Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Psychol., 21 October 2016
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic The aging decision-maker: Advances in understanding the impact of cognitive change on decision-making View all 8 articles

Editorial: The Aging Decision-Maker: Advances in Understanding the Impact of Cognitive Change on Decision-Making

  • Department of Psychology, Middlesex University, London, UK

Decision-making is a dynamic skill rather than a static capacity (Dhami et al., 2011), and so may alter on a developmental scale. A reduction in cognitive functioning has been linked to suboptimal decisions in older adults (Finucane et al., 2000). For example, declines in memory (Ratcliff et al., 1992), perceptual processing (Ratcliff et al., 2006), and inhibition (Williams et al., 1999) have been related to impairment in decision-making competence. Despite this, older individuals are regularly required to make important life decisions, such as choosing a medical treatment or retirement plan.

This Research Topic brings together a selection of articles that advance our understanding of the effect of aging on decision-making. A common theme among the articles is risk, which characterizes the decisions faced by older individuals. Each article is accompanied by a Commentary providing constructive critique of the research and broadening the scope of the issues discussed.

The first article in this Research Topic is by Koscielniak et al., who found that, compared to younger adults (aged 18–23), older adults (aged 65–80) exhibited a lower propensity to take risks on the Balloon Analog Risk Task (BART; Lejuez et al., 2002). The effect was linked to a decline in deliberative processes, including reduced processing speed in older than younger adults. The effect was also associated with motivational factors, including a greater desire for predictability, order, and structure in older adults. This compliments prior research suggesting that older adults' allocation of limited cognitive resources may be linked to increased risk aversion (e.g., Bruine de Bruin et al., 2015). Moreover, as pointed out by Hess (2014), if risks are perceived as high, and resources (and confidence) are low, older adults may be less motivated to engage in the decision-making process. Importantly, however, Koscielniak et al. found that older adults were able to adapt to initial failures and successes in order to adjust their risk taking behavior over repeated trials. Further work is needed to investigate the factors that contribute to age-differences in risky decision-making, but we would like to echo the point made in the commentary to this article by Walasek, that caution should be applied when making inferences and predictions based on verbal theoretical frameworks to understand the cognitive underpinnings of such age differences.

The article by Seaman et al., examined the relationship between aging and risky decision-making in relation to residential choice. It was found that older adults living in a retirement community were more risk adverse than older adults living independently. The authors suggested that this may at least partly be due to age differences in the initial perception of risk, but the causal relationship remains unclear: Are risk adverse older adults more likely to choose the security of living in a retirement community, or does living in such a setting make older adults more risk adverse? Also, as pointed out in the commentary to this article by Petrova and Garcia-Retamero, it is often the case that relocating to a retirement community is the consequence of negative life events, such as poor health and/or the death of a spouse—it is not always a choice. Nevertheless, it is possible that negative life events surrounding the need to move to a retirement community, together with the challenge of adjusting to the new living arrangements, may make the individual risk adverse.

Taken together, the articles by Koscielniak et al. and Seaman et al. make a valuable contribution to understanding how age-related changes in cognitive and motivational factors may influence choices and circumstances. We reiterate Petrova and Garcia-Retamero's recommendation for conducting further studies to disentangle how the aforementioned potential mediators may be related to increased risk aversion and residential circumstances with age, and we agree with Walasek, that modeling techniques should also be employed in order to uncover the specific cognitive factors that differentially affect risk preferences in younger and older adults.

The final article in this Research Topic, by Bjalkebring et al., also addresses a specific real-world decision pertinent to older adults, namely charitable giving. The authors report evidence that older adults experience stronger feelings of sympathy and compassion than younger adults, and are more likely to report feeling positive emotions when making charitable donations. Moreover, older adults show greater motivation to make future donations, and experience more positive affect regarding previous donations. This is consistent with the positivity bias (the tendency to remember or focus more on pleasant than unpleasant information, e.g., Isaacowitz et al., 2006), and has important potential implications for wellbeing. “Doing good” is associated with “feeling good” (Dunn et al., 2008), and has even been linked to health benefits (Kim and Ferraro, 2014). It is noteworthy therefore, that older adults gain more from charitable giving than their younger counterparts, and it would be useful for future research to examine other forms of prosocial behavior, such as volunteering (e.g., Souza and Dhami, 2008). We emphasize the point made in the commentary by Hargis and Oppenheimer, that cognitive factors remain underexplored. For example, declines with age in inhibition and memory may affect decisions about charitable giving.

As the proportion of the global population over the age of 65 steadily rises, and policy makers implement interventions to ensure quality of life, it becomes increasingly important to systematically investigate how changes in cognition affect decision-making. At a broader level, cognitive functions that require elaborative processing are more susceptible to the deleterious effects of aging than those that require perceptual and familiarity-based processing (e.g., Ward et al., 2013a,b), but any resulting age differences in strategy use (e.g., intuitive versus rule-based), and the optimality of the decision outcome, remain underexplored. We hope that the collection of articles in this Research Topic will stimulate further theorizing, research and debate on the concept of the aging decision-maker.

Author Contributions

All authors listed, have made substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Bruine de Bruin, W., McNair, S. J., Taylor, A. L., Summers, B., and Strough, J. (2015). Thinking about numbers is not my idea of fun: need for cognition mediates age differences in numeracy performance. Med. Decis. Mak. 35, 22–26. doi: 10.1177/0272989X14542485

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhami, M. K., Schlottmann, A., and Waldmann, M. (eds.) (2011). Judgment and Decision Making As a Skill: Learning, Development, and Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Dunn, E. W., Aknin, L. B., and Norton, M. I. (2008). Spending money on others promotes happiness. Science 319, 1687–1688. doi: 10.1126/science.1150952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finucane, P. A., MacGregor, M. L., and Slovic, P. (2000). “The bearable lightness of aging: Judgment and decision processes in older adults,” in The Aging Mind: Opportunities in Cognitive Research eds P. C. Stern and L. L. Carstensen (Washington, DC: National Academies Press), 144–165.

Google Scholar

Hess, T. M. (2014). Selective engagement of cognitive resources: motivational influences on older adults' cognitive functioning. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 9, 388–407. doi: 10.1177/1745691614527465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isaacowitz, D. M., Wadlinger, H. A., Goren, D., and Wilson, H. R. (2006). Is there an age-related positivity effect in visual attention? A comparison of two methodologies. Emotion 6:511. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S., and Ferraro, K. F. (2014). Do productive activities reduce inflammation in later life? Multiple roles, frequency of activities, and C-reactive protein. Gerontologist 54, 830–839. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnt090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., et al. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 8, 75–84. doi: 10.1037/1076-898x.8.2.75

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratcliff, R., Thapar, A., and McKoon, G. (1992). Effects of aging and IQ on item and associative memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 140, 464–487. doi: 10.1037/a0023810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratcliff, R., Thapar, A., and McKoon, G. (2006). Aging, practice, and perceptual tasks: a diffusion model analysis. Psychol. Aging 21, 353–371. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Souza, K. A., and Dhami, M. K. (2008). A study of volunteers in community-based restorative justice programs. Can. J. Criminol. Crim. Justice 50, 31–57. doi: 10.3138/cjccj.50.1.31

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, E. V., Berry, C. J., and Shanks, D. R. (2013a). Age effects on explicit and implicit memory. Front. Cogn. 4:639. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, E. V., Berry, C. J., and Shanks, D. R. (2013b). An effect of age on implicit memory that is not due to explicit contamination: implications for single and multiple-systems theories. Psychol. Aging 28, 429–442. doi: 10.1037/a0031888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, B. R., Ponesse, J. S., Schachar, R. J., Logan, G. D., and Tannock, R. (1999). Development of inhibitory control across the life span. Dev. Psychol. 35, 205–213. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aging, decision making, cognitive decline, risky decision making, cognitive aging

Citation: Ward EV and Dhami MK (2016) Editorial: The Aging Decision-Maker: Advances in Understanding the Impact of Cognitive Change on Decision-Making. Front. Psychol. 7:1622. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01622

Received: 19 September 2016; Accepted: 04 October 2016;
Published: 21 October 2016.

Edited and reviewed by: Bernhard Hommel, Leiden University, Netherlands

Copyright © 2016 Ward and Dhami. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Emma V. Ward, e.ward@mdx.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.