Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 03 March 2017
Sec. Organizational Psychology

Reciprocal Relations between Work-Related Authenticity and Intrinsic Motivation, Work Ability and Depressivity: A Two-Wave Study

  • 1Work and Organizational Psychology, Leipzig University, Leipzig, Germany
  • 2Work, Organizational and Business Psychology, Johannes Gutenberg University, Mainz, Germany

This study investigates the role of context-specific authenticity at work for work-related outcomes (intrinsic motivation, work ability) and depressivity. Furthermore reciprocal relations between work-related authenticity and healthy psychological functioning are investigated. Longitudinal data from 1,243 employees from 63 subsidiaries of a non-profit organization in the social sector were analyzed using multilevel structural equation modeling. Work-related authenticity at T1 predicted work ability and depressivity, but not intrinsic motivation at T2, about 6 months later. Work-related authenticity at T2 was predicted by intrinsic motivation and depressivity, but not by work ability at T1. We conclude that work-related authenticity and healthy psychological functioning are positively reinforcing each other. Thus, enabling employees to be authentic supposedly increases their well-being and is a pivotal opportunity for organizations to foster health and performance-related indicators like work ability and prevent negative health indicators like depressivity. At the same time, authenticity of employees can be fostered through workplace health promotion.

Introduction

The European Working Conditions Survey reports that 25% of EU employees have to hide their feelings at work either all or most of the time, while 20% reported that their work sometimes or always involves tasks that conflict with their personal values (Eurofound, 2010). Even though being your “true self” has been considered a major life goal ever since ancient times (Taylor, 2003), it is not self-evident that employees can be authentic in their daily working lives (Hewlin, 2003) and experience fulfillment and self-actualisation by living out their potential (Deci and Ryan, 1985, 2000). The need to “act professional” in work settings can contradict being “real” as employees are often expected to follow role expectations and demands from supervisors, clients and colleagues that are not necessarily consistent with their feelings, values or ideas.

Authenticity has been discovered to be negatively related to anxiety (Sheldon et al., 1997; Ryan et al., 2005; Wood et al., 2008), stress (Sheldon et al., 1997; Ryan et al., 2005; Kernis and Goldman, 2006; Wood et al., 2008) and depression (Sheldon et al., 1997; Ryan et al., 2005), as well as positively linked to life satisfaction (Kernis and Goldman, 2006; Wood et al., 2008; Di Fabio and Kenny, 2016), well-being (Ryan et al., 2005; Kernis and Goldman, 2006; Robinson et al., 2013) and positive affect (Kernis and Goldman, 2006; Wood et al., 2008; Di Fabio and Kenny, 2016). This health-promoting role seems also hold true for authenticity at work. So far, studies in the work context were able to provide empirical evidence for a positive link between authenticity and work engagement (Cable et al., 2013; van den Bosch and Taris, 2013, 2014), subjective well-being (Ménard and Brunet, 2011) and positive affect (Kifer et al., 2013) as well as negative relations to burnout, stress (van den Bosch and Taris, 2014), negative affect (Kifer et al., 2013), irritation and symptoms of physical illness (Knoll et al., 2015).

In the following, we draw upon Self-Determination Theory (SDT; Deci and Ryan, 1985, 2000) to develop hypotheses about the relationship between authenticity, intrinsic motivation, work ability and depressivity at work. We suggest that authenticity at work increases intrinsic motivation as well as work ability, but also impacts well-being. We extend prior interpretations of these relationships by addressing their potential order of causality.

While there is compelling evidence about the relation between authenticity and well-being and healthy psychological functioning, to our knowledge only two studies have moved beyond a cross-sectional design (Boyraz et al., 2014; Knoll et al., 2015). For this reason, we conducted a two-wave study in order to examine relations across time—linking authenticity at work to well-being and health, but also to check for potential reciprocal effects. We will propose that not only does authenticity influence healthy psychological functioning, but that there are also reciprocal processes taking place.

In the next section, a short introduction to the concept of authenticity will be given. Then, we will explicate the proposed relationships between work-related authenticity and depressivity, intrinsic motivation and work ability based on SDT (Deci and Ryan, 1985, 2000), followed by explications for the proposed reciprocal effects.

The Concept of Work-Related Authenticity

Authenticity can be described as “acting in accord with the true self, expressing oneself in ways that are consistent with inner thoughts and feelings” (Harter, 2005, p. 382). Drawing from historical philosophical perspectives as well as past and contemporary psychological perspectives, Kernis and Goldman (2006) developed a multicomponent conceptualisation of authenticity. This includes self-awareness which means knowing one self, e.g., one's own motives, preferences, feelings and being motivated to extend that knowledge constantly. Unbiased processing as the second component includes the relative absence of distortions in processing self-relevant information and objectivity, e.g., to positive and negative self-aspects. The third component refers to behavior that is in accordance with oneself, including one's values, preferences and needs. Relational orientation describes the relational component of authenticity and includes openness and honesty in close relationships as well as the willingness to show others one's true self. Authenticity has both trait and state components (Lenton et al., 2013; Robinson et al., 2013; Smallenbroek et al., 2016). In this study, we will investigate context-specific authenticity at work which refers to the extent that one is in tune with one's true self at work (van den Bosch and Taris, 2013). Kernis and Goldman (2006) promoted a second order factor model (based on item parcels). Hence, authenticity is considered as consisting of four interrelated but distinct components with relations that are explained by a higher order authenticity factor. Later studies mostly used authenticity as a composite measure (e.g., Ménard and Brunet, 2011; Davis and Hicks, 2013; Wickham, 2013). As we could not find solid empirical evidence on differential validity for the sub-dimensions, and we also do not see theoretical reasons for differential effects, we will formulate our hypotheses by referring to authenticity as a global construct.

Authenticity and Intrinsic Motivation

Deci and Ryan (2000) propose that “intrinsically motivated behaviors are those that are freely engaged out of interest without the necessity of separable consequences” (p. 233). Self-Determination-Theory (SDT) suggests that increased intrinsic motivation, as well as social development and well-being, result from self-determined behavior. As authentic behavior has its source in the true self of an individual, authentic behavior is self-determined by nature and supposedly leads to improved intrinsic motivation, social development and well-being.

SDT explains the effect of self-determined behavior through the satisfaction of basic psychological needs: the need for autonomy, relatedness and competence (Deci and Ryan, 2000). When employees act self-determined and authentic, they seek activities that reflect their true self which ultimately fulfills their basic psychological needs and increases intrinsic motivation. The striving for, and satisfaction of, these needs results in intrinsic motivation and well-being. Van den Broeck et al. (2010) and Richer et al. (2002) were able to show that the satisfaction of these proposed basic psychological needs was related to intrinsic and autonomous motivation. In conclusion, we propose that work-related authenticity fosters intrinsic motivation:

Hypothesis 1a: There is a positive lagged effect of work-related authenticity on intrinsic motivation.

Authenticity and Work Ability

Next, we propose that authenticity is not only a factor that influences the motivation to work but also the actual ability to cope with work demands and the ability to perform. The concept of work ability is grounded on the question “How good are workers at present and in the near future and how able are they to do their job with respect to work demands, health, and mental resources”? (Ilmarinen et al., 1997, p. 49). Hasselhorn and Freude (2007) describe work ability as the capability of an individual to perform well in their job at a particular time.

According to the conceptualisation of Kernis and Goldman (2006), authentic behavior is characterized by increased self-awareness and unbiased processing. Thus, authentic employees possess a more detailed, complex and accurate knowledge about their abilities and inner states regarding their work and performance. We propose that this enables them to manage themselves better regarding work demands, e.g., knowing which would be the best way for them to perform in their occupation. Furthermore, being more aware of inner states supposedly enables employees to deal with their inner states, e.g., knowing how to deal with challenging and stressful situations. In addition, unbiased processing enables employees to analyse mistakes and shortcomings in a less distorted way, which helps them to improve their abilities and performance based on that.

We further propose that when individuals act authentically at work, their work goals are in congruence with their personal goals. Sheldon and Elliot (1999) state that individuals put more effort in such self-concordant goals, which should further foster employees' work ability. Moreover, when employees are authentic in the workplace, they are supposedly spending less psychological resources on self-control or fake behaviors, e.g., through surface acting, following display rules (Grandey, 2000) or employee silence (Knoll and van Dick, 2013) which in turn leads to an increased work ability. In addition, we suggest that authentic employees benefit from increased social resources (Hobfoll, 1989) through authentic relationships at work, e.g., by the opportunity to share “actual” emotions, thoughts, perceived stress and problems (Grandey et al., 2012).

Hypothesis 1b: There is a positive lagged effect of work-related authenticity on work ability.

Authenticity and Depressivity

Another purpose we aim to investigate is whether work-related authenticity has an impact on depressivity. In order to investigate this relationship, we use non-clinical depression (Mohr and Müller, 2012) as an indicator. Non-clinical depression reflects impaired psychological well-being resulting from daily and continuous stressors (Mohr, 1986, 1991) and is based on the “cognitive triad” (Beck, 1967), a negative view of oneself, one's environment and one's future. It does not indicate a severe mental illness and is thus more useful for research in the work setting.

As previously mentioned, there are several studies that provide evidence for a relationship between general authenticity and well-being, e.g., positive mental health (Robinson et al., 2013), happiness (Wood et al., 2008), self-esteem (Kernis and Goldman, 2006; Davis et al., 2015) and decreased stress, anxiety and physical symptoms (Sheldon et al., 1997; Ryan et al., 2005). Moreover, Sheldon et al. (1997) found a negative relationship between authenticity across different social roles and depression. Theran (2011) found authenticity in relationships to be negatively related to depressivity. Based on the assumptions of SDT, we suggest that authentic self-determined behavior not only fosters positive functioning at work like work ability, but that it also has the potential to decrease depressivity. In conclusion, we hypothesize:

Hypothesis 1c: There is a negative lagged effect of work-related authenticity on depressivity.

Reciprocal Processes: The Impact of Intrinsic Motivation, Work Ability, and Depressivity on Work-Related Authenticity

Though most of the studies on authenticity assume it to be a precursor for healthy functioning of individuals rather than an outcome, Wood et al. (2008) state that both causal directions between authenticity and well-being are possible. So far, only Knoll et al. (2015) and Boyraz et al. (2014) moved beyond a cross-sectional design and examined longitudinal relations. Knoll et al. (2015) found authenticity to be predictive of strain after a 6-week-interval as well as a purpose in life as an indicator for well-being to be predictive for authenticity 6 weeks later. Boyraz et al. (2014) found initial authenticity to be predictive for increased life satisfaction and decreased distress later on, yet no significant reciprocal effects were found. This indicates a need to further investigate possible reciprocal relationships. So far, we proposed a unidirectional impact of authenticity on the examined outcomes, but it is also possible that authenticity and work-related outcomes are positively reinforcing each other, resulting in a positive reciprocal circle.

Generally, the three investigated outcomes can be considered as indicators of healthy psychological functioning. Accordingly, a reciprocal effect would mean that healthy psychological functioning predicts increased authenticity. Furthermore, authenticity is one facet of eudaimonic well-being (Di Fabio and Palazzeschi, 2015; Di Fabio and Kenny, 2016). Following this idea, we would like to suggest that healthy psychological functioning can be a facilitator for being authentic.

Waterman (1993) describes eudaimonia as the result of behavior that is in accord with and toward the direction of one's true potential. In the work context, this would mean that eudaimonic well-being increases by achieving personal goals, which are in tune with one's true nature as well as from engaging in achieving goals that were difficult to accomplish and thus challenges employees to realize their full potential. The link between personal goals and eudaimonic well-being (Kiaei and Reio, 2014) as well as the link between striving for the optimal outcome and eudaimonic well-being found empirical support (Kokkoris, 2016).

We suggest the idea that intrinsic motivation, work ability and low levels of depressivity as healthy psychological functioning provide the necessary resources for engaging in personal and challenging goals. In particular, the three indicators are important because they reflect if individuals are striving for personal and challenging goals (intrinsic motivation) and if individuals have the resources to do so (work ability, low depressivity).

Thus, we hypothesize that not only does authenticity influence intrinsic motivation, work ability and depressivity, but additionally all three indicators are supposedly predictive of authenticity as a facet of eudaimonic well-being.

Intrinsic Motivation

When people act out of intrinsic motivation, they find fulfillment in those activities and enjoy doing them (Ryan and Deci, 2000). We suggest that employees who act intrinsically motivated, feel more closely connected to their work, e.g., in regard to their motives, abilities, feelings or values. Intrinsically motivated individuals were found to be more involved (Lee et al., 2011) and cognitively engaged (Walker et al., 2006) in their work. We propose that this accompanies an increased “sense of self” (Goldman, 2004) and employees are not only more aware of their inner states but also act more according to their authentic selves, increasing authenticity as a facet of eudaimonic well-being.

Hypothesis 2a: There is a positive lagged effect of intrinsic motivation on work-related authenticity.

Work Ability and Depressivity

We further propose that high work ability and low depressivity positively affect eudaimonic well-being and authenticity as they reflect healthier psychological functioning to achieve personal and challenging goals.

We propose that individuals who report better health are supposedly more likely to approach challenging goals, which ultimately helps them to achieve those goals, resulting in eudaimonic well-being. Kifer et al. (2013) found a relationship between perceived power and authenticity and Satici et al. (2013) found a relationship between self-efficacy and authenticity. This supports the idea that individuals who perceive themselves as powerful are experiencing higher self-realization and eudaimonic well-being, because they are more likely to engage in challenging activities that require their maximal potential.

Thus, though not every individual that is characterized by healthy psychological functioning is automatically pursuing personal and challenging goals, it is a prerequisite to do so. Accordingly, we would like to suggest a link from high work ability and low depressivity to work-related authenticity.

Hypothesis 2b: There is a positive lagged effect of work ability on work-related authenticity.

Hypothesis 2c: There is a negative lagged effect of depressivity on work-related authenticity.

Methods

Respondents and Procedure

The study was conducted in a German non-profit organization for education, youth and social work. The sample covered a variety of occupations, mainly jobs related to education, training and psychosocial care as well as employees in administration. The employees of the organization received an email with a link to the online survey (Unipark) and were able to answer it during their working time within 2 weeks. Employees received information about the survey. They were assured of anonymity, that participation was voluntary and that they would get a report for their location if a minimum of eight employees from an organizational unit took part.

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Federation of the German Psychologists Association's Code of Ethics. The survey was conducted with the agreement of the workers' council of the participating organization. All subjects participated voluntarily in the survey in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical review and approval was not required for this study as per the institutional and national requirements.

As we were interested in mid-term effects that would indicate a sustaining effect above the short term 6-weeks effects that were found by Boyraz et al. (2014) and Knoll et al. (2015) of authenticity on well-being and vice versa, we chose an interval of 6 months between the two waves. The first measurement took place between July and September 2013, dependent on organizational considerations in the subsidiaries. The second survey (T2) was conducted between January and March 2014, approximately 6 months after T1 in each subsidiary.

The invitation to participate at the first measurement time (T1) was sent out to 2,731 employees across 64 subsidiaries, and 1,110 employees responded to the questionnaire (response rate = 40.6%). At T2, due to organizational issues, only 37 subsidiaries could be connected, comprising a total of 2.666 employees, of which 673 followed the link to the online questionnaire (response rate = 25.2%).

As we used MPlus for our analyses, we did not perform a listwise deletion, but instead made use of all information available. The analyses are based on information from N = 1,243 employees coming from 63 different subsidiaries. This sample for analyses consisted of more women (61.5%) than men (36.5%; 2% missing) with an average age of 44.6 years (SD = 11.1) at T1. On average, employees had been working in the organization at T1 for 11.4 years (SD = 9.0) and in their current position for 7.8 years (SD = 7.5). They were contracted to work on average for 32.1 h a week (SD = 10.1); their actual working time was 33.5 h on average a week (SD = 12.4). To check if there was a systematic drop out of respondents between T1 and T2, we conducted t-tests and Chi-Square-tests for the sociodemographic and study variables. Employees that participated only at T1 did differ from employees that took part at both measurement times in regard to working time. Participants who took part at both measurement times reported more contracted (T1 only: M = 31.81; T1 & T2: M = 33.09, p = 0.05) and actual (T1 only: M = 33.04; T1 & T2: M = 34.75, p = 0.04) weekly working hours. No systematic dropout effects were found for any of the study variables.

Measures

Work-Related Authenticity

To measure work-related authenticity, items were adapted from the Authenticity Inventory (AI; Kernis and Goldman, 2006). The initial version consists of 45 items. The authors theoretically distinguished between four dimensions, which they labeled as awareness, unbiased processing, authentic behavior, and relational orientation. Kernis and Goldman (2006) used item-parceling to test the factor structure, and found the best fit for a second order-factor model. Subsequent research employing the AI is inconclusive concerning its factor structure: Lakey et al. (2008) as well as Gillath et al. (2010) used the four sub-dimensions, as well as a composite score without providing information on results of factor analyses within their samples. Ménard and Brunet (2011) contextualized 25 items to the working context, and constructed two dimensions labeled as Unbiased Awareness, and Authentic Behaviors. Hence, a closer look at the dimensionality of the measure seemed warranted. For our study, to create a shorter, more economic measure, five scholars from the field of work and organizational psychology were asked to rate items concerning their content validity for the four sub-dimensions, and their applicability to the working context. This resulted in 25 items (6–7 items per theoretical dimension). All of these items were translated by two people and translated back by one person (Brislin, 1986), and adapted to the working context. Due to the skewed distribution of items, we performed factor analyses on a matrix of polychoric correlations by specifying all variables as categorical and using the robust weighted least square estimator (WLSMV; Flora and Curran, 2004) in Mplus version 7.11 (Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2015). We started with an exploratory two-level factor analysis using the sample at T1 (N = 929 nested in k = 61 organizations). A parallel analysis (Horn, 1965; Liu and Rijmen, 2008; for applications with ordered categorical data see Cho et al., 2009) showed that the first four empirical eigenvalues were larger than eigenvalues derived from a random data-set. Hence, we retained four factors. The factor loading matrix revealed substantial cross-loadings of many items. In order to retain the theoretical structure of the AI, we finally chose three items with distinct factor-loadings on the four factors. For a further test of the psychometric properties we ran a set of (multilevel) confirmatory factor analyses. A one factor model [χ(195)2 = 500.99, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.04, CFI = 0.80, TLI = 0.79] showed obviously a worse fit when compared to a second-order factor model [χ(116)2 = 138.12, p = 0.08, RMSEA = 0.01, CFI = 0.98, TLI = 0.98]. Although a model with a correlated four factors showed even a slightly better fit [χ(114)2 = 128.13, p = 0.17, RMSEA = 0.01, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.99], we used the second-order factor model in subsequent cross-lagged panel analyses to test our hypotheses, as there is no clear evidence on a differential validity of sub-dimensions. However, in addition, we will report results from exploratory analyses based on the four sub-dimensions. Sample items of the retained items are: “I am often confused about my feelings at work [recoded].” (awareness), “I often find that I am overly critical about my performance at work [recoded].” (unbiased processing), “I've often used my silence or head-nodding in a team meeting to convey agreement with someone else's statement even though I really disagree[recoded].” (authentic behavior), and “I want my colleagues to understand my weaknesses.” (relational orientation).

Intrinsic Motivation

For assessing intrinsic motivation, the 3-item intrinsic motivation subscale from the Work Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation Scale (Tremblay et al., 2009) in a German version (Nestler, 2011) was used. The items ask for reasons for employees to be involved in their work, e.g., “For the satisfaction I experience when I am successful at doing difficult tasks.” Answers are given on a 7-point frequency rating scale ranging from “1- never” to “7– always/every day.” Studies confirmed the internal and construct validity of the scale. Güntert (2015), for instance, reported high positive correlations to job satisfaction [r(201) = 0.76, p < 0.01], and civic virtue [r(201) = 0.38, p < 0.01].

Work Ability

Work ability was assessed with one item from the Work Ability Index (Hasselhorn and Freude, 2007). Participants were asked to imagine the highest work ability that they have ever achieved as “10” and then rate their current work ability on a scale between “1- completely unable to work” to “10– highest work ability.” The single item, when compared to the seven items instrument of work ability has shown very similar correlations to, e.g., sick leave or reports of symptoms (El Fassi et al., 2013). Ahlstrom et al. (2010) concluded: “In our study, both the WAI and the single item question strongly predicted the future degree of sick leave […]” (p. 410).

Depressivity

Depression in a nonclinical context was assessed with an 8-item scale by Mohr and Müller (2012). Items were answered on a 7-point frequency rating scale ranging from “1- never” to “7– almost always,” e.g., “I am in a sad mood.” A two-level confirmatory factor analyses using the eight items as indicators for a single factor showed a good fit to the data at T1 [χ(48)2 = 107.73 p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.04, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.99]. As item distributions were skewed, we treated the items to be ordinal scaled, and used the WLSMV estimation method. Mohr and Müller (2012) reported solid evidence on the validity of this measure.

Results

Correlations of study variables at T1 and T2 are presented in Table 1. These correlations represent standardized estimates of covariation between the (latent) factors of a multilevel structural equation model. Correlations with authenticity were moderate for intrinsic motivation and work ability while being strong for depressivity. Intrinsic motivation and work ability were moderately positively related. Depressivity was moderately negatively related to intrinsic motivation, whereas it was strongly negatively related to work ability. It should also be pointed out that the factor relational orientation is mostly uncorrelated to the other dimensions, and thus seems to represent a distinct concept.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Correlations between the (latent) study variables.

In a first step we examined whether measurement invariance across T1 and T2 existed for the latent variables as a precondition for testing cross-lagged effects (Vandenberg and Lance, 2000). Several types of measurement invariance can be distinguished, but only configural (same factor pattern across time points) and metric invariance (identical loadings) are considered important for cross-lagged panel analyses (cf. Hu and Cheung, 2008). In all models we allowed measurement errors for same items to correlate between T1 and T2 (Little et al., 2007). As a simple χ2 difference test cannot be applied to categorical data, and the Satorra-Bentler correction (DIFFTEST-option in Mplus) is not available for multilevel SEM, we decided to perform the measurement invariance tests on the individual level, not considering the nested structure of the data. Results are presented in Table 2. For the measurement model of authenticity, we compared the unrestricted model to a model with invariant factor loadings for the second order factor, and also to a model with invariant factor loadings for the first and second factor. Chen (2007) recommended cut-off values of CFI ≤0.010 and RMSEA ≤0.015 (for N > 300) for model comparisons. Overall, the results support metric invariance of latent variables across measurement points.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Measurement invariance analyses.

We further analyzed data with multilevel structural equation modeling using Mplus Version 7.11 (Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2015). As we sampled employees from different locations, we had a nested data structure. To account for the dependency of answers from employees working at the same site, we decided to run the analyses in multilevel-models. We treated all variables to be ordered categorical and used the robust weighted least square estimation method. To evaluate the models in terms of fit to the data, we report the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), and the root-mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) following common research practice (cf. Coovert and Craiger, 2000).

We started by evaluating the overall measurement model. The model that allowed all factors/constructs to covary showed a good fit to the data [χ(2171)2 = 2362.50, p = 0.002, RMSEA = 0.01, CFI = 0.97, TLI = 0.97]. We tested the hypothesis by specifying a cross-lagged structural model that included authenticity as well as intrinsic motivation, work ability and depressivity at both measurement times (see Figure 1; Model 1, Table 3). For all models jointly including intrinsic motivation, work ability and depressivity, we allowed depressivity and work ability to covary across time points. As intrinsic motivation, work ability, and depressivity are intercorrelated, we also ran a set of models by including only one of these variables at a time (Models 2–4, Table 3). We used items as indicators for latent constructs as described in the methods section. Work ability was included as a manifest variable as it only consisted of one item. In addition, we ran exploratory cross-lagged panel models looking at the subdimensions of authenticity in isolation (Models 5–8) in order to gain information about potential differential validity of these facets.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Standardized parameter estimates of the reciprocal model with reciprocal time-lagged effects. Dashed paths represent regular causation paths, dotted paths represent reverse causation paths; bold coefficients are significant on p < 0.05-level.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Standardized parameter estimates of multilevel cross-lagged-panel models.

Results of the cross-lagged-panel analysis are presented in Table 3. The table reports correlations between latent variables at T1, stability of each construct across T1 and T2, cross-lagged effects as well as the correlations between the error terms of latent variables at T2 (disturbance correlations). Figure 1 presents standardized coefficients for the reciprocal model, based on a second order factor of authenticity. All constructs showed significant stabilities with medium (work ability) to large effect sizes. Predictor correlations at T1 were all significant on the p < 0.001 level and in the expected direction.

There was no significant lagged effect from authenticity to intrinsic motivation (Model 1: Υ = 0.08; p = 0.249; Model 2: Υ = −0.02; p = 0.819), which leads to a rejection of H1a. Our second hypothesis (H1b) was supported by significant cross-paths from authenticity to work ability (Model 1: Υ = 0.35; p < 0.001; Model 3: Υ = 0.34; p < 0.001). Authenticity showed a significant lagged effect to depressivity in the overall model (Model 1: Υ = −0.20, p = 0.038), and a marginal significant effect in the isolated model (Model 4: Υ = −0.18; p = 0.069). Taking the additional exploratory analyses with the single facets of authenticity into account (Model 6: Unbiased Processing, and Model 7: Authentic Behavior), this overall provides support for H1c.

Concerning our hypotheses on reversed causation, intrinsic motivation showed a significant lagged effect on authenticity (Model 1: Υ = 0.21; p = 0.002; Model 2: Υ = 0.23; p = 0.002), supporting H2a. Work ability did not show significant lagged effects on authenticity or any of its subdimensions (Model 1: Υ = −0.08; p = 352; Model 3: Υ = 0.01; p = 0.935). Hence, H2b needs to be rejected. Depressivity showed (marginally) significant lagged effects on authenticity (Model 1: Υ = −0.21; p = 0.071; Model 4: Υ = −0.24; p = 0.040).

Discussion

The purpose of the current study was to investigate lagged reciprocal relationships between work-related authenticity and intrinsic motivation, work ability and depressivity in a sample of employees working for a non-profit organization. Results showed significant lagged effects of work-related authenticity, as a multi-dimensional, global construct on work ability and depressivity, but not on intrinsic motivation. Regarding reverse relationships, intrinsic motivation and depressivity predicted authenticity, but no significant lagged effect of work ability was found.

These results confirm and expand the empirical evidence on the relationship between authenticity and psychological health (Sheldon et al., 1997; Ryan et al., 2005; Kernis and Goldman, 2006; Wood et al., 2008; Robinson et al., 2013). We were able to contribute to previous findings by providing evidence for the idea that there are reciprocal relationships between work-related authenticity and healthy psychological functioning so that healthy psychological functioning and authenticity would positively reinforce each other. Based on our results, it can be concluded that employees that are able to live out their true self at work are expanding their ability to perform and are becoming less depressed. Moreover, increased well-being (high intrinsic motivation and low depressivity) of employees promotes their authentic behavior. The results further point out that authentic behavior at work not only benefits employees in terms of increased well-being, but also the organization as a whole as it leads to increased work ability.

These results show that work-specific authenticity is not only of importance for work-specific outcomes, but also for overall well-being. The reported lagged effects of authenticity on our proposed outcomes confirm findings from Knoll et al. (2015) and Boyraz et al. (2014) with longitudinal data where authenticity was predictive of decreased strain and distress and increased life satisfaction. In contrast, Robinson et al. (2013) did not find a significant relationship between authenticity with work colleagues and general well-being in their American and English subsample. Future studies may take a closer look at domain-specific effects of authenticity and the relation between general and domain-specific authenticity. We also know little about potential compensatory effects across life domains.

Additional exploratory analyses on the sub-dimensions of authenticity revealed that the effect of authenticity on depressivity seems mainly be attributable to unbiased processing, but also authentic behavior. An unbiased evaluation of one's self, including positive as well as negative aspects, as well as openness to personal feedback from others seems to decrease the risk for the development of depressive symptoms. Likewise, taking a stand in social situations, instead of nodding, and agreeing to decisions, which are not in accordance with own norms and values, can help to retain healthy functioning.

We found no evidence for a lagged effect of global work-related authenticity on intrinsic motivation. This conflicts with self-determination theory (Deci and Ryan, 2000), which proposes self-determined behavior to be an important predictor of intrinsic motivation. There was, however, a significant lagged effect from the sub-dimension authentic behavior to intrinsic motivation. It seems that only concrete actions that are in accordance with one's self influence intrinsic motivation. Only concrete actions can alter the social and work characteristics, which in turn promote intrinsic motivation. Authentic behavior is in contrast to “acting merely to please others, or to attain rewards” (Kernis and Goldman, 2006, p. 347), and might even be regarded a strategy of job crafting (e.g., Tims et al., 2012).

It seems important to note that the factor relational orientation, when analyzed in isolation, was neither predictive for our outcomes, nor were there significant lagged effects from intrinsic motivation, work ability or depressivity on relational orientation. It was also the subdimensions which contributed the least to the second order factor of authenticity. On a theoretical level, based on our results, relational orientation seems to be a distinct facet of authenticity that might be more relevant in regard to social relationships within the team and outcomes on team rather than individual level. In accordance with our hypotheses, there were significant lagged effects of intrinsic motivation and depressivity on work-related authenticity. Employees who act intrinsically motivated, feel more intrinsically connected to their work and thus show increased levels of true self behavior. This is interesting to note as it could imply that the awareness for one's true self could be fostered through activities that are resulting out of the intrinsic, true self of a person. Moreover, so far, most studies on authenticity implicitly assumed that authentic awareness would be leading to authentic behavior, yet we would like to raise the question if perhaps authentic behavior could lead to authentic awareness. Contrary to our hypothesis, work ability did not predict authenticity or any of its theoretical facets. Knoll et al. (2015) reported purpose in life to further authenticity. Together with our results, this supports the idea that individuals that report low levels of depressivity have the necessary resources to achieve personal and challenging goals which then fosters eudaimonic well-being. The reason why work ability showed no significant lagged effect may be that it is not only an indicator of personal resources, but also of skills and capabilities, lowering its impact on authenticity in comparison to intrinsic motivation and depressivity.

Additionally, based on the current results, we would like to propose that the processes that are influencing authenticity should not only be regarded as the result of self-determined behavior as suggested by SDT, but they should alternatively be investigated with regard to resources, e.g., in the context of the Conservation of Resources Theory (Hobfoll, 1989). For instance, authenticity might reduce resource loss through less faking behavior and less suppression of thoughts and emotions (Hobfoll, 1989).

Limitations and Future Research

We want to address some limitations of the study as well as implications for further research. First, data came from a single source. Though we consider self-reports to be the most appropriate way to assess authenticity, supplemental assessment by colleagues or peers could provide additional insights (see Knoll et al., 2015). Additionally, our current findings need to be further confirmed with objective data for well-being, psychological health and performance.

In the current sample, the interval between T1 and T2 differed between, and within subsidiaries due to organizational issues which potentially has an effect on parameter estimates. Thus, it cannot be exactly stated that the occurring effects appear within a 6-month time frame. Moreover, due to relatively high stabilities of most investigated variables, longer time lags might show even stronger effects (Dormann and Griffin, 2015). Additionally, though we were able to assess cross-lagged effects with two measurement times, multiple assessments across time are necessary to assess developments and possible non-linear effects (Roe, 2014). Moreover, our data was assessed in one single company. The respondents however came from 63 different sites. We accounted for the dependency of responses of employees working at the same subsidiary by employing multilevel modeling. Though authenticity maybe is of particular importance in the non-profit and in the social sector, future studies should investigate if the impact of authenticity varies across sectors and occupations, e.g., by the extent that workers have to deal with customers, manage emotions or engage themselves personally in their work rather than “just” do their job.

We further propose that future research should examine different facets of authenticity and how they relate to outcomes like work satisfaction, well-being and performance. For example, authenticity could refer to one's values and the extent to which an employee can make decisions based on his or her values (Smallenbroek et al., 2016). Authenticity could also refer to the extent that an employee is able to show emotions, personality traits or abilities that he or she experiences as their “true self.” There are also differences in the understanding and extent of authenticity across cultures and contexts (English and Chen, 2007, 2011; Robinson et al., 2013), making cross cultural comparisons a worthwhile goal for future studies.

Our study revealed that context-specific authenticity had an impact on overall well-being and vice versa. We suggest that future research should investigate how different context-specific authenticity measures contribute to overall well-being and in which ways different well-being indicators impact context-specific authenticity measures. For instance, being authentic in one or more life domains could have buffering effects for being inauthentic in other social contexts. Alternatively, the effects of authenticity in different social roles could be additive and well-being would increase with the number of roles in which a person could be authentic. Moreover, uncertain or unfavorable working conditions might have an impact on the relation between authenticity and well-being outcomes: Davis et al. (2015) found that authenticity was a stronger predictor of self-esteem when future time was limited, considering a limited time frame as a threat to self-esteem. In the same way, authenticity as an “anchor” of one's self-worth could be a stronger predictor under working conditions that are threatening to one's self-esteem such as job insecurity, low occupational status or an effort-reward-imbalance.

Finally, future research should investigate mediating mechanisms between authenticity and healthy psychological functioning and vice versa. SDT could be used as a framework, suggesting that basic need satisfaction mediates the relationship between authentic self-determined behavior and healthy psychological functioning (Leroy et al., 2015). Findings on the relationship between authenticity and power (Kifer et al., 2013) and self-efficacy (Satici et al., 2013) go along with the idea that authentic self-determined behavior leads to an increased satisfaction of the need for competence. Conservation of Resources Theory (Hobfoll, 1989) could be used as an alternative approach as increased or decreased resources due to inauthentic behavior could mediate the relationship between authenticity and health and well-being outcomes. Moreover, we would like to suggest increased self-awareness, self-concordant goals, less pretending behavior and better social relationships as mediating mechanisms for the impact on work ability and depressivity. Further, insights about mediating mechanisms for the reversed relationships are necessary. Our suggestion that intrinsic motivation and depressivity are predictive of work-related authenticity due to the achievement of difficult and challenging goals needs to be empirically tested.

Practical Implications

Based on our results that authenticity in the workplace has a positive impact on employees' well-being, it is recommended to organizations to enable their employees to live in accord with their true selves at work. organizations should strive to enable their employees to be as authentic as possible and not require them to wear a professional mask or to conform to rigid role expectations.

At the same time, fostering well-being in organizations is beneficial for authentic behavior of employees. This is a worthwhile goal as authenticity is not only desirable for a lot of employees and would increase, e.g., their job satisfaction, but it is also necessary in various organizational contexts such as in the service sector or value-based organizations as well as in general in work contexts, e.g., in order to foster positive relationships to colleagues.

We suggest that fostering authenticity is a pivotal opportunity for organizations to conduct primary prevention in order to reduce negative health and increase work ability whereby authenticity can serve as an individual resource, profiting both individuals and organizations. Authenticity enables individuals to be aware of their selves and to critically reflect on their well-being, their performance and development. Being able to always refer back to an “inner, true self” supposedly helps individuals to manage themselves and their lives through work demands and challenges (Di Fabio and Kenny, 2016), especially in the context of the increasing flexibilities and insecurities of the modern work life inside and outside organizations (Savickas, 2011).

We propose that fostering authenticity in organizations can hardly be implemented by “technical” approaches such as setting up training and seminars but it should, rather, penetrate all other activities such as leadership (Avolio et al., 2004; Algera and Lips-Wiersma, 2012) as well as communication and everyday business (Bujisic et al., 2014). However, coaching and training could be useful for increasing the awareness about authenticity and reflecting on how to promote it in daily working life.

Moreover, we suggest that fostering authenticity is of particular importance for non-profit organizations because they are often characterized by less external rewards in comparison to for-profit organizations (Ben-Ner et al., 2011), more unfavorable working conditions (Kosny and Eakin, 2008) and because non-profits are often value- and purpose-driven. Thus, fostering authenticity is a good starting point for its employees to live out the purposes and values of their organization in their daily working lives and in order to promote well-being and health.

Our results imply that workers will be more profitable for the organization if they are “free” to be themselves. Thus, it should lead organizations to reconsider their practices, e.g., in regard to display rules, strict role expectations, autonomy and opportunities for employees to speak up (Hewlin, 2009).

According to Gable and Haidt (2005), positive psychology approaches should ultimately lead from positive individual traits and experiences to positive institutions. We suggest that the best approach to foster individual authenticity is to create environments where employees do not need to “fit in” or follow strict expectations but are rather given the autonomy to express themselves and develop a professional role that suits their identity (Hannah et al., 2011; Grandey et al., 2012; Cable et al., 2013). This will ultimately lead from authentic individuals to authentic teams and an authentic organization as a whole. An authentic organization will then in turn be perceived as genuine and trustworthy (Henderson and Edwards, 2010).

Ethics Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the Institutional and National Guidelines and with the Declaration of World Medical Association (2013). Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. Ethical review and approval was not required for this study as per the institutional and national requirements.

Author Contributions

Study conceptualization: AE and TR. Execution of survey: AE. Analysis and write-up: AE and TR.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer LP and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation, and the handling Editor states that the process nevertheless met the standards of a fair and objective review.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge support from the German Research Foundation (DFG) and Universität Leipzig within the program of Open Access Publishing. We thank Michael Knoll for his helpful comments during the development of the manuscript.

References

Ahlstrom, L., Grimby-Ekman, A., Hagberg, M., and Dellve, L. (2010). The work ability index and single-item question: associations with sick leave, symptoms, and health – a prospective study of women on long-term sick leave. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 36, 404–412. doi: 10.5271/sjweh.2917

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Algera, P. M., and Lips-Wiersma, M. (2012). Radical authentic leadership: co-creating the conditions under which all members of the organization can be authentic. Leadersh. Q. 23, 118–131. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2011.11.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avolio, B. J., Gardner, W. L., Walumbwa, F. O., Luthans, F., and May, D. R. (2004). Unlocking the mask: a look at the process by which authentic leaders impact follower attitudes and behaviors. Leadersh. Q. 15, 801–823. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2004.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. (1967). Depression. New York, NY: Harper and Row.

Google Scholar

Ben-Ner, A., Ren, T., and Paulson, D. F. (2011). A sectoral comparison of wage levels and wage inequality in human services industries. Nonprofit Volunt. Sec. Q. 40, 608–633. doi: 10.1177/0899764010365012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyraz, G., Waits, J. B., and Felix, V. A. (2014). Authenticity, life satisfaction, and distress: a longitudinal analysis. J. Couns. Psychol. 61, 498–505. doi: 10.1037/cou0000031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brislin, R. W. (1986). “The wording and translation of research instruments,” in Cross-cultural Research and Methodology Series: Vol. 8. Field Methods in Cross-cultural Research, eds W. J. Lonner and J. W. Berry (Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications), 137–164.

Bujisic, M., Wu, L., Mattila, A., and Bilgihan, A. (2014). Not all smiles are created equal. Int. J. Contemp. Hospital. Manage. 26, 293–306. doi: 10.1108/IJCHM-10-2012-0181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cable, D. M., Gino, F., and Staats, B. R. (2013). Breaking them in or eliciting their best? Reframing socialization around newcomers' authentic self-expression. Adm. Sci. Q. 58, 1–36. doi: 10.1177/0001839213477098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct. Equation Model. 14, 464–504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, S.-J., Li, F., and Bandalos, D. (2009). Accuracy of the parallel analysis procedure with polychoric correlations. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 69, 748–759. doi: 10.1177/0013164409332229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coovert, M. D., and Craiger, J. P. (2000). An expert system for integrating multiple fit indices for structural equation models. New Rev. Appl. Expert Syst. Emerg. Technol. 6, 39–56.

Davis, W. E., and Hicks, J. A. (2013). Maintaining hope at the 11th hour: authenticity buffers the effect of limited time perspective on hope. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 39, 1634–1646. doi: 10.1177/0146167213500150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, W. E., Hicks, J. A., Schlegel, R. J., Smith, C. M., and Vess, M. (2015). Authenticity and self-esteem across temporal horizons. J. Posit. Psychol. 10, 116–126. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2014.910830

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic Motivation and Self-determination in Human Behavior. New York, NY: Plenum.

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The what and why of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inq. 11, 226–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text

Di Fabio, A., and Kenny, M. E. (2016). From decent work to decent lives: positive self and relational management (PS&RM) in the twenty-first century. Front. Psychol. 7:361. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Fabio, A., and Palazzeschi, L. (2015). Hedonic and eudaimonic well-being: the role of resilience beyond fluid intelligence and personality traits. Front. Psychol. 6:1367. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dormann, C., and Griffin, M. A. (2015). Optimal time lags in panel studies. Psychol. Methods 20, 489–505. doi: 10.1037/met0000041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

El Fassi, M., Bocquet, V., Majery, N., Lair, M. L., Couffignal, S., and Mairiaux, P. (2013). Work ability assessment in a worker population: comparison and determinants of Work Ability Index and Work Ability score. BMC Public Health 13:305. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

English, T., and Chen, S. (2007). Culture and self-concept stability: consistency across and within contexts among Asian Americans and European Americans. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 478–490. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

English, T., and Chen, S. (2011). Self-Concept consistency and culture: the differential impact of two forms of consistency. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 838–849. doi: 10.1177/0146167211400621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eurofound (European Foundation for the Improvement of Living Working Conditions) (2010). Fifth European Working Conditions Survey. Available online at: http://www.eurofound.europa.eu/surveys/data-visualisation/european-working-conditions-survey-2010

Flora, D. B., and Curran, P. J. (2004). An empirical evaluation of alternative methods of estimation for confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data. Psychol. Methods 9, 466–491. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.9.4.466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gable, S. L., and Haidt, J. (2005). What (and why) is positive psychology? Rev. Gen. Psychol. 9, 103–110. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.9.2.103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillath, O., Sesko, A. K., Shaver, P. R., and Chun, D. S. (2010). Attachment, authenticity, and honesty: dispositional and experimentally induced security can reduce self- and other-deception. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 841–855. doi: 10.1037/a0019206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldman, B. M. (2004). The Interrelated Roles of Dispositional Authenticity, Self-Processes, and Global Role Functioning in Affecting Psychological Adjustment. Athens: University of Georgia.

Google Scholar

Grandey, A. A. (2000). Emotional regulation in the workplace: a new way to conceptualize emotional labor. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 5, 95–110. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.5.1.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandey, A., Foo, S. C., Groth, M., and Goodwin, R. E. (2012). Free to be you and me: a climate of authenticity alleviates burnout from emotional labor. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 17, 1–14. doi: 10.1037/a0025102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Güntert, S. T. (2015). The impact of work design, autonomy support, and strategy on employee outcomes: a differentiated perspective on self-determination at work. Motiv. Emot. 39, 74–87. doi: 10.1007/s11031-014-9412-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hannah, S. T., Walumbwa, F. O., and Fry, L. W. (2011). Leadership in action teams: team leader and member's authenticity, authenticity strength, and team outcomes. Pers. Psychol. 64, 771–802. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2011.01225.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harter, S. (2005). “Authenticity,” in Handbook of Positive Psychology, eds C. R. Snyder and S. J. Lopez (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 382–394.

Google Scholar

Hasselhorn, H. M., and Freude, G. (2007). Der Work Ability Index - ein Leitfaden. [The Work Ability Index – A Guideline]. Schriftenreihe der Bundesanstalt für Arbeitsschutz und Arbeitsmedizin. Dortmund; Berlin; Dresden: Verlag für Neue Wissenschaft.

Henderson, A., and Edwards, L. (2010). Authenticity in organisational context: fragmentation, contradiction and loss of control. J. Comm. Manag. 14, 192–205. doi: 10.1108/13632541011064481

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hewlin, P. F. (2003). And the award for the best actor goes to…: Facades of conformity in organizational settings. Acad. Manag. Rev. 28, 633–642.

Google Scholar

Hewlin, P. F. (2009). Wearing the cloak: antecedents and consequences of creating facades of conformity. J. Appl. Psychol. 94, 727–741. doi: 10.1037/a0015228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: a new attempt at conceptualizing stress. Am. Psychol. 44, 513–524. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.3.513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika 30, 179–185. doi: 10.1007/BF02289447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, C., and Cheung, G. W. (2008). Eight general questions on measurement equivalence/invariance that bother me most. Acad. Manag. Proc. 2008, 1–6. doi: 10.5465/AMBPP.2008.33641699

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ilmarinen, J., Tuomi, K., and Klockars, M. (1997). Changes in the work ability of active employees over an 11-year period. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 23(Suppl. 1), 49–57.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kernis, M. H., and Goldman, B. M. (2006). A multicomponent conceptualization of authenticity: theory and research. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38, 283–357. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38006-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiaei, Y. A., and Reio, T. G. (2014). Goal pursuit and eudaimonic well-being among university students: metacognition as the mediator. Behav. Dev. Bull. 19, 91–104. doi: 10.1037/h0101085

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kifer, Y., Heller, D., Perunovic, W. Q., and Galinsky, A. D. (2013). The good life of the powerful: the experience of power and authenticity enhances subjective well-being. Psychol. Sci. 24, 280–288. doi: 10.1177/0956797612450891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knoll, M., Meyer, B., Kroemer, N. B., and Schröder-Abé, M. (2015). It takes two to be yourself. J. Individ. Diff. 36, 38–53. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001/a000153

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knoll, M., and van Dick, R. (2013). Authenticity, employee silence, prohibitive voice, and the moderating effect of organizational identification. J. Posit. Psychol. 8, 346–360. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2013.804113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokkoris, M. D. (2016). Revisiting the relationship between maximizing and well-being: an investigation of eudaimonic well-being. Pers. Individ. Dif. 99, 174–178. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosny, A. A., and Eakin, J. M. (2008). The hazards of helping: work, mission and risk in non-profit social service organizations. Health Risk Soc. 10, 149–166. doi: 10.1080/13698570802159899

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakey, C. E., Kernis, M. H., Heppner, W. L., and Lance, C. E. (2008). Individual differences in authenticity and mindfulness as predictors of verbal defensiveness. J. Res. Pers. 42, 230–238. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S.-K., Dashnyam, A., and Lee, D.-J. (2011). The Effects of intrinsic motivation on job satisfaction and job involvement: focus on the moderate effects of procedural justice, distributive justice. J. Digit. Convergence 9, 157–168.

Google Scholar

Lenton, A. P., Bruder, M., Slabu, L., and Sedikides, C. (2013). How does ‘being real’ feel? The experience of State Authenticity. J. Person. 81, 276–289. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00805.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leroy, H., Anseel, F., Gardner, W. L., and Sels, L. (2015). Authentic leadership, Authentic followership, basic need satisfaction, and work role performance: a cross-level study. J. Manage. 41, 1677–1697. doi: 10.1177/0149206312457822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Little, T. D., Preacher, K. J., Selig, J. P., and Card, N. A. (2007). New developments in latent panel analyses of longitudinal data. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 31, 357–365. doi: 10.1177/0165025407077757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, O. L., and Rijmen, F. (2008). A modified procedure for parallel analysis of ordered categorical data. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 556–562. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.2.556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ménard, J., and Brunet, L. (2011). Authenticity and well-being in the workplace: a mediation model. J. Manage. Psychol. 26, 331–346. doi: 10.1108/02683941111124854

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohr, G. (1986). “Die erfassung psychischer Befindensbeeinträchtigungen bei industriearbeitern [Assessment of mental impairments for industrial workers],” Europäische Hochschulschriften, ed V. I. Reihe (Frankfurt am Main; New York, NY: P. Lang).

Mohr, G. (1991). “Fünf subkonstrukte psychischer Befindensbeeinträchtigungen bei industriearbeitern,” in Auswahl und Entwicklung. [Five Subfacets of Mental Impairments for Industrial Workers: Selection and Development] Psychischer Stress am Arbeitsplatz, eds S. Greif, E. Bamberg, and N. Semmer (Göttingen: Hogrefe), 91–119.

Mohr, G., and Müller, A. (2012). “Depressivität im nichtklinischen Kontext [Depressivity in a nonclinical context],” in, Zusammenstellung sozialwissenschaftlicher Items und Skalen. ZIS Version 15.00 [collection of socio-scientific items and scales. ZIS Version 15.00], ed A. Glöckner-Rist (Bonn: GESIS).

Muthén, L., and Muthén, B. (1998–2015). Mplus User's Guide, 7th Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén Muthén.

Nestler, F. (2011). “Selbstbestimmt - Gut Gestimmt?' Ein Test der Selbstbestimmungstheorie im Arbeitskontext ['self-Determined - in a Good Mood?,” A Test of Self-Determination-Theory in the Workplace]. Unpublished thesis, University of Leipzig.

Richer, S. F., Blanchard, C., and Vallerand, R. J. (2002). A motivational model of work turnover. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 32, 2089–2113. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb02065.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, O. C., Lopez, F. G., Ramos, K., and Nartova-Bochaver, S. (2013). Authenticity, social context, and well-being in the United States, England, and Russia: a three country comparative analysis. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 44, 719–737. doi: 10.1177/0022022112465672

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roe, R. A. (2014). Test validity from a temporal perspective: incorporating time in validation research. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 23, 754–768. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2013.804177

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., LaGuardia, J. G., and Rawsthorne, L. J. (2005). Self-complexity and the Authenticity of self-aspects: effects on well being and resilience to stressful events. N. Am. J. Psychol. 7, 431–448.

Google Scholar

Satici, S. A., Kayis, A. R., and Akin, A. (2013). Investigating the predictive role of social self-efficacy on Authenticity in Turkish University students. Eur. J. Psychol. 9, 572–580. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v9i3.579

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savickas, M. L. (2011). New questions for vocational psychology: premises, paradigms, and practices. J. Career Assess. 19, 251–258. doi: 10.1177/1069072710395532

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheldon, K. M., and Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: the self-concordance model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 482–497. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.3.482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Rawsthorne, L. J., and Ilardi, B. (1997). Trait self and true self: cross-role variation in the big-five personality traits and its relations ith psychological authenticity and subjective well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73, 1380–1393. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.6.1380

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smallenbroek, O., Zelenski, J. M., and Whelan, D. C. (2016). Authenticity as a eudaimonic construct. The relationships among authenticity, values, and valence. J. Posit. Psychol. 11, 1–13. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2016.1187198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, C. (2003). The Ethics of Authenticity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Theran, S. A. (2011). Authenticity in relationships and Depressivity: a gender analysis. Pers. Individ. Dif. 51, 423–428. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tims, M., Bakker, A. B., and Derks, D. (2012). Development and validation of the job crafting scale. J. Vocat. Behav. 80, 173–186. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2011.05.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, M. A., Blanchard, C. M., Taylor, S., Pelletier, L. G., and Villeneuve, M. (2009). Work extrinsic and intrinsic motivation scale: its value for organizational psychology research. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 41, 213–226. doi: 10.1037/a0015167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandenberg, R. J., and Lance, C. E. (2000). A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organ. Res. Methods 3, 4–70. doi: 10.1177/109442810031002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Bosch, R., and Taris, T. W. (2013). Authenticity at work: development and validation of an individual authenticity measure at work. J. Happiness Stud. 15, 1–18. doi: 10.1007/s10902-013-9413-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Bosch, R., and Taris, T. W. (2014). The authentic worker's well-being and performance: the relationship between authenticity at work, well-being, and work outcomes. J. Psychol. 148, 659–681. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2013.820684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van den Broeck, A., Vansteenkiste, M., Witte, H., Soenens, B., and Lens, W. (2010). Capturing autonomy, competence, and relatedness at work: construction and initial validation of the work-related basic need satisfaction scale. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 83, 981–1002. doi: 10.1348/096317909X481382

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walker, C. O., Greene, B. A., and Mansell, R. A. (2006). Identification with academics, intrinsic/extrinsic motivation, and self-efficacy as predictors of cognitive engagement. Learn. Individ. Diff. 16, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2005.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 64, 678–691. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.678

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wickham, R. E. (2013). Perceived authenticity in romantic partners. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 49, 878–887. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, A. M., Linley, P. A., Maltby, J., Baliousis, M., and Joseph, S. (2008). The authentic personality: a theoretical and empirical conceptualization and the development of the authenticity scale. J. Couns. Psychol. 55, 385–399. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.55.3.385

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Medical Association (2013). WMA Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. Retrieved from: http://www.wma.net/en/30publications/10policies/b3/

Keywords: authenticity, well-being, intrinsic motivation, work ability, depression

Citation: Emmerich AI and Rigotti T (2017) Reciprocal Relations between Work-Related Authenticity and Intrinsic Motivation, Work Ability and Depressivity: A Two-Wave Study. Front. Psychol. 8:307. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00307

Received: 19 December 2016; Accepted: 17 February 2017;
Published: 03 March 2017.

Edited by:

Annamaria Di Fabio, University of Florence, Italy

Reviewed by:

Letizia Palazzeschi, University of Florence, Italy
Harsha Perera, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, USA

Copyright © 2017 Emmerich and Rigotti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Astrid I. Emmerich, astrid.emmerich@uni-leipzig.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.