Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 04 January 2017
Sec. Cognition

Commentary: From ‘sense of number’ to ‘sense of magnitude’ – The role of continuous magnitudes in numerical cognition

  • Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Padova, Italy

A commentary on
From ‘sense of number’ to ‘sense of magnitude’ – The role of continuous magnitudes in numerical cognition

by Leibovich, T., Katzin, N., Harel, M., and Henik, A. (2016). Behav. Brain. Sci. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X16000960

Unlike abstract ones in mathematics, concrete sets of elements in the real world have continuous physical properties, such as overall area and density. The dominant view has it that humans can estimate the discrete numerosities of such sets independently of the co-varying continuous magnitudes; i.e., that humans have a “sense of number.” It has indeed been claimed that various animals, ranging from monkeys to tiny fish, have this sense too. A recent paper by Leibovich et al. (2016) questions all of this (see also Morgan et al., 2014; Gebuis et al., 2016) and argues convincingly that numerosity estimation is not independent from continuous magnitudes but relies on them; that we have not a “sense of number” but a “sense of magnitude.”

Yet the authors fail to cite a classic article that made the very same argument 25 years ago, and—unlike Leibovich et al.—supported it with a quantitative model (Allik and Tuulmets, 1991). Although neither density, nor overall area, nor any other single continuous magnitude can provide reliable information about numerosity, Leibovich et al. imply that all of them together can; they suggest that “statistical learning” will take care of extracting this information and turn numerosity estimates out of it. How statistical learning achieves this feat and whether the resulting numerosity estimates will fit observed ones remains, unfortunately, unclear. Allik and Tuulmets's alternative “occupancy” model has its limits (e.g., Kramer et al., 2011; Bertamini et al., 2016) but it is specific, it is quantitative, and it predicts observed numerosity estimation surprisingly well with just a single free parameter.

To understand the occupancy model, consider a set of identical dots and imagine that each of them is covered with a larger disk, as in Figure 1. The model posits that the total area occupied by the disks (occupancy) will be linearly related to the estimated numerosity of the dots. As long as dot densities remain relatively low (Durgin, 1995), the model accounts for nearly 90% of the variance in human data (Allik and Tuulmets, 1991). The closer together the dots are and thus the more the disks overlap, the smaller occupancy is and thus the smaller the estimated numerosity of the dots is predicted to be—as indeed observed (e.g., DeWind et al., 2015). Notably, because large numerosities tend to be dense, the occupancy model predicts they will tend to be underestimated. This prediction has been corroborated repeatedly too (e.g., Izard and Dehaene, 2008). How Leibovich et al.'s statistical learning could make the same prediction is hard to tell.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Effect of item spacing on occupancy. The “occupancy” of a dot may be represented as a larger, concentric virtual disk that covers it, shown here in gray. (The radius of the disk is a free parameter that, during model fitting, Allik and Tuulmets estimated to be 0.33° of visual angle.) The three black dots in the bottom row are closer together than those in the top row. As a result, the virtual disks in the bottom row overlap, making for a smaller occupancy (total gray area) and therefore a smaller predicted numerosity estimate. For illustration purposes only a few dots are shown; note, however, that numerosity estimation concerns numerosities outside the “subitizing” range of one to four items.

The occupancy model—even in Durgin's (1995) variant—is too simple to work under all conditions (Kramer et al., 2011). Still, if the degree of overlap of the virtual disks covering the dots is interpreted as an inverse measure of the mutual discriminability of the dots, the model makes an intuitive point. The point is that the less discriminable from one another items are, the lower their estimated numerosity ought to be. If so, the occupancy model could be expanded and improved (Kramer et al., 2011) by taking into account anything that affects the items' (1) discriminability (their distance from one another and from fixation, for example), (2) retention across saccades (if presentation times allow eye movements), and (3) retention in memory (if the stimulus is presented or inspected sequentially or compared to another one).

Although people are under the impression they are blessed with a high-resolution image of the world around them, careful experimentation has revealed that this is largely an illusion (e.g., Durgin, 1995). In numerosity-estimation experiments the stimuli tend to be randomly placed items, usually dots, and some of them end up clumped together. These items may be impossible to discriminate from one another, even when they can all be perceptually detected. This crowding effect (Whitney and Levi, 2011) is a serious concern, because the opportunities for crowding necessarily grow with numerosity and have been shown to affect its estimation (Valsecchi et al., 2013; Anobile et al., 2015).

So far, studies that claim that numerosity estimation does not fully rely on continuous magnitudes, or that it is more cognitive than perceptual in nature, have not been very specific about how the perceptual system extracts numerosity from the stimulus (see also Morgan et al., 2014). These studies have considered a wide range of perceptual factors such as density, overall area, and item size. None of these factors, however, has ever been claimed to be crucial in numerosity estimation. The one factor that has for as long as 25 years (i.e., occupancy) is instead consistently ignored, including now by Leibovich et al. (2016). Izard and Dehaene (2008), for example, do cite Allik and Tuulmets (1991) and control for various factors including “occupied area,” but they use the term to mean overall area (which nobody considers critical to numerosity estimation) and not occupancy (which Allik and Tuulmets themselves consider critical). Izard and Dehaene dismiss occupancy as “a complex combination” of two parameters (density and occupied area). Occupancy does not strike us as complex, but even if it were, the fact that it reflects well-established perceptual discriminability limitations that have been demonstrated to affect numerosity estimation—rather than hypothetical cognitive ones that continue to be debated—should give the occupancy model an edge over any “sense of number” model. In defense of their own “sense of magnitude” model, which Leibovich et al. fail to cite too, Morgan et al. (2014) make a similar point.

The occupancy model deserves at the very least discussion—if not revival and development. It has the potential to challenge not only “sense of number” models but also Leibovich et al.'s statistical-learning alternative to them. Unlike the latter, the occupancy model is a “sense of magnitude” model that does not require learning, is deterministic, quantitative, perception- rather than cognition-based, and not inconsistent with the idea that numerosity estimation might be innate and so simple that even fish could do it.

Author Contributions

PK wrote the first draft of the manuscript; both authors discussed it, critically revised it, and agreed on the final version.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Allik, J., and Tuulmets, T. (1991). Occupancy model of perceived numerosity. Percept. Psychophys. 49, 303–314. doi: 10.3758/BF03205986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anobile, G., Turi, M., Cicchini, G. M., and Burr, D. C. (2015). Mechanisms for perception of numerosity or texture-density are governed by crowding-like effects. J. Vis. 15, 1–12. doi: 10.1167/15.5.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertamini, M., Zito, M., Scott-Samuel, N. E., and Hulleman, J. (2016). Spatial clustering and its effect on perceived clustering, numerosity, and dispersion. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 78, 1460–1471. doi: 10.3758/s13414-016-1100-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeWind, N. K., Adams, G. K., Platt, M. L., and Brannon, E. M. (2015). Modeling the approximate number system to quantify the contribution of visual stimulus features. Cognition 142, 247–265. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durgin, F. H. (1995). Texture density adaptation and the perceived numerosity and distribution of texture. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 21, 149–169.

Google Scholar

Gebuis, T., Cohen Kadosh, R., and Gevers, W. (2016). Sensory-integration system rather than approximate number system underlies numerosity processing: a critical review. Acta Psychol. 171, 17–35. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izard, V., and Dehaene, S. (2008). Calibrating the mental number line. Cognition 106, 1221–1247. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, P., Di Bono, M. G., and Zorzi, M. (2011). Numerosity estimation in visual stimuli in the absence of luminance-based cues. PLoS ONE 6:e17378. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leibovich, T., Katzin, N., Harel, M., and Henik, A. (2016). From ‘sense of number’ to ‘sense of magnitude’ - the role of continuous magnitudes in numerical cognition. Behav. Brain. Sci. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X16000960. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, M. J., Raphael, S., Tibber, M. S., and Dakin, S. C. (2014). A texture-processing model of the ‘visual sense of number’. Proc. Biol. Sci. 281:20141137. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valsecchi, M., Toscani, M., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2013). Perceived numerosity is reduced in peripheral vision. J. Vis. 13, 1–16. doi: 10.1167/13.13.7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitney, D., and Levi, D. M. (2011). Visual crowding: a fundamental limit on conscious perception and object recognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 160–168. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sense of number, sense of magnitude, numerosity estimation, occupancy, statistical learning

Citation: Kramer P and Bressan P (2017) Commentary: From ‘sense of number’ to ‘sense of magnitude’ – The role of continuous magnitudes in numerical cognition. Front. Psychol. 7:2032. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02032

Received: 31 October 2016; Accepted: 14 December 2016;
Published: 04 January 2017.

Edited by:

Bert Reynvoet, KU Leuven, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Marcus Lindskog, Uppsala University, Sweden

Copyright © 2017 Kramer and Bressan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Peter Kramer, peter.kramer@unipd.i

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.