic data). A detailed description of those questionnaires relevant for this publication is given below. The experimenter then looked through the results and decided upon inclusion of participants. In case of inclusion, the experimenter and subject made an appointment for the next visit at the laboratory for experiment day 1 (6 weeks later at maximum).

Psychotherapeutic Techniques

We instructed the participants in four psychotherapeutic techniques, first at the preliminary testing day, then at the beginning of the session at experiment day 1, and briefly reiterated these instructions at the end of this session. We told the participants that they might find these techniques useful to accomplish the upcoming tasks during the RT-fMRI NF experiment in terms of modulating their brain activity as well as their stress level. The following four techniques were instructed: (i) viscerosensory attention, (ii) emotional imagery, (iii) facial expression, and (iv) contemplative repetition. Additionally, participants were allowed to use (v) any other individual technique that they felt would be helpful. A brief outline of the techniques, as provided at the end of experiment day 1, is depicted in Table 1. In brief, (i) viscerosensory attention consisted of shifting attention toward vs. away from bodily sensations, for example heartbeat or breathing; (ii) emotional imagery consisted of imagining emotionally positive (e.g., great holidays, a beloved person), negative (e.g., a stressful exam, a conflict) or neutral (e.g., a bus ride, reading the newspaper) situations; (iii) facial expression consisted of making different emotional facial expressions, e.g., a happy, angry, or neutral face; and (iv) contemplative repetition consisted of repeating a short simple sentence or word over and over again, or slowly and repeatedly counting from 1 to 10. The shifting between different extremities, as instructed for viscerosensory attention, emotional imagery, and facial expression, was to exploit a preferably large scope of modifiability. To ensure that subjects well remembered the techniques for application during the smartphone-based micro-interventions, at the end of experiment day 1, we asked subjects (i) to take some time to vividly remember the technique that they had just applied in the scanner and that worked best for them, and to briefly describe this technique in written form; (ii) to think of and write down a keyword that might help them to call up this technique once they would apply it during the subsequent micro-intervention sessions; and (iii) to think of a picture that might help them to recall this technique during the micro-intervention sessions, and to describe it in words or draft it. All four psychotherapeutic techniques have been shown to be related to changes in mood (Kleinke et al., 1998; Holmes et al., 2006; Lane et al., 2007; Pollatos et al., 2015), with potential for the treatment of mental disorders (Ito et al., 2001; Holmes et al., 2007; Orme-Johnson and Barnes, 2014; Lin et al., 2015).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Description of techniques applied during the micro-interventions.

Smartphone-Based Micro-Interventions

To familiarize the participants with the smartphone-based micro-intervention, we asked all subjects to undergo one micro-intervention session for training purposes, while still in the laboratory at the end of experiment day 1. Data collected during this training session were not included in our analyses.

On the 13 days between experiment day 1 and 2, each participant underwent one session of smartphone-based micro-intervention per day during their daily life, in which he applied one of the psychotherapeutic techniques outlined above. We instructed subjects to use their own smartphones for participating in the micro-intervention sessions (see Supplementary Material Table 1 for additional information on smartphone types, operating systems, and Internet browsers used). Subjects were free to choose the time of day at which they underwent the micro-intervention session. The time window during which the subjects had to undergo the daily micro-intervention session started each day at 0800 h when they received the invitation-e-mail including the personalized and day-specific hyperlink for access to the micro-intervention session. This hyperlink expired at 0300 h on the following day. In addition to the daily invitation-e-mail at 0800 h, subjects received a reminder-e-mail at 2000 h if they had not yet participated since the last invitation.

We used EFS Survey 10.0 (Questback GmbH, Berlin, Germany) to conduct the smartphone-based micro-interventions, including instructions, presentation of a video-clip, and collection of questionnaire data, as well as for automatically sending the invitation- and reminder-e-mails.

The detailed procedure of each session was as follows: (1) Subjects used their smartphones to connect via internet browser, using a personalized hyperlink provided in the daily invitation or reminder e-mails, to the server hosted by Questback. (2) We instructed the subjects by text display to seek a quiet place allowing them to concentrate on the micro-intervention, and to ensure having a stable Internet connection. Furthermore, we instructed them that the end of the micro-intervention would be signalized by a sound, and that they should therefore ensure to plug in their headphones or set the loudspeakers of their smartphone on high volume, if possible, and that alternatively, the end of the micro-intervention would also be recognizable by visual cues. (3) We asked the subjects to enter their individual subject ID that we had previously provided, as well as a self-generated personal code that they had already generated during the preliminary testing day. This code allowed verifying subject identity. (4) Subjects responded to the Multidimensional Mood State Questionnaire (MDMQ), described in more detail below, and the self-assessment manikin (SAM) scales (Bradley and Lang, 1994). (5) We instructed the subjects to prepare for the micro-intervention, including (i) asking them to remember the technique that they successfully applied during experiment day 1 and telling them that they should use this technique on each of the daily micro-intervention sessions, (ii) instructing them that a micro-intervention session would consist of two rounds lasting 2 min each, interrupted by a pause of 30 s and that in order to start with the session, they should click on the “play”-button of the video player; (iii) asking them—if their Internet connection was weak—to click on the “stop”-button to wait until the player had completely loaded the video, then to reset the video, and to the start the video by clicking on “play” again; and (iv) informing them that the end of the micro-intervention session was signalized by a sound and visually announced in the video, and instructing them not to click on “Continue” before they heard the sound or before the end of the video was reached, as this is important to ensure a standardized duration of the session for each participant and on each day; (v) After this, we asked subjects to select the psychotherapeutic technique they wanted to use during this session (for details, see previous sections). (6) Then, subjects underwent the micro-intervention by following the instructions provided within a short video-clip (duration each: ~4 min 40 s), presented according to the technique that they wanted to apply (the video-clips are provided as Supplementary Material Video 1–5; details of the structure and content of the video-clips are as Supplementary Material Data Sheet 2; additional information regarding the video files as Supplementary Material Data Sheet 3). (7) Subjects again responded to the MDMQ and the SAM scales. (8) Then, subjects replied to two questions related to the micro-intervention session: first, they were asked how successful their session was today, with possible replies on a 5-level scale ranging from −2 (much less than expected/very bad) to +2 (much more than expected/very good). Second, they were asked how they could optimize their micro-intervention (e.g., conditions, motivation, timing, etc.), with an open answer format. The aim of this second question was to guide subjects toward individual optimization of their personal micro-intervention. (9) The session finished by thanking them for their participation in today's session and reminding them of the next micro-intervention session on the subsequent day (or of experiment day 2 on the last day of micro-intervention sessions).

For each page that EFS provided, it recorded a time-stamp, from which we were able to derive date and time of each micro-intervention session.

Assessment Instruments

Assessment of Inclusion/Exclusion Criteria

We applied a set of well-established questionnaires (presented as paper-pencil questionnaires or electronically) to gather information from the participants along the study. Further, we used a set of short checklists to collect additional information, such as data regarding eligibility criteria and feedback regarding the smartphone-based micro-interventions.

To verify the eligibility criterion “right handedness” and the exclusion criterion “color-blindness,” we asked the study participants to fill in the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (EHI; Oldfield, 1971) and the Ishihara test for color-blindness (Ishihara and Force, 1943), respectively, on the preliminary testing day.

Assessment of Mood and Feedback on Micro-Intervention Sessions

We applied the 12-item MDMQ to assess current mood on three dimensions ranging from good to bad (GB), awake to tired (AT), and calm to nervous (CN). The MDMQ is the English version of the German Mehrdimensionale Befindlichkeitsfragebogen (MDBF; Steyer et al., 1997; Steyer, 2014), which is a well-established tool for the assessment of current mood, with very good psychometric properties, especially suited for repeated measures within short intervals. For each dimension, a score is calculated, ranging from 4 to 24. Depending on the dimension, high scores suggest positive affectivity, wakefulness, and calmness, respectively. We applied the MDMQ twice during each smartphone-based micro-intervention session, both before and after subjects practiced the psychotherapeutic technique.

We obtained feedback regarding the smartphone-based micro-intervention sessions at the beginning of experiment day 2, asking the subjects if they agreed with the four statements displayed in Supplementary Material Table 2. Additionally, participants were encouraged to provide further comments regarding the micro-intervention sessions.

Two researchers (AB and JA) independently entered all data from paper-pencil questionnaires into electronic spreadsheets, and a third researcher (ES) crosschecked their entries.

Participants

We recruited participants from the student body of the Korea University. Advertisements for the study were posted on the university website and a local bulletin board. Participants had to fulfill the following eligibility criteria, which were based on the requirements of the larger RT-fMRI NF study: male, age 18–65 years, right-handed, no color-blindness, no history of cardiovascular or neurological diseases or mental disorders, sufficient English language skills to follow the experimental instructions, and self-reported at least minimal familiarity with smartphone-use to carry out the micro-interventions. The sample size was determined a priori, based on the requirements of the randomized trial assessing RT-fMRI NF effects, to provide sufficient statistical power to test the main hypotheses of the trial. Samples size estimates were based on previous studies, demonstrating large effect sizes within RT-fMRI NF paradigms (deCharms et al., 2005; Yoo et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2015). We estimated, by calculating a priori power analysis (using G* Power 3, Faul et al., 2007, RRID: SCR_013726) that with n = 14 subjects in each condition, effects of d = 1.0 can be detected with sufficient power (1–ß > 0.80; given α = 0.05, one-sided test).

After completion of the study, each subject received 60,000 KRW (≈57 USD) in compensation for his participation. The compensation was split in three parts, for the participation at experiment day 1, smartphone-based micro-interventions, and experiment day 2, and paid out in part if the subject did not take part in the complete study.

Statistical Analyses

We checked the data for distribution properties and verified normality by inspecting histograms and qq-plots. For descriptive analyses, we calculated means and standard deviations for continuous normally distributed variables and absolute and relative frequencies for categorical variables with categories outlined in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Characteristics of the study sample (N = 27).

As the values of the scales of the MDMQ were approximately normally distributed, transformation was not required. Twenty-six of the 27 participants applied the same psychotherapeutic technique across micro-intervention days, but one subject extensively varied the psychotherapeutic technique across days. Therefore, we did not enter psychotherapeutic technique as factor at the level of the micro-intervention day, but entered it at the participant level. To this end, we assigned each of the above-mentioned 26 participants to the psychotherapeutic technique category that they used, and created an additional category “mixed techniques” for the subject that extensively varied the psychotherapeutic techniques. As in four of the resulting six categories there were only few subjects (“emotional imagery”, n = 3; “facial expression”, n = 2; “other technique”, n = 2; and “mixed techniques”, n = 1), we collapsed these four categories, leading to the trichotomous variable “psychotherapeutic technique” with the three levels “viscerosensory attention” (n = 14), “contemplative repetition” (n = 5), and “other” (n = 8).

Each scale of the MDMQ was entered as outcome variable in separate linear mixed-effects models (Singer and Willett, 2003), to estimate mood changes from pre- to post-micro-intervention and across micro-intervention days, as well as differences in mood changes from pre- to post-micro-intervention across micro-intervention days and between psychotherapeutic techniques. Furthermore, we adjusted analyses for the condition (experimental or sham/control condition) to which subjects had been assigned within the larger randomized controlled trial from which the data was derived. Hence, we entered the following predictors into the model: (i) “pre- vs. post-micro-intervention”, (ii) “micro-intervention day” (dimensional, day 1–13), iii) “psychotherapeutic technique” (trichotomous, see above), and iv) “condition” (experimental vs. sham/control), as well as the interactions of “pre- vs. post micro-intervention” with “micro-intervention day”, “psychotherapeutic technique”, and “condition”. We entered random intercept and random slope parameters when this improved model fit, with the latter being assessed based on Akaike's Information Criterion (AIC; Singer and Willett, 2003). Furthermore, we tested whether entering a higher order polynomial of the variable “micro-intervention day” would improve model fit. We first fitted models including main and interaction effects, as outlined above. In case of the interaction effects being statistically not significant, we repeated analyses with main effects only, leading to the main results reported. We calculated 95% confidence intervals (CIs) using the Wald method. For the main mixed model analyses, we included all subjects that took part in at least 3 micro-intervention sessions. Mixed models accommodated further missing data.

To test whether average mood improvements from pre- to post-micro-interventions were associated with overall baseline mood improvements over all intervention days, we calculated Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients between the mean of the non-missing mood changes from pre- to post-micro-interventions averaged across days 2–12 and change in mood from pre-micro-intervention day 1 to pre-micro-intervention day 13, separately for GB, AT, and CN mood.

All tests were two-tailed and we set the significance level at 0.05. We used the statistical software package R (version 3.2.3 and above; R Project for Statistical Computing, RRID: SCR_001905; R Core Team, 2015) for all data analyses and statistical testing, including the packages to conduct the mixed models, “lme4” (Bates et al., 2014) and “optimx” (Nash and Varadhan, 2011), as well as further packages, required for data preparation and descriptive statistics “car” (Fox and Weisberg, 2011), “dplyr” (Wickham and Francois, 2015), “haven” (Wickham and Miller, 2015), “Hmisc” (Frank and Dupont, 2015), “lmerTest” (Kuznetsova et al., 2015), “lsmeans” (Lenth, 2016), “pastecs” (Grosjean and Ibanez, 2014), and “tidyr” (Wickham, 2015).

Results

Flow and Descriptive Information on Study Participants

The flowchart of participants is provided in Figure 2. From the 31 subjects included in the study, one participant did not show up on experiment day 1 and hence neither received instructions for nor participated in any smartphone-based micro-intervention. Three other subjects did participate in <3 micro-intervention sessions (one subject participated in 1 session and two subjects participated in 2 sessions) and were hence excluded from further analyses. All subjects were males of Korean nationality. Characteristics of the study sample on which the analyses are based (N = 27) are provided in Table 2. (For the sake of transparency, characteristics of the full study sample (N = 30) is provided in Supplementary Material Table 3).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Participant flow through study.

Descriptive Information on Smartphone-Based Micro-Intervention Sessions

The 27 subjects participated in 336 out of 351 possible smartphone-based micro-intervention sessions in total (95.73%). The mean number of micro-intervention sessions per subject was 12.44 (standard deviation, SD = 0.80, Range: 11–13) [respective information regarding the sample of N = 30 participants, who in total participated in 342 out of 390 possible micro-intervention sessions (87.69%) is provided in Supplementary Material Table 3]. 26 sessions (7.74%) were conducted at 0800 h or later but before 0900 h, 63 sessions (18.75%) were conducted at 0900 h or later but before 1200 h, 54 sessions (16.07%) were conducted at 1200 h or later but before 1500 h, 44 sessions (13.10%) were conducted at 1500 h or later but before 1800 h, 69 sessions (20.54%) were conducted at 1800 h or later but before 2100 h, 69 sessions (20.54%) were conducted at 2100 h or later but before 0000 h, and 11 sessions (3.27%) were conducted at 0000 h or later but before 0300 h. The relative frequency of psychotherapeutic techniques applied during the micro-intervention sessions is depicted in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Relative frequency of psychotherapeutic techniques applied during the smartphone-based micro-intervention sessions. Information on selected technique missing for one session; s, number of sessions.

Main Results from the Mixed Model Analyses

Changes in mood from pre- to post-micro-intervention and across micro-intervention days are depicted in Figure 4 (MDMQ good-bad mood; Figures 4B,A, respectively), Figure 5 (MDMQ awake-tired mood; Figures 5B,A, respectively), and Figure 6 (MDMQ calm-nervous mood; Figures 6B,A, respectively). Mood changes stratified according to psychotherapeutic technique are depicted in Figure 7. All mixed models included a random intercept and slope of day varying among subjects. Entering “micro-intervention day” as higher order than linear polynomial did not improve model fit. In all three mixed models (with GB, AT, and CN as outcome) none of the interaction terms were statistically significant (see Supplementary Material Table 4 for related statistical parameters) and they were hence removed from the models. This means that there was no indication that changes in mood from pre- to post-micro-intervention differed across micro-intervention days, between psychotherapeutic techniques, and between conditions.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Good–bad mood (predicted marginal means, 95%CIs) pre- and post-micro-intervention, by the micro-intervention day (A) and across all micro-intervention days (B). *b = 0.464, 95%CI: 0.068–0.860, t(613.3) = 2.298, p = 0.022; Higher values indicate better mood; In (A), for the pre-micro-intervention values, the lower boundaries of the 95%CIs are depicted, for the post-micro-intervention values, the upper boundaries of the 95%CIs are depicted; CI, confidence interval; MDMQ, Multidimensional Mood State Questionnaire; PMM, predicted marginal means.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Awake–tired mood (predicted marginal means, 95%CIs) pre- and post-micro-intervention, by the micro-intervention day (A) and across all micro-intervention days (B). *b = 0.514, 95%CI: 0.103–0.924, t(612.4) = 2.456, p = 0.014; Higher values indicate being more awake; In (A), for the pre-micro-intervention values, the lower boundaries of the 95%CIs are depicted, for the post-micro-intervention values, the upper boundaries of the 95%CIs are depicted; CI, confidence interval; MDMQ, Multidimensional Mood State Questionnaire; PMM, predicted marginal means.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Calm–nervous mood (predicted marginal means, 95%CIs) pre- and post-micro-intervention, by the micro-intervention day (A) and across all micro-intervention days (B). *b = 0.685, 95%CI: 0.360–1.010, t(612.3) = 4.137, p < 0.001; Higher values indicate being calmer; In (A), for the pre-micro-intervention values, the lower boundaries of the 95%CIs are depicted, for the post-micro-intervention values, the upper boundaries of the 95%CIs are depicted; CI, confidence interval; MDMQ, Multidimensional Mood State Questionnaire; PMM, predicted marginal means.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Mood changes (means, 95%CIs) from pre- to post-micro-intervention, stratified according to psychotherapeutic technique. Depending on the scale, higher values indicate better mood, being more awake, and calmer, respectively; CI, confidence interval; s, number of sessions.

With regard to good or bad mood as outcome, mood improved from pre- to post-micro-intervention [b = 0.464, 95%CI [0.068, 0.860], t(613.3) = 2.298, p = 0.022]. Increases in mood across days were statistically non-significant [b = 0.051, 95%CI [–0.039, 0.140], t(26.8) = 1.112, p = 0.276]. With regard to awake–tired (AT) mood as outcome, subjects became more awake from pre- to post-micro-intervention [b = 0.514, 95%CI [0.103, 0.925], t(612.4) = 2.456, p = 0.014], but not across days [b = 0.002, 95%CI [−0.073, 0.077], t(25.5) = 0.048, p = 0.962]. With regard to calm–nervous (CN) mood as outcome, subjects became calmer from pre- to post-micro-intervention [b = 0.685, 95%CI [0.360, 1.010], t(612.3) = 4.137, p < 0.001], but not across days [b = −0.018, 95%CI [−0.088, 0.052], t(26.3) = 0.502, p = 0.620].

Additional Results Regarding Mood Changes, and Participants' Feedback

Average mood improvements from pre- to post-micro-interventions across day 2 to day 12 were significantly associated with an increase in mood pre-micro-interventions from day 1 to day 13 with regard to GB mood (r = 0.614, 95%CI [0.297, 0.809], p < 0.001), but not AT mood (r = 0.279, 95%CI [−0.122, 0.602], p = 0.167) and CN mood (r = 0.277, 95%CI [−0.124, 0.601], p = 0.170) (calculations based on n = 26, due to missing data).

The feedback of the participants (N = 27) regarding the number of days of the smartphone-based micro-intervention revealed that 5 subjects (18.52%) agreed that 2 weeks were too short to be successful, while 10 subjects (37.04%) disagreed (the other 12 subjects were neutral); 13 subjects (48.15%) agreed that 2 weeks were well tolerable, while 3 subjects (11.11%) disagreed (the other 11 subjects were neutral). Regarding the duration of the sessions, 8 subjects (29.63%) agreed that the duration was too short to be successful, while 13 subjects (48.10%) disagreed (among which one subject even “strongly disagreed”; the other 6 subjects were neutral); 18 subjects (66.66%) agreed (among which one subject even “strongly agreed”) that the duration was well tolerable, while 2 subjects (7.40%) disagreed (among which one subject even “strongly disagreed”) (the other seven subjects were neutral). More detailed information on the feedback, as well as respective information based on the sample of participants who at least received the micro-intervention instructions (N = 30) with relative frequency of responses virtually identical to those reported here, are provided in Supplementary Material Table 2.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to scrutinize in a real-world setting the use of smartphone-based micro-interventions in form of psychotherapeutic techniques and related changes in mood in a non-clinical sample. We hypothesized that mood improved from pre- to post micro-intervention sessions. Our hypothesis was confirmed. Subjects reported better mood and being calmer and more awake at post- as compared to pre-micro-intervention. However, there was no indication of increases in mood across days. Notably, greater mood improvements (GB mood) from pre- to post-micro-intervention were associated with overall changes in mood from the 1st day until the last day, which would be in line with micro-interventions incrementally improving mood across days if successful on individual days, even though our study design does not allow inferring causality or making assumptions about the long-term stability of the effects. There was no indication that mood improvements from pre- to post-micro-intervention differed between techniques or across the 13 micro-intervention days; hence there was no evidence for habituation of potential micro-intervention effects.

Participants conducted the vast majority of the requested micro-interventions sessions, and only a minority of subjects provided negative feedback regarding the number of micro-intervention days or the duration of the sessions. This indicates that a repeated application of smartphone-based micro-intervention sessions is generally well tolerated. Still, some individuals reported that they would have preferred a higher or lower number of sessions or a longer or shorter training duration, which indicates that personalization also of these parameters may have the potential to further improve the acceptance of smartphone-based micro-interventions.

Our findings extend previous evidence that short-term interventions, using different strategies, can modify mood in non-clinical samples in well-controlled laboratory settings (Velten, 1968; Martin, 1990; Schaefer et al., 2010), by indicating that this holds true when interventions are applied via smartphone in a real-world setting. They are in line with preliminary laboratory-based evidence that smartphone-based interventions can elicit positive mood states (e.g., Cipresso et al., 2012).

With regard to studies with clinical samples assessing psychotherapeutic face-to-face settings, our findings are in line with evidence that mindfulness-based strategies can improve mood and distress (e.g., Brake et al., 2016), even though the current study was only performed on a non-clinical sample of participants. A recent meta-analysis reported that online mindfulness-based intervention programs of 2–12 weeks duration were effective to reduce symptoms of mental disorders, notably with larger effect sizes for interventions of longer duration (Spijkerman et al., 2016). Furthermore, there is preliminary evidence that smartphone-based mindfulness intervention programs, lasting one to several weeks, may improve mood and reduce stress or symptoms of mental disorders (e.g., Brake et al., 2016). Our findings—notably based on a non-clinical sample of participants—are in line with this observation, indicating that (i) mindfulness-based micro-interventions of only several minutes duration, applied via smartphone, go along with rapid mood improvements, and (ii) if these interventions are successful during daily individual sessions, they are potentially leading to mood improvements across 2 weeks, even though we cannot make any assumptions about potential longer-term effects. Furthermore, our findings are in line with evidence from the field of smoking cessation, indicating that mobile phones (however, primarily via text messaging), have been successfully used to trigger behavior change (Whittaker et al., 2016). Notably, studies on smartphone-based interventions that target mental health related behavior do not always provide evidence that the interventions have the intended effects; in contrast, some interventions may even lead to opposite effects, at least in subgroups (Gajecki et al., 2014).

Our study design does not allow to disentangle potential processes underlying the mood changes observed in our study, and to identify to which extent different features of the intervention may have contributed to the observed mood changes and the overall good engagement of study participants with the digital intervention. One may speculate that the use of prompts and reminders in our study has improved digital engagement, in line with what has been previously shown (Alkhaldi et al., 2016). Further, consistent with previous evidence, the personal encounter between study personnel and the participants preceding the real-world micro-interventions may have enhanced intervention effects and digital engagement (Palmqvist et al., 2007; Spek et al., 2007; Andersson and Cuijpers, 2009; Richards and Richardson, 2012; Baumeister et al., 2014).

Important strengths of this study include, first, the use of psychotherapeutic techniques for which previous evidence indicated potential to improve stress-related processes; second, the participants' individual selection of their preferred techniques and, third, individual selection of training times; fourth, the use of video-clip supported procedures, ensuring a standardized application of the micro-intervention; and fifth, the use of mixed model analyses, taking into account, amongst others, individual mood variations across days.

There are also limitations. First, we did not include a randomized control condition. Therefore, we cannot determine which factors led to the mood related changes, and we cannot exclude changes in mood driven by digital placebo effects (Torous and Firth, 2016), which, however, is an issue not only in our study but in numerous other studies on the effects of psychotherapeutic interventions (Ioannidis, 2016). Future studies should estimate the effects of micro-interventions on mood within larger randomized controlled trials. Second, the data presented here were collected within a larger study. We cannot finally exclude that our findings were influenced by procedures during the preceding study days. However, there was no statistically significant association between the experimental condition participants were assigned to on experiment day 1 and smartphone-based micro-intervention-related mood changes during the 13 real-world sessions, making it rather unlikely that the randomization within the framework of the larger study was of substantial relevance. Third, the study sample was rather homogenous, with all participants being male and the majority being rather experienced using smartphones. Notably, given that males seek less traditional face-to-face treatment for mental health issues than females (Rhodes et al., 2002), males may be of special interest as target group for alternative interventive approaches. Our findings should be generalized with caution, and future studies are needed that target populations of different cultural backgrounds and more heterogeneous with regard to sex, age, educational background, and digital literacy. Fourth, even though participants were of Korean nationality, having Korean as a first language, written study material was provided in English, which was not adapted or normed to the local population. However, all participants had excellent knowledge of written English, and using the English versions of assessment instruments ensured that well-validated versions were applied. Fifth, without follow-up assessment, we cannot draw any conclusion regarding the long-term stability of the mood changes. Finally, we did not randomize the order in which the different techniques were introduced to the participants. Hence, we cannot exclude that order of introduction may have influenced the individual choice of techniques. However, identifying differences in mood changes across psychotherapeutic techniques was not the main goal of our study, and we would have needed a larger sample size and randomized assignment to techniques to further scrutinize this question. Notably, allowing participants to select the technique of their choice increased external validity of our study design, as smartphone-users usually substantially participate in the choice of apps that they apply, and the individual selection may also have improved engagement (Schueller, 2010).

Our findings may have different implications. They suggest the applicability of smartphone-based micro-interventions based on techniques that have been previously applied across a range of therapeutic approaches, including mindfulness-based psychotherapy. If our findings are corroborated in randomized controlled settings and different patient groups, targeted smartphone-based micro-interventions may represent a promising tool to modify mood in real-world settings, as part of more complex behavioral intervention technologies (BITs; Mohr et al., 2014), and complementing other psychotherapeutic interventions within blended treatments (Ly et al., 2015), and in line with the precision medicine approach (Insel, 2014; Collins and Varmus, 2015). Furthermore, they may be used to provide in-the-moment support for non-clinical populations to improve their mood, and allow delivering state-of-the-art psychotherapeutic techniques in a non-stigmatizing fashion to individuals who otherwise would not have access to therapy (Morris et al., 2010).

As mentioned above, future randomized controlled trials are needed to further scrutinize the effects of smartphone-based micro-interventions on mood, including studies addressing in detail the underlying mechanisms. Notably, alternative methodological frameworks, such as the “Continuous Evaluation of Evolving Behavioral Intervention Technologies (CEEBIT)” approach (Mohr et al., 2013b) or the “Person-Based Approach to Intervention Development” (Yardley et al., 2015), may allow evaluating the micro-interventions in different application contexts and help to further tailor applications based on the micro-interventions toward the target user population. In this context, we acknowledge that techniques used in the present study (i.e., the micro-interventions based on video-clips accessed via smartphones) may be considered rather “conventional,” in light of the rapid technological advancements made in smartphone technology. More recently, more advanced techniques, such as game-like applications, have been used for delivering smartphone-based psychotherapeutic interventions (e.g., Franklin et al., 2016). However, we also note that the advantage of our approach is that the video-clips can be easily integrated into more complex interventive “apps” (mobile phone applications) or within the contexts of communication routes, for example social media or messenger services (Dinakar et al., 2015). These approaches, with proper steps taken to safeguard information privacy, may confer low-barrier psychosocial interventions. With regard to technological advances, one may also consider combining the approach with the collection of information based on ambulatory biomarkers (Ben Khelil et al., 2011; Tegethoff et al., 2011; Choi et al., 2014), which may allow the application of micro-interventions based on multi-source information. Besides this, future studies should elucidate emerging questions, such as (i) to which extend the mood changes related to the micro-interventions depend on preceding personal contact with the subjects undergoing the micro-intervention, (ii) to which extend mood changes depend on whether subjects self-selected the type of technique applied, and (iii) whether individual mood changes triggered by a micro-intervention session can be predicted by contextual or time factors (Paredes et al., 2014), which will provide a basis for the further personalization of interventions.

Taken together, we provided evidence that smartphone-based micro-interventions are well-tolerated and go along with improvements in mood. In line with the precision medicine approach, smartphone-based micro-interventions may represent a promising tool to modify mood in real-world settings.

Author Contributions

GM, JL, SY, and MT designed the study; GM, JL, ES, and MT prepared the study material and data acquisition; JL, ES, MO, EJ, and HK recruited participants and acquired the data; ES, AB, and JA entered the data and prepared it for statistical analyses; GM, ES, and AB analyzed the data; GM, JL, and MT interpreted the data; GM wrote the first draft of the manuscript; GM, JL, ES, AB, MO, EJ, HK, JA, SY, and MT critically revised the manuscript for important intellectual content; MT obtained funding; All authors gave final approval of the manuscript version to be published and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Funding

GM, JL, and MT receive funding from the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) within the Global Research Network Program (under project no. 2013S1A2A2035364). MT receives funding from the Swiss National Science Foundation, SNSF (project no. PZ00P1_137023). JL receives funding from the NRF, Ministry of Science, ICT, and Future Planning, Korea (2015R1A2A2A03004462) and the Korean Health Technology R&D Project, Ministry of Health and Welfare, Korea (HI12C1847); GM receives funding from the SNSF (project no. 100014_135328).

Conflict of Interest Statement

GM acts as consultant for Janssen Research & Development, LLC.

The other authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01112

References

Ahmedani, B. K., Crotty, N., Abdulhak, M. M., and Ondersma, S. J. (2015). Pilot feasibility study of a brief, tailored mobile health intervention for depression among patients with chronic pain. Behav. Med. 41, 25–32. doi: 10.1080/08964289.2013.867827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alkhaldi, G., Hamilton, F. L., Lau, R., Webster, R., Michie, S., and Murray, E. (2016). The effectiveness of prompts to promote engagement with digital interventions: a systematic review. J. Med. Internet Res. 18, e6. doi: 10.2196/jmir.4790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (dsm-5). Arlington, TX: American Psychiatric Association.

Andersson, G., and Cuijpers, P. (2009). Internet-based and other computerized psychological treatments for adult depression: a meta-analysis. Cogn. Behav. Ther. 38, 196–205. doi: 10.1080/16506070903318960

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, G., Cuijpers, P., Carlbring, P., Riper, H., and Hedman, E. (2014). Guided internet-based vs. Face-to-face cognitive behavior therapy for psychiatric and somatic disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. World Psychiatry 13, 288–295. doi: 10.1002/wps.20151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asselbergs, J., Ruwaard, J., Ejdys, M., Schrader, N., Sijbrandij, M., and Riper, H. (2016). Mobile phone-based unobtrusive ecological momentary assessment of day-to-day mood: an explorative study. J. Med. Internet Res. 18, e72. doi: 10.2196/jmir.5505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakker, D., Kazantzis, N., Rickwood, D., and Rickard, N. (2016). Mental health smartphone apps: review and evidence-based recommendations for future developments. JMIR Ment. Health 3, e7. doi: 10.2196/mental.4984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. arXiv preprint arXiv:1406.5823. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/

Baumeister, H., Reichler, L., Munzinger, M., and Lin, J. (2014). The impact of guidance on internet-based mental health interventions—a systematic review. Internet Interv. 1, 205–215. doi: 10.1016/j.invent.2014.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben Khelil, M., Tegethoff, M., Meinlschmidt, G., Jamey, C., Ludes, B., and Raul, J. S. (2011). Simultaneous measurement of endogenous cortisol, cortisone, dehydroepiandrosterone, and dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate in nails by use of uplc-ms-ms. Anal. Bioanal. Chem. 401, 1153–1162. doi: 10.1007/s00216-011-5172-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Zeev, D., Brenner, C. J., Begale, M., Duffecy, J., Mohr, D. C., and Mueser, K. T. (2014). Feasibility, acceptability, and preliminary efficacy of a smartphone intervention for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 40, 1244–1253. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben-Zeev, D., Schueller, S. M., Begale, M., Duffecy, J., Kane, J. M., and Mohr, D. C. (2015). Strategies for mhealth research: lessons from 3 mobile intervention studies. Adm. Policy Ment. Health 42, 157–167. doi: 10.1007/s10488-014-0556-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bower, J. L., and Christensen, C. M. (1995). Disruptive technologies: catching the wave. Harv. Bus. Rev. 73, 43–53.

Bradley, M. M., and Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 25, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brake, C. A., Sauer-Zavala, S., Boswell, J. F., Gallagher, M. W., Farchione, T. J., and Barlow, D. H. (2016). Mindfulness-based exposure strategies as a transdiagnostic mechanism of change: an exploratory alternating treatment design. Behav. Ther. 47, 225–238. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2015.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, S., Kim, S., Yang, J.-S., Lee, J.-H., Joo, C., and Jung, H.-I. (2014). Real-time measurement of human salivary cortisol for the assessment of psychological stress using a smartphone. Sens. Bio-Sens. Res. 2, 8–11. doi: 10.1016/j.sbsr.2014.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cipresso, P., Serino, S., Villani, D., Repetto, C., Sellitti, L., Albani, G., et al. (2012). Is your phone so smart to affect your state? An exploratory study based on psychophysiological measures. Neurocomputing 84, 23–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neucom.2011.12.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clough, B. A., and Casey, L. M. (2015). The smart therapist: a look to the future of smartphones and mhealth technologies in psychotherapy. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pr. 46, 147. doi: 10.1037/pro0000011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, F. S., and Varmus, H. (2015). A new initiative on precision medicine. N. Engl. J. Med. 372, 793–795. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp1500523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, P. Y., Patel, V., Joestl, S. S., March, D., Insel, T. R., Daar, A. S., et al. (2011). Grand challenges in global mental health. Nature 475, 27–30. doi: 10.1038/475027a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuijpers, P., Donker, T., van Straten, A., Li, J., and Andersson, G. (2010). Is guided self-help as effective as face-to-face psychotherapy for depression and anxiety disorders? A systematic review and meta-analysis of comparative outcome studies. Psychol. Med. 40, 1943–1957. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710000772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

deCharms, R. C., Maeda, F., Glover, G. H., Ludlow, D., Pauly, J. M., Soneji, D., et al. (2005). Control over brain activation and pain learned by using real-time functional mri. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 18626–18631. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0505210102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinakar, K., Chen, J., Lieberman, H., Picard, R., and Filbin, R. (2015). “Mixed-initiative real-time topic modeling & visualization for crisis counseling,” in IUI'15 Proceedings of the 20th International Conference on Intelligent User Interfaces (New York, NY: ACM), 417–426. Available online at: http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=2701395

Donker, T., Petrie, K., Proudfoot, J., Clarke, J., Birch, M. R., and Christensen, H. (2013). Smartphones for smarter delivery of mental health programs: a systematic review. J. Med. Internet Res. 15, e247. doi: 10.2196/jmir.2791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebert, D. D., Zarski, A. C., Christensen, H., Stikkelbroek, Y., Cuijpers, P., Berking, M., et al. (2015). Internet and computer-based cognitive behavioral therapy for anxiety and depression in youth: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled outcome trials. PLoS ONE 10:e0119895. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0119895

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehrenreich, B., Righter, B., Rocke, D. A., Dixon, L., and Himelhoch, S. (2011). Are mobile phones and handheld computers being used to enhance delivery of psychiatric treatment? A systematic review. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 199, 886–891. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182349e90

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ericsson (2015). Ericsson Mobility Report [Online]. Available online at: http://www.ericsson.com/res/docs/2015/ericsson-mobility-report-june-2015.pdf (Accessed February 14, 2016).

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., and Buchner, A. (2007). G* power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, J., and Weisberg, S. (2011). An {r} Companion to Applied Regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/car/

Frank, E. H. Jr., and Dupont, C. (2015). Hmisc: Harrell Miscellaneous. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/Hmisc/

Franklin, J. C., Fox, K. R., Franklin, C. R., Kleiman, E. M., Ribeiro, J. D., Jaroszewski, A. C., et al. (2016). A brief mobile app reduces nonsuicidal and suicidal self-injury: evidence from three randomized controlled trials. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 84, 544–557. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 359, 1367–1377. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gajecki, M., Berman, A. H., Sinadinovic, K., Rosendahl, I., and Andersson, C. (2014). Mobile phone brief intervention applications for risky alcohol use among university students: a randomized controlled study. Addict. Sci. Clin. Pract. 9:11. doi: 10.1186/1940-0640-9-11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, K. M., Farrer, L., and Christensen, H. (2010). The efficacy of internet interventions for depression and anxiety disorders: A review of randomised controlled trials. Med. J. Aust. 192, S4–S11.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Grosjean, P., and Ibanez, F. (2014). Pastecs: Package for Analysis of Space-Time Ecological Series. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/pastecs/

Gustafson, D. H., McTavish, F. M., Chih, M. Y., Atwood, A. K., Johnson, R. A., Boyle, M. G., et al. (2014). A smartphone application to support recovery from alcoholism: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 566–572. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.4642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, A. M., and Goozee, R. (2014). Psych-related iphone apps. J. Ment. Health 23, 48–50. doi: 10.3109/09638237.2013.869575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, E. A., Arntz, A., and Smucker, M. R. (2007). Imagery rescripting in cognitive behaviour therapy: images, treatment techniques and outcomes. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 38, 297–305. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, E. A., Mathews, A., Dalgleish, T., and Mackintosh, B. (2006). Positive interpretation training: effects of mental imagery versus verbal training on positive mood. Behav. Ther. 37, 237–247. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2006.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Insel, T. R. (2014). The nimh research domain criteria (rdoc) project: precision medicine for psychiatry. Am. J. Psychiatry 171, 395–397. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14020138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ioannidis, J. P. (1998). Effect of the statistical significance of results on the time to completion and publication of randomized efficacy trials. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 279, 281–286. doi: 10.1001/jama.279.4.281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2016). Most psychotherapies do not really work, but those that might work should be assessed in biased studies. Epidemiol. Psychiatr. Sci. doi: 10.1017/S2045796015000888. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishihara, S., and Force, R. A. A. (1943). Ishihara Tests for Colour Blindness. Sydney, NSW: Shephard & Newman.

Ito, L. M., de Araujo, L. A., Tess, V. L., de Barros-Neto, T. P., Asbahr, F. R., and Marks, I. (2001). Self-exposure therapy for panic disorder with agoraphobia: randomised controlled study of external v. Interoceptive self-exposure. Br. J. Psychiatry 178, 331–336. doi: 10.1192/bjp.178.4.331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kay, M., Santos, J., and Takane, M. (2011). Mhealth: New horizons for Health through Mobile Technologies: Based on the Findings of the Second Global Survey on ehealth (Global Observatory for ehealth Series, Volume 3). Geneva: World Health Organization.

Kazdin, A. E., and Blase, S. L. (2011). Rebooting psychotherapy research and practice to reduce the burden of mental illness. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 21–37. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, D. Y., Yoo, S. S., Tegethoff, M., Meinlschmidt, G., and Lee, J. H. (2015). The inclusion of functional connectivity information into fmri-based neurofeedback improves its efficacy in the reduction of cigarette cravings. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 1552–1572. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klee, A., Stacy, M., Rosenheck, R., Harkness, L., and Tsai, J. (2016). Interest in technology-based therapies hampered by access: a survey of veterans with serious mental illnesses. Psychiatr. Rehabil. J. 39, 173–179. doi: 10.1037/prj0000180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleinke, C. L., Peterson, T. R., and Rutledge, T. R. (1998). Effects of self-generated facial expressions on mood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74:272. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.272

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kostkova, P. (2015). Grand challenges in digital health. Front. Public Health 3:134. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2015.00134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuester, A., Niemeyer, H., and Knaevelsrud, C. (2016). Internet-based interventions for posttraumatic stress: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 43, 1–16. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, S., Nilsen, W. J., Abernethy, A., Atienza, A., Patrick, K., Pavel, M., et al. (2013). Mobile health technology evaluation: the mhealth evidence workshop. Am. J. Prev. Med. 45, 228–236. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2013.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., and Christensen, R. H. B. (2015). Lmertest: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lmerTest/

Lane, J. D., Seskevich, J. E., and Pieper, C. F. (2007). Brief meditation training can improve perceived stress and negative mood. Altern. Ther. Health Med. 13, 38–44.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lenth, R. V. (2016). Least-squares means: the {r} package {lsmeans}. J. Stat. Softw. 69, 1–33. doi: 10.18637/jss.v069.i01. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lsmeans/

CrossRef Full Text

Lin, W., Hu, J., and Gong, Y. (2015). Is it helpful for individuals with minor depression to keep smiling? An event-related potentials analysis. Soc. Behav. Pers. 43, 383–396. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2015.43.3.383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindhiem, O., Bennett, C. B., Rosen, D., and Silk, J. (2015). Mobile technology boosts the effectiveness of psychotherapy and behavioral interventions: a meta-analysis. Behav. Modif. 39, 785–804. doi: 10.1177/0145445515595198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ly, K. H., Topooco, N., Cederlund, H., Wallin, A., Bergström, J., Molander, O., et al. (2015). Smartphone-supported versus full behavioural activation for depression: a randomised controlled trial. PLoS ONE 10:e0126559. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ly, K. H., Trüschel, A., Jarl, L., Magnusson, S., Windahl, T., Johansson, R., et al. (2014). Behavioural activation versus mindfulness-based guided self-help treatment administered through a smartphone application: a randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open 4:e003440. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2013-003440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mani, M., Kavanagh, D. J., Hides, L., and Stoyanov, S. R. (2015). Review and evaluation of mindfulness-based iphone apps. JMIR mHealth and uHealth 3:e82. doi: 10.2196/mhealth.4328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, M. (1990). On the induction of mood. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 10, 669–697. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(90)90075-L

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClernon, F. J., and Roy Choudhury, R. (2013). I am your smartphone, and i know you are about to smoke: the application of mobile sensing and computing approaches to smoking research and treatment. Nicotine Tob. Res. 15, 1651–1654. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntt054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melioli, T., Bauer, S., Franko, D. L., Moessner, M., Ozer, F., Chabrol, H., et al. (2016). Reducing eating disorder symptoms and risk factors using the internet: a meta-analytic review. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 49, 19–31. doi: 10.1002/eat.22477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohr, D. C., Burns, M. N., Schueller, S. M., Clarke, G., and Klinkman, M. (2013a). Behavioral intervention technologies: evidence review and recommendations for future research in mental health. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 35, 332–338. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2013.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohr, D. C., Cheung, K., Schueller, S. M., Hendricks Brown, C., and Duan, N. (2013b). Continuous evaluation of evolving behavioral intervention technologies. Am. J. Prev. Med. 45, 517–523. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2013.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohr, D. C., Schueller, S. M., Montague, E., Burns, M. N., and Rashidi, P. (2014). The behavioral intervention technology model: an integrated conceptual and technological framework for ehealth and mhealth interventions. J. Med. Internet Res. 16:e146. doi: 10.2196/jmir.3077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, M. E., and Aguilera, A. (2012). Mobile, social, and wearable computing and the evolution of psychological practice. Prof. Psychol. Res. Pr. 43, 622. doi: 10.1037/a0029041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, M. E., Kathawala, Q., Leen, T. K., Gorenstein, E. E., Guilak, F., DeLeeuw, W., et al. (2010). Mobile therapy: case study evaluations of a cell phone application for emotional self-awareness. J. Med. Internet Res. 12, e10. doi: 10.2196/jmir.1371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, C. J., Vos, T., Lozano, R., Naghavi, M., Flaxman, A. D., Michaud, C., et al. (2012). Disability-adjusted life years (dalys) for 291 diseases and injuries in 21 regions, 1990-2010: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2010. Lancet 380, 2197–2223. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61689-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nash, J. C., and Varadhan, R. (2011). Unifying optimization algorithms to aid software system users: optimx for r. J. Stat. Softw. 43, 1–14. doi: 10.18637/jss.v043.i09. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/optimx/

PubMed Abstract| CrossRef Full Text

Newby, J. M., McKinnon, A., Kuyken, W., Gilbody, S., and Dalgleish, T. (2015). Systematic review and meta-analysis of transdiagnostic psychological treatments for anxiety and depressive disorders in adulthood. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 40, 91–110. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newby, J. M., Twomey, C., Yuan Li, S. S., and Andrews, G. (2016). Transdiagnostic computerised cognitive behavioural therapy for depression and anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 199, 30–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholas, J., Larsen, M. E., Proudfoot, J., and Christensen, H. (2015). Mobile apps for bipolar disorder: a systematic review of features and content quality. J. Med. Internet Res. 17, e198. doi: 10.2196/jmir.4581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olesen, J., Gustavsson, A., Svensson, M., Wittchen, H. U., and Jonsson, B. (2012). The economic cost of brain disorders in europe. Eur. J. Neurol. 19, 155–162. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2011.03590.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olff, M. (2015). Mobile mental health: a challenging research agenda. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 6:27882. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v6.27882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliver, M. I., Pearson, N., Coe, N., and Gunnell, D. (2005). Help-seeking behaviour in men and women with common mental health problems: cross-sectional study. Br. J. Psychiatry 186, 297–301. doi: 10.1192/bjp.186.4.297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olthuis, J. V., Watt, M. C., Bailey, K., Hayden, J. A., and Stewart, S. H. (2016). Therapist-supported internet cognitive behavioural therapy for anxiety disorders in adults. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 3:CD011565. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD011565.pub2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Orme-Johnson, D. W., and Barnes, V. A. (2014). Effects of the transcendental meditation technique on trait anxiety: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 20, 330–341. doi: 10.1089/acm.2013.0204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palmqvist, B., Carlbring, P., and Andersson, G. (2007). Internet-delivered treatments with or without therapist input: does the therapist factor have implications for efficacy and cost? Expert Rev. Pharmacoecon. Outcomes Res. 7, 291–297. doi: 10.1586/14737167.7.3.291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paredes, P., Gilad-Bachrach, R., Czerwinski, M., Roseway, A., Rowan, K., and Hernandez, J. (2014). “Poptherapy: coping with stress through pop-culture,” in PervasiveHealth 2014, (Gent: Institute for Computer Sciences, Social-Informatics and Telecommunications Engineering (ICST)), 109–117. Available online at: http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=2686909

Pollatos, O., Matthias, E., and Keller, J. (2015). When interoception helps to overcome negative feelings caused by social exclusion. Front. Psychol. 6:786. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2015). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna.

Rhodes, A. E., Goering, P. N., To, T., and Williams, J. I. (2002). Gender and outpatient mental health service use. Soc. Sci. Med. 54, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(01)00002-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, D., and Richardson, T. (2012). Computer-based psychological treatments for depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 32, 329–342. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richards, D., Richardson, T., Timulak, L., and McElvaney, J. (2015). The efficacy of internet-delivered treatment for generalized anxiety disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Internet Interv. 2, 272–282. doi: 10.1016/j.invent.2015.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riley, W. T., Glasgow, R. E., Etheredge, L., and Abernethy, A. P. (2013). Rapid, responsive, relevant (r3) research: a call for a rapid learning health research enterprise. Clin. Transl. Med. 2:10. doi: 10.1186/2001-1326-2-10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riper, H., Blankers, M., Hadiwijaya, H., Cunningham, J., Clarke, S., Wiers, R., et al. (2014). Effectiveness of guided and unguided low-intensity internet interventions for adult alcohol misuse: a meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 9:e99912. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099912

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaefer, A., Nils, F., Sanchez, X., and Philippot, P. (2010). Assessing the effectiveness of a large database of emotion-eliciting films: a new tool for emotion researchers. Cogn. Emotion 24, 1153–1172. doi: 10.1080/02699930903274322

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schueller, S. M. (2010). Preferences for positive psychology exercises. J. Posit. Psychol. 5, 192–203. doi: 10.1080/17439761003790948

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shallcross, A. J., Troy, A. S., Boland, M., and Mauss, I. B. (2010). Let it be: accepting negative emotional experiences predicts decreased negative affect and depressive symptoms. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 921–929. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, N., Levitan, M. J., Johnson, A., Bender, J. L., Hamilton-Page, M., Jadad, A. A., et al. (2015). Finding a depression app: a review and content analysis of the depression app marketplace. JMIR mHealth and uHealth 3, e16. doi: 10.2196/mhealth.3713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, J. D., and Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Spek, V., Cuijpers, P., Nyklícek, I., Riper, H., Keyzer, J., and Pop, V. (2007). Internet-based cognitive behaviour therapy for symptoms of depression and anxiety: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 37, 319–328. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706008944

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spijkerman, M., Pots, W., and Bohlmeijer, E. (2016). Effectiveness of online mindfulness-based interventions in improving mental health: a review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 45, 102–114. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steyer, R. (2014). Mdmq Questionnaire (English Version of Mdbf) [Online]. Jena: Friedrich-Schiller-Universität Jena, Institut für Psychologie, Lehrstuhl für Methodenlehre und Evaluationsforschung. Available online at: https://www.metheval.uni-jena.de/mdbf.php (Accessed April 4, 2016).

Steyer, R., Schwenkmezger, P., Notz, P., and Eid, M. (1997). Der Mehrdimensionale Befindlichkeitsfragebogen (mdbf): Handanweisung [The Multidimensional Mood State Questionnaire (mdmq): Manual]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

Tegethoff, M., Raul, J. S., Jamey, C., Khelil, M. B., Ludes, B., and Meinlschmidt, G. (2011). Dehydroepiandrosterone in nails of infants: a potential biomarker of intrauterine responses to maternal stress. Biol. Psychol. 87, 414–420. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thibault, R. T., Lifshitz, M., and Raz, A. (2016). The self-regulating brain and neurofeedback: experimental science and clinical promise. Cortex 74, 247–261. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.10.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson-Hollands, J., Sauer-Zavala, S., and Barlow, D. H. (2014). Cbt and the future of personalized treatment: a proposal. Depress. Anxiety 31, 909–911. doi: 10.1002/da.22301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torous, J., and Firth, J. (2016). The digital placebo effect: mobile mental health meets clinical psychiatry. Lancet Psychiatry 3, 100–102. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00565-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torous, J., and Powell, A. C. (2015). Current research and trends in the use of smartphone applications for mood disorders. Internet Interv. 2, 169–173. doi: 10.1016/j.invent.2015.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Velten, E. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states. Behav. Res. Ther. 6, 473–482. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(68)90028-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watts, S., Mackenzie, A., Thomas, C., Griskaitis, A., Mewton, L., Williams, A., et al. (2013). Cbt for depression: a pilot rct comparing mobile phone vs. Computer. BMC Psychiatry 13:49. doi: 10.1186/1471-244x-13-49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whittaker, R., McRobbie, H., Bullen, C., Rodgers, A., and Gu, Y. (2016). Mobile phone-based interventions for smoking cessation. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 4:CD006611. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD006611.pub4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickham, H. (2015). Tidyr: Easily Tidy Data with “Spread()” and “Gather()” Functions. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/tidyr/

Wickham, H., and Francois, R. (2015). Dplyr: A Grammar of Data Manipulation. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/dplyr/

Wickham, H., and Miller, E. (2015). Haven: Import Spss, Stata and Sas Files. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/haven/

Wilamowska, Z. A., Thompson-Hollands, J., Fairholme, C. P., Ellard, K. K., Farchione, T. J., and Barlow, D. H. (2010). Conceptual background, development, and preliminary data from the unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders. Depress. Anxiety 27, 882–890. doi: 10.1002/da.20735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittchen, H.-U., Jacobi, F., Rehm, J., Gustavsson, A., Svensson, M., Jönsson, B., et al. (2011). The size and burden of mental disorders and other disorders of the brain in europe 2010. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 21, 655–679. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yardley, L., Morrison, L., Bradbury, K., and Muller, I. (2015). The person-based approach to intervention development: application to digital health-related behavior change interventions. J. Med. Internet Res. 17, e30. doi: 10.2196/jmir.4055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoo, S. S., Lee, J. H., O'Leary, H., Panych, L. P., and Jolesz, F. A. (2008). Neurofeedback fmri-mediated learning and consolidation of regional brain activation during motor imagery. Int. J. Imaging Syst. Technol. 18, 69–78. doi: 10.1002/ima.20139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zachariae, R., Lyby, M. S., Ritterband, L. M., and O'Toole, M. S. (2016). Efficacy of internet-delivered cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia–a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Sleep Med. Rev. 30, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2015.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zeevi, D., Korem, T., Zmora, N., Israeli, D., Rothschild, D., Weinberger, A., et al. (2015). Personalized nutrition by prediction of glycemic responses. Cell 163, 1079–1094. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: behavioral intervention technology, ehealth, health information technology, information and communication technology, Internet- and mobile-based intervention, mental disorder, mhealth, wireless health

Citation: Meinlschmidt G, Lee J-H, Stalujanis E, Belardi A, Oh M, Jung EK, Kim H-C, Alfano J, Yoo S-S and Tegethoff M (2016) Smartphone-Based Psychotherapeutic Micro-Interventions to Improve Mood in a Real-World Setting. Front. Psychol. 7:1112. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01112

Received: 02 May 2016; Accepted: 11 July 2016;
Published: 28 July 2016.

Edited by:

Angelo Compare, University of Bergamo, Italy

Reviewed by:

Silvia Serino, Istituto Auxologico Italiano (IRCCS), Italy
Inês Mendes, University of Minho, Portugal

Copyright © 2016 Meinlschmidt, Lee, Stalujanis, Belardi, Oh, Jung, Kim, Alfano, Yoo and Tegethoff. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marion Tegethoff, marion.tegethoff@unibas.ch

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.