Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 02 May 2017
Sec. Developmental Psychology

Posterior–Anterior Brain Maturation Reflected in Perceptual, Motor and Cognitive Performance

\r\nPatrícia Gervn,*Patrícia Gerván1,2*Pter SoltszPéter Soltész2Orsolya FilepOrsolya Filep2Andrea Berencsi,Andrea Berencsi2,3Ilona Kovcs,Ilona Kovács1,2
  • 1Department of General Psychology, Institute of Psychology, Pázmány Péter Catholic University, Budapest, Hungary
  • 2Laboratory for Psychological Research, Pázmány Péter Catholic University, Budapest, Hungary
  • 3Bárczi Gusztáv Faculty of Special Education, Institute for Methodology of Special Education and Rehabilitation, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary

Based on several postmortem morphometric and in vivo imaging studies it has been postulated that brain maturation roughly follows a caudal to rostral direction. In this study, we linked this maturational pattern to psychological function employing a series of well-established behavioral tasks. We addressed three distinct functions and brain regions with a perceptual (contour integration, CI), motor (finger tapping, FT), and executive control (Navon global–local) task. Our purpose was to investigate basic visual integration functions relying on primary visual cortex (V1) in CI; motor coordination function related to primary motor cortex (M1) in FT, and the executive control component, switching, related to the dorsolateral prefrontal region of the brain in the Navon task. 122 volunteer subjects were recruited to participate in this study between the ages of 10 and 20 (females n = 63, males n = 59). Employing conventional statistical methods, we found that 10 and 12 year olds are performing significantly weaker than 20 year olds in all three tasks. In the CI and Navon global–local tasks, even 14 years old perform poorer than adults. We have also investigated the developmental trajectories by fitting sigmoid curves on our data streams. The analysis of the developmental trajectories of the three tasks showed a posterior to anterior pattern in the emergence of the developmental functions with the earliest development in the visual CI task (V1), followed by motor development in the FT task (M1), and cognitive development as measured in the Navon global–local task (DLPC) being the slowest. Gender difference was also present in FT task showing an earlier maturation for girls in the motor domain.

Introduction

Early postmortem (e.g., Huttenlocher, 1990; Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997) and positron emission tomography (Chugani et al., 1987; Chugani, 1994) studies on cortical gray matter development have already suggested that maturation does not proceed in a homogenous temporal and topographic sequence, but shows a characteristic posterior to anterior direction. Structural MRI studies strengthened the earlier findings and demonstrated that the caudal to rostral direction is discernible on a large scale (Reiss et al., 1996; Sowell et al., 1999). However, more recent imaging studies revealed that the timing of the regional maturation is far more complex than a canonical back to front progression: the temporal sequence of maturation is more connected to the function served by the specific area rather than its location (e.g., Sowell et al., 2003, 2004, 2007; Gogtay et al., 2004). These studies have shown concordant results on the earliest development of cortical brain regions’ underlying basic sensory (i.e., primary visual area in occipital lobe, primary sensory areas of parietal cortex, primary areas for olfaction and taste in the frontal operculum, etc.) and motor functions (precentral gyrus of the frontal lobe). Furthermore, findings also demonstrated that areas connected to complex pattern processing (inferior, posterior temporal areas) or spatial orientation and attention (inferior parietal regions) mature next, and finally, regions involved in complex executive functions and multimodal integration (orbitofrontal and superior temporal areas) develop well into adolescence.

Similar to gray matter maturation, white matter (WM) also shows a massive change during childhood and adolescence (e.g., Courchesne et al., 2000; Matsuzawa et al., 2001; Sowell et al., 2003). Several cross-sectional structural MRI studies (e.g., Girard et al., 1991; Paus et al., 2001) and a longitudinal study (Thompson et al., 2000) reported that the age-related pattern of myelination also proceeds along a caudal-rostral arc, however, others failed to find these systematic regional differences in WM developmental changes (e.g., Giedd et al., 1999; Courchesne et al., 2000). Diffusion tensor imaging studies showed that the somatosensory pathway matures early in infancy (Qiu et al., 2008), while frontotemporal tracts showed extended maturational trajectories persisting over adolescence (e.g., Lebel et al., 2008; Asato et al., 2010). These results agree with the earlier mentioned anatomical MRI studies on cortical gray matter maturation.

Gender differences in brain maturation are present already in fetal life. Sex steroids and other hormones significantly affect neural development (see e.g., Giedd et al., 1997). Adolescence, with a massive change in hormone levels, results in further sexual dimorphisms in brain development (see e.g., Lenroot et al., 2007; Lenroot and Giedd, 2010). The elevations in luteinizing hormone and inhibin B levels are clear endocrinological markers of the onset of puberty in both genders (Lahlou and Roger, 2004). An increased production of sex steroids in both males and females also accompanies these changes. The onset of hormonal puberty occurs later in boys. Puberty onset determined by inhibin B levels is between the age of 11 and 12 years in boys (Crofton et al., 2002), and between 10.1 and 10.4 years in girls (Addo et al., 2014). These results are in good agreement with imaging studies that have shown differences in the temporal pattern of brain maturation between the genders. An approximately 1–2 years shift between girls and boys has been reported in terms of peaks in gray matter: 8.5 years in females, and 10.5 years in males (Lenroot et al., 2007). Also indicating an earlier maturation for girls, more prominent cortical surface area expansion was found in males compared to females between 8 and 14 years of age (Koolschijn and Crone, 2013). There are gender differences in WM development in terms of timing and volumetric changes as well. During childhood and adolescence females have an overall earlier maturation of WM tracts than males (Asato et al., 2010), while boys show a far more prominent WM volumetric increase than girls (De Bellis et al., 2001; Lenroot et al., 2007). Current MRI studies seem to validate the view that WM volume increase during the teenage years is associated with testosterone levels and androgen receptor genes in adolescent boys (Perrin et al., 2008; Paus et al., 2010), and luteinizing hormone levels in both genders (Peper et al., 2008).

Our purpose was to investigate whether the posterior–anterior wave of cortical structural changes, possibly determined by pubertal hormones, can be matched to a similar wave of improvement in behavioral function. The issue of matching whole-brain cortical structure to function is a complex one, especially in the developmental domain. However, an approximation with probes at strategic points both in terms of structure and function, and in terms of developmental time might be a good start. To this end, we selected well-established behavioral tasks that are believed to be localized differently along the sagittal axis of the brain. We use these tasks to probe how anatomical maturity might be linked to developmental trajectories of different functions (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Summary of the paradigms and related brain regions. This side view of the human brain shows the three Brodmann areas (BA) addressed by the tasks used in this study. (A) The panel shows stimuli at three levels of difficulty in the Contour integration (CI) task. These stimuli are addressing long-range connections in the primary visual cortex (V1; BA 17). The collinear chain of Gabor patches forming a horizontally placed egg-shape is hidden in the background of randomly positioned and oriented noise elements. Relative noise density varied through six difficulty levels. (B) Four-element movement-sequence in the Finger tapping (FT) task addressing long-range connectivity of the primary motor cortex (M1; BA 4). Subjects were instructed to touch four other fingers with the non-dominant thumb in a given order which was index – ring – middle –little finger. Participants had to carry out the sequence repetitively, as fast and as correctly as they could. (C) The Navon global–local (Navon GL) task consisted of hierarchical stimuli of geometric shapes where the lines of a larger, “global” shape are composed of much smaller, “local” figures (cc. 10th the size of the large ones). Participants were instructed to identify the shape, either at the global or the local level, depending on the color of the background, which was yellow or blue, respectively. This task requires the use of a set of executive control components such as switching and updating which have neural correlates in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 46. BA9).

Behavioral Tasks as Probes of Posterior–Anterior Brain Maturation

Contour Integration Task

Stimuli consist of a collinear chain of Gabor patches embedded in the background of randomly positioned and oriented noise elements (see e.g., Figure 1A). This paradigm has been developed to test the long-range intrinsic cortical connections in the primary visual cortex (Field et al., 1993; Kovács and Julesz, 1993). This paradigm has been extensively studied in the last several decades in the field of visual integration, and it has a well-established account on the underlying neural mechanisms. Neurophysiological studies described a correlation between the responses of neurons in V1 and the perceptual saliency of contours (Li et al., 2006), supporting the idea that V1 has a cardinal role in contour integration. Optical imaging of contextual interactions in monkeys (Kinoshita et al., 2009), human neuropsychological (Giersch et al., 2000), and fMRI (Altmann et al., 2003; Kourtzi et al., 2003) studies also indicate the relevance of low-level visual areas in detecting and integrating the contour elements embedded in noise.

Finger Tapping Task

Finger tapping is a motor coordination paradigm. It has many variations in terms of the complexity of the tapping task and a ‘pacing’ stimulus. Here we used a self-paced version where participants have to touch the thumb with the other fingers in a given four-element sequence (see Figure 1B). Motor function of distal arm and hand movements is controlled by four distinct regions in the frontal lobes: the primary motor cortex (M1), supplementary motor area, the lateral premotor cortex and the cingulate motor area (Todorov, 2000; Lemon, 2008). Direct input to the spinal cord from these areas is shown to be necessary for manual dexterity in primates (Dum and Strick, 2004, 2005). The majority of cortical motor neurons that give direct input to lower motor neurons in the spinal cord originate in M1 (Seo and Jang, 2013), ensuring fast and selective activation of hand muscles. Several studies combined FT with imaging techniques, TMS or electrophysiology leading to increasing amount of evidence of practice driven changes in M1 (Karni et al., 1995, 1998; Muellbacher et al., 2002; Zhu et al., 2010). Changes occurred in the contralateral primary motor cortex during simple, self-paced movements.

Navon Global–Local Task

In the Navon global–local task (Miyake et al., 2000), a geometric figure (a greater figure is composed of several smaller figures; see Figure 1C) is presented to the subjects, and they are requested to respond either to the “global,” or to the “local” stimuli depending on a visual cue (Navon, 1977). This cue-based switching requires interpreting a display of symbols, selectively attending to certain features and ignoring others, and remembering and applying a complex set of rules. Shifting, or in other terms, switching is an executive control component identified by Miyake et al. (2000). It is assumed to be a component of any goal-directed behavior (Miller and Cohen, 2001). Neuroimaging studies have linked the performance on the Navon global–local task mainly to prefrontal, parietal and subcortical areas, and especially to the bilateral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC, BA 32), the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC, BA 9 and 46), the medial regions of the inferior frontal gyrus (BA 45/47) extending into the insula, and the inferior parietal lobule (IPL, BA 7 and 40) (e.g., Hedden and Gabrieli, 2010). Switching has generally been related to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (e.g., MacDonald et al., 2000; Monsell, 2003). This is consistent with neuropsychological findings demonstrating that patients with left frontal damage perform poorly in switching between attributes (Rogers et al., 1998; Keele and Rafal, 2000). In a task-switching version of the Stroop test, the left DLPF seemed to contribute to the maintenance of task demands in switching, while ACC activation was related to performance monitoring (MacDonald et al., 2000). The role of parietal regions in the Navon global–local task was investigated in repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (Qin and Han, 2007) and transcranial direct current stimulation (Bardi et al., 2013) studies. The results show that the parietal areas are involved in attaining the level of stimulus representation and not directly in the process of switching.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

One hundred and twenty-two volunteer subjects participated in this study (female n = 63, male = 59). Subjects were voluntarily recruited via the Internet by publicizing and advertising our research. All participants were healthy, right-handed individuals with normal or corrected to normal vision. Participants had no history of psychiatric or neurological illnesses, including ADHD, and were free of any medication. Adult participants or the legal guardians of subjects under the age of 18 were provided with written information, and were asked for consent before participating in the study. Subjects participated in two experimental sessions at the Developmental Neuroscience Laboratory of the Institute of Psychology at PPCU, and were paid for their attendance.

The sample included subjects between the ages of 10 and 20, in 6 age groups. Five subjects were excluded based on their inadequate performance on at least one of the subtests. Additional subjects were recruited subsequently. Table 1 shows the demographics of the sample.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Demographic data of the participants.

The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Institute of Psychology at PPCU.

Procedure

Contour Integration Task

Stimuli were composed of a collinear chain of Gabor elements forming a horizontally placed egg shape on a background of randomly positioned and oriented Gabor patches (see Figure 1A). The relative noise density (D) was varied throughout six difficulty levels. D is defined as the ratio of average noise spacing over contour spacing (see Kovács and Julesz, 1993). In our study, D ranged between 1.1 and 0.6, and was varied with a step size of 0.1. Blocks of images were presented in an increasing order of difficulty, starting with the easiest (D = 1.1) level, and followed by more difficult levels (up to D = 0.6), in a two-alternative forced-choice procedure. The CI task was presented using a HP ProBook 450 G3 laptop computer with a 15.6 inch monitor, and was programmed in Delphi language. Stimulus duration was 2000 ms, with a fixation cross between stimuli (0.5 s, or until the subject responded). The task was to indicate which side of the screen the narrower part of the egg was pointing to by pressing one of two assigned buttons on the keyboard. Subjects were tested binocularly and were seated at about 0.4 m away from the monitor in a normally lit testing room. The size of the stimulus field was 19.93 × 26.57 degrees of visual angle.

Finger Tapping Task

Participants were asked to touch the thumb with the other fingers of the non-dominant hand (see Figure 1B) in a predetermined order, and as fast and as correctly as possible. 160 repetitions of a four-element-sequence (index-ring-middle-little finger) were distributed into 10 practice blocks with rests between them (the length of the rest was controlled by the subjects). Data acquisition was performed by a custom made data glove. It consisted of metal rings placed on each fingertip that were connected to a PC with a Java-based data acquisition software that detected exact timing and order of finger taps.

Taking the speed/accuracy trade-off into account, motor performance was monitored both in terms of speed and error rate. Performance rate was calculated by multiplying the time between finger taps (speed) with the ratio of the number of finger taps in incorrect sequences compared to all sequences (error rate). In order to eliminate the effect of different corticospinal tract myelination levels (due to e.g., age) on speed, participants had to carry out an additional task where they were asked to touch the thumb with the index finger of the non-dominant hand as fast as they could (maximum finger tapping speed). The above calculated performance rate in the sequential FT task was corrected with the maximum motor speed by subtracting the time between finger taps in the maximum motor speed task from the time between taps in the sequential task, then it was multiplied by the error rate of the sequential task.

Navon Global–Local Task

The Navon global–local task consisted of hierarchical stimuli of geometric shapes, often called Navon figures (Navon, 1977, see Figure 1C), presented on a computer screen. In a Navon figure, the lines of a larger, “global” shape are composed of much smaller, “local” figures, 10th the size of the large ones. In this version of the task, geometric shapes (circle, triangle, x, and square) were used instead of letters to rule out the effect of reading experience in younger subjects. The Navon global–local task was presented using a HP ProBook 450 G3 laptop computer with a 15.6 inch monitor, and was programmed with the OpenSesame 3.0 software (Mathôt et al., 2012). Participants were instructed to identify the shape, either at the global or the local level, depending on the color of the background, which was blue or yellow, respectively. Subjects were asked to provide button-press responses on the computer keyboard. Stimuli were organized into three blocks. The first and second blocks involved 24 randomized non-shifting trials (only blue/global or only yellow/local) and the third consisted of 48 quasi-random shifting trials, where 25 of the trials required a switch from local to global features or vice-versa. The order of non-shifting trials was alternated among subjects, half of the subjects started with the global set of the task followed by the local set and vice versa. Each block was preceded with instructions and a practice block with 6 stimuli for the non-shifting, and 12 stimuli for the shifting condition. Stimuli appeared for 600 ms, followed by a visual cue for the location of the responding keys, and the trial ended with a resting screen. Responses were allowed only after the stimulus ceased from the screen. Reaction time (RT) and precision were recorded. We expressed the ‘cost’ of switching by calculating the difference between the RT of the correct responses in the alternation trials (participants were required to switch rules between two consecutive stimuli) and the average RT of the correct responses in the repetition global and local trials (participants did not have to switch rules).

Results

Data Analysis

For descriptive statistical analysis IBM SPSS Version 21.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) was used. Performance less than 60% correct on one of the conditions or outliers with greater values than 2 SDs at age and gender group-level per condition were excluded from the analysis, which amounted less than 10% of the sample. Table 2 presents the descriptive statistics of the data.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Descriptive statistics of the data.

Multivariate ANOVA was used to analyze the relationship between each behavioral task, gender and age.

Higher levels of contour integration (CI) performance were associated with development [F(5,99) = 14.210, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.418], and gender [F(1,99) = 5.296, p = 0.023, ηp2 = 0.051], females demonstrating better performance. Gender × development interaction was not present [F(99,546) = 0.809, p = 0.546, ηp2 = 0.039]. Simple contrasts test revealed 10 [t(35) = -6.272,35, p < 0.001, r2 = 0.727], 12 [t(37) = -4.844, p < 0.001, r2 = 0.623], and 14 [r(36) = -2.469, p = 0.016, r2 = 0.381] years old adolescents showing a significantly poorer performance than the 20 years old group (adults).

Higher levels of finger tapping (FT) performance were associated with age [F(5,102) = 10.326, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.336], but there was no main effect of gender [F(1,102) = 1.723, p = 0.192, ηp2 = 0.017], or gender × age interaction [F(5,102) = 1.777, p = 0.124, ηp2 = 0.080]. Simple contrasts test indicated significant differences between 10 [t(35) = -5.875, p < 0.001, r2 = 0.705] and 12 [t(37) = -3.877, p < 0.001, r2 = 0.537] years olds versus adults.

Higher performance on the Navon global–local task was associated with age [F(5,102) = 7.794, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.276], but there was no main gender effect [F(1,102) = 1.159, p = 0.284, ηp2 = 0.011] or gender × age interaction [F(5,102) = 0.233, p = 0.947, ηp2 = 0.011] evident. Simple contrasts test revealed, that 10 [t(38) = 5.22, p < 0.001, r2 = 0.646] and 12 [t(77) = 2.078, p = 0.04, r2 = 0.319] years olds preformed significantly worse, than adults (20 years).

Sigmoid Curve Fitting

As a fitting method we used the Curve Fitting Toolbox of MatLab (2014b). The three data streams were transformed so that their zero value has a meaning of the entire function missing thus enabling us omitting one parameter (see Figure 2). We fitted data using the Curve Fitting Toolbox of MatLab (2014b) with the equation of

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Derived developmental trajectories for the three tasks. The curves exhibit the characteristic ‘S’-shaped curve of a sigmoid. Red and blue stands for females and males, respectively. Lighter red and blue dots represent the female and male normalized data, and the lighter, dotted lines depict the 95% prediction bounds. (A) Fitted sigmoid curves for normalized CI performance data. (B) Fitted sigmoid curves for normalized FT performance (C) Fitted sigmoid curves for normalized Navon global–local task.

Predicted function level=0+Saturation level1+einflexionpoint-agedevelopmentspeed

Different fittings were calculated for females and males and after the fitting process we transformed the data and the fitted curves in a way that different saturation levels of males and females were filtered out their respective saturation levels transformed to 1. Thus, we eliminated gender performance differences to concentrate purely on the developmental process dynamics of the two genders.

We also constrained the saturation level parameter between 0 and 2, the age at development deceleration parameter between 0 and 25 and development speed parameter between 0.5 and 25, note that age was measured in months before transformation. The descriptive details of the fitted models are presented in Table 3. A potential limitation of the interpretation of our data is that the model explains less than 50% variance. For further theoretical and technical details about the curve fitting see Supplementary Material.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Details of the fitted sigmoid curves for females and males in the three tasks.

Comparing Developmental Trajectories

Figure 3 illustrates the developmental trajectories of the tree tasks (Figures 2A–C) for females and males, and the maximum acceleration timings of these functions. By analyzing the developmental trajectories of the three tasks, we found a posterior to anterior pattern in the emergence of the inflection points of the fitted sigmoid functions. Inflection points are at the earliest ages in the CI task (8.75 years on average), the FT task follows (9.95 years on average), and the last is the Navon global–local task (11.6 years on average).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Developmental trajectories of the tree tasks for females and males. In this figure, the most relevant areas of the fitted sigmoid curves are highlighted: these are the steepest parts of the slopes between the lower and upper plateau, the so called inflection points. The vertical lines project the inflection points of the sigmoid curves to the x-axis, showing the age when the maximum acceleration occurs according to the model. The earliest inflection points occur in the CI task (girls: 8.6 years, boys: 8.9 years), followed by the FT task (girls: 9.4 years, boys: 10.5 years). The latest inflection points appear in the Navon global–local task (girls: 11.5 years, boys: 11.7 years).

Gender differences were also present in all tasks showing an earlier development for girls. A small gender difference occurred in the CI task (inflection at 8.6 years for girls, and 8.9 years for girls). In the FT task the difference was larger (9.4 years for girls, and 10.5 years for boys), and it was also relatively small in the Navon global–local task (11.5 years for girls, and 11.7 years for boys).

Discussion

Our purpose was to investigate the similarities between behavioral development and brain maturation during childhood and adolescence. We focused on three specific functions related to distinct cortical areas. The behavioral paradigms were well-established paradigms with extensively studied neural correlates: (i) low-level visual spatial integration relying on the primary visual area was addressed by contour integration task; (ii) fine motor control function, mediated by the primary motor cortex in the precentral gyrus in the posterior frontal lobe, was investigated by a self-paced finger tapping task; and (iii) executive control, mediated by the dorsolateral prefrontal cortical area, was studied by the Navon global–local task. After obtaining data from 122 typically developing subjects, we applied fitted sigmoid curves on the normalized and corrected raw performance data of the three tasks for females and males. Sigmoid curves have a distinguished part called inflection point which signifies the point where development is the fastest and most prominent. Acknowledging the theoretical considerations and neural correlates provided by several researches for the three tasks we employed, we found a similar posterior to anterior pattern in the maturation of the task related functions. Emergence of the inflection points of the fitted sigmoid functions is earliest in the case of the CI task (female = 8.6 years; male = 8.9 years), the FT task follows (female = 9.4 years; male = 10.5 years) and the last is the Navon global–local task (female = 11.5 years; male = 11.7 years). Imaging studies also agree on the earliest maturation of the occipital pole (Sowell et al., 2003, 2004, 2007; Gogtay et al., 2004).

Developmental changes of the frontal lobe display an inhomogeneous timing; maturation shows roughly a posterior to anterior direction with an exception of the frontal pole that matures at about same time as posterior regions (Gogtay et al., 2004). Accordingly, developmental alterations start earliest in the precentral gyrus (primary motor cortex), and the prefrontal cortex matures last (Gogtay et al., 2004). The data obtained from the sigmoid models are consistent with the imaging findings revealing a posterior to anterior direction of frontal lobe maturation, sigmoid curves of FT task show earlier inflection points than curves related to Navon global–local task.

Brain maturation displays a gender specific timing and pattern during childhood and adolescence (for review see e.g., Lenroot and Giedd, 2010), girls mature earlier than boys. Our results also show gender difference in the timing of the changes in behavioral functions connected to M1 area, while there is only a slight difference between males and females in the visual and executive control task. In the fine motor task (FT), female and male developmental curves reach the inflection points by the age 9.4 and 10.5 showing 1.1 years difference between gender groups. Delayed developmental growth curves for males in the FT task could be related to later volume growth peaks for frontal GM in males (Giedd et al., 1997; Giedd and Rapoport, 2010) and earlier myelination for females (Benes et al., 1994). The earlier inflection points in females compared to males may reflect an influence of sex hormones on the maturation of these brain regions. In the Navon global–local task, the female and male developmental curves reach the inflection points by the age of 11.5 and 11.7, respectively. The lack of significant gender difference might be explained by the explained by the massive inhomogeneity of males’ performance in this task. In CI task, female and male developmental curves reach the inflection points by the age of 8.6 and 8.9, respectively. This is consistent with the findings of a recent structural MRI study (Koolschijn and Crone, 2013) where data show only moderate occipital volume gray matter loss after age of 8, indicating that the vast majority of structural maturational changes of this lobule have proceeded by this age. The volumetric loss in gray matter is likely associated with two simultaneous maturational processes: synaptic pruning, i.e., the elimination of unused synaptic connections (see e.g., Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997); WM growth, i.e., myelination (see e.g., Benes et al., 1994; Courchesne et al., 2000; Gogtay et al., 2004). However, the study of Koolschijn and Crone (2013) reports temporal, parietal and frontal gray matter volume loss in early adolescent years which could be associated with more protracted brain maturation in these lobules. The occurrence of the maximum acceleration of maturation prior to puberty, and the lack of significant gender differences in the CI task might imply that the maturation of this visual function is less hormonally driven than the other two investigated functions.

We conclude that the posterior to anterior structural and functional maturational direction of the human brain could be grasped by behavioral paradigm addressing specific cortical areas. Clearly, further research with younger age groups is needed to verify our developmental trajectory predictions regarding the exact timing of the maximum accelerations of the investigated functions.

Author Contributions

IK and PG contributed to the conception and design of the research, and wrote the paper. OF coordinated data acquisition and analysis. PS fostered data analysis and the interpretation of the results. AB participated in the analysis of the data. All authors discussed the results and implications and commented on the manuscript at all stages. All authors approved the manuscript and this submission.

Funding

Supported by OTKA NN 110466 to IK.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00674/full#supplementary-material

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Addo, O. Y., Miller, B. S., Lee, P. A., Hediger, M. L., and Himes, J. H. (2014). Age of Hormonal onset of Puberty based on Luteinizing Hormone, Inhibin B, and Body Composition in Preadolescent U.S. girls. Pediatr. Res. 76, 564–570. doi: 10.1038/pr.2014.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altmann, C. F., Bulthoff, H. H., and Kourtzi, Z. (2003). Perceptual organization of local elements into global shapes in the human visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 13, 342–349. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00052-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asato, M. R., Terwilliger, R., Woo, J., and Luna, B. (2010). White matter development in adolescence: a DTI study. Cereb. Cortex 20, 2122–2131. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bardi, L., Kanai, R., Mapelli, D., and Walsh, V. (2013). Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) reveals parietal asymmetry in local/global and salience-based selection. Cortex 49, 850–860. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90349-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ben Bashat, D., Kronfeld-Duenias, V., Zachor, D., Ekstein, P., Hendler, T., Tarrasch, R., et al. (2007). Accelerated maturation of white matter in young children with autism: a high b value DWI study. Neuroimage 37, 40–47. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benes, F. M., Turtle, M., Khan, Y., and Farol, P. (1994). Myelination of a key relay zone in the hippocampal formation occurs in the human brain during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51, 477–484. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950060041004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chugani, H. T. (1994). “Development of regional brain glucose metabolism in relation to behavior and plasticity,” in Human Behavior and the Developing Brain, eds G. Dawson and K. W. Fischer (New York, NY: Guilford), 153–175.

Google Scholar

Chugani, H. T., Phelps, M. E., and Mazziotta, J. C. (1987). Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development. Ann. Neurol. 22, 487–497. doi: 10.1002/ana.410220408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Courchesne, E., Chisum, H. J., Townsend, J., Cowles, A., Covington, J., Egaas, B., et al. (2000). Normal brain development and aging: quantitative analysis at in vivo MR imaging in healthy volunteers. Radiology 216, 672–682. doi: 10.1148/radiology.216.3.r00au37672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crofton, P. M., Evans, A. E., Groome, N. P., Taylor, M. R., Holland, C. V., and Kelnar, C. J. (2002). Inhibin B in boys from birth to adulthood: relationship with age, pubertal stage, FSH and testosterone. Clin. Endocrinol. 56, 215–221. doi: 10.1046/j.0300-0664.2001.01448.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Bellis, M. D., Keshavan, M. S., Beers, S. R., Hall, J., Frustaci, K., Masalehdan, A., et al. (2001). Sex differences in brain maturation during childhood and adolescence. Cereb. Cortex 11, 552–557. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.6.552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dean, D. C. III, O’Muircheartaigh, J., Dirks, H., Waskiewicz, N., Lehman, K., Walker, L., et al. (2014). Modeling healthy male white matter and myelin development: 3 through 60 months of age. Neuroimage 84, 742–752. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deoni, S. C. L., Dean, D. C., O’Muircheartaigh, J., Dirks, H., and Jerskey, B. A. (2012). Investigating white matter development in infancy and early childhood using myelin water faction and relaxation time mapping. Neuroimage 63, 1038–1053. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dum, R. P., and Strick, P. L. (2004). “Motor areas in the frontal lobe: the anatomical substrate for the central control of movement,” in Motor Cortex in Voluntary Movements: A Distributed System for Distributed Functions, eds A. Riehle and E. Vaadia (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press).

Google Scholar

Dum, R. P., and Strick, P. L. (2005). Frontal lobe inputs to the digit representations of the motor areas on the lateral surface of the hemisphere. J. Neurosci. 25, 1375–1386. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3902-04.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, D. J., Hayes, A., and Hess, R. F. (1993). Contour integration by the human visual system: evidence for a local “association field”. Vis. Res. 33, 173–193. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90156-Q

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., et al. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedd, J. N., Castellanos, F. X., Rajapakse, J. C., Vaituzis, A. C., and Rapoport, J. L. (1997). Sexual dimorphism of the developing human brain. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 21, 1185–1201. doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(97)00158-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedd, J. N., and Rapoport, J. L. (2010). Structural MRI of pediatric brain development: what have we learned and where are we going? Neuron 67, 728–734. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedd, J. N., Snell, J. W., Lange, N., Rajapakse, J. C., Casey, B. J., Kozuch, P. L., et al. (1996). Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development: ages 4–18. Cereb. Cortex 6, 551–559. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.4.551

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giersch, A., Humphreys, G., Boucart, M., and Kovacs, I. (2000). The computation of occluded contours in visual agnosia: evidence for early computation prior to shape binding and figure-ground coding. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 17, 731–759. doi: 10.1080/026432900750038317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Girard, N., Raybaud, C., and du Lac, P. (1991). MRI study of brain myelination. J. Neuroradiol. 18, 291–307.

Google Scholar

Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 8174–8179. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0402680101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hedden, T., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2010). Shared and selective neural correlates of inhibition, facilitation, and shifting processes during executive control. Neuroimage 51, 421–431. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huttenlocher, P. R. (1990). Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia 28, 517–527. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90031-I

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huttenlocher, P. R., and Dabholkar, A. S. (1997). Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 387, 167–178. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19971020)387:2<167::AID-CNE1>3.0.CO;2-Z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jandó, G., Mikó-Baráth, E., Markó, K., Hollódy, K., Török, B., and Kovacs, I. (2012). Early-onset binocularity in preterm infants reveals experience-dependent visual development in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 11049–11052. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1203096109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karni, A., Meyer, G., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., and Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature 377, 155–158. doi: 10.1038/377155a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karni, A., Meyer, G., Rey-Hipolito, C., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., et al. (1998). The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-drive changes in primary motor cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 861–868. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.861

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keele, S. W., and Rafal, R. (2000). “Deficits of task-set in patients with left prefrontal cortex lesions,” in Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII, eds S. Monsell and J. S. Driver (Cambridge. MA: MIT Press), 627–651.

Google Scholar

Kinoshita, M., Gilbert, C. D., and Das, A. (2009). Optical imaging of contextual interactions in V1 of the behaving monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 1930–1944. doi: 10.1152/jn.90882.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koolschijn, P. C., and Crone, E. A. (2013). Sex differences and structural brain maturation from childhood to early adulthood. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 106–118. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kourtzi, Z., Tolias, A. S., Altmann, C. F., Augath, M., and Logothetis, N. K. (2003). Integration of local features into global shapes: monkey and human FMRI studies. Neuron 37, 333–346. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01174-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovacs, I., and Julesz, B. (1993). A closed curve is much more than an incomplete one — effect of closure on figure ground segmentation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 90, 7495–7497. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.16.7495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lahlou, N., and Roger, M. (2004). Inhibin B in pubertal development and pubertal disorders. Semin. Reprod. Med. 22, 165–175. doi: 10.1055/s-2004-831892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebel, C., Walker, L., Leemans, A., Phillips, L., and Beaulieu, C. (2008). Microstructural maturation of the human brain from childhood to adulthood. Neuroimage 40, 1044–1055. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemon, R. N. (2008). Descending pathways in motor control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 195–218. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenroot, R. K., and Giedd, J. N. (2010). Sex differences in the adolescent brain. Brain Cogn. 72:46. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenroot, R. K., Gogtay, N., Greenstein, D. K., Wells, E. M., Wallace, G. L., Clasen, L. S., et al. (2007). Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence. Neuroimage 36, 1065–1073. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, W., Piech, V., and Gilbert, C. D. (2006). Contour saliency in primary visual cortex. Neuron 50, 951–962. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., and Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science 288, 1835–1838. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1835

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., and Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: an open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 44, 314–324. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuzawa, J., Matsui, M., Konishi, T., Noguchi, K., Gur, R. C., Bilker, W., et al. (2001). Age-related volumetric changes of brain gray and white matter in healthy infants and children. Cereb. Cortex 11, 335–342. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.4.335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 134–140. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muellbacher, W., Ziemann, U., Wissel, J., Dang, N., Kofler, M., Facchini, S., et al. (2002). Early consolidation in human primary motor cortex. Nature 415, 640–644. doi: 10.1038/nature712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception. Cogn. Psychol. 9, 353–383. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paus, T., Collins, D. L., Evans, A. C., Leonard, G., Pike, B., and Zijdenbos, A. (2001). Maturation of white matter in the human brain: a review of magnetic resonance studies. Brain Res. Bull. 54, 255–266. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(00)00434-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paus, T., Nawaz-Khan, I., Leonard, G., Perron, M., Pike, G. B., Pitiot, A., et al. (2010). Sexual dimorphism in the adolescent brain: role of testosterone and androgen receptor in global and local volumes of grey and white matter. Horm. Behav. 57, 63–75. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peper, J. S., Brouwer, R. M., Schnack, H. G., van Baal, G. C., van Leeuwen, M., van den Berg, S. M., et al. (2008). Cerebral white matter in early puberty is associated with luteinizing hormone concentrations. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 909–915. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrin, J. S., Hervé, P. Y., Leonard, G., Perron, M., Pike, G. B., Pitiot, A., et al. (2008). Growth of white matter in the adolescent brain: role of testosterone and androgen receptor. J. Neurosci. 28, 9519–9524. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1212-08.2008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qin, J., and Han, S. (2007). The role of parietal cortex in global/local processing of hierarchical stimuli: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Neuroreport 18, 1921–1924. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f1c9d2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qiu, D., Tan, L. H., Zhou, K., and Khong, P. L. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging of normal white matter maturation from late childhood to young adulthood: voxel-wise evaluation of mean diffusivity, fractional anisotropy, radial and axial diffusivities. and correlation with reading development. Neuroimage 41, 223–232. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiss, A. L., Abrams, M. T., Singer, H. S., Ross, J. L., and Denckla, M. B. (1996). Brain development, gender and IQ in children. A volumetric imaging study. Brain 1119, 1763–1774. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.5.1763

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, R. D., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., Polkey, C. E., Kennard, C., and Robbins, T. W. (1998). Dissociating executive mechanisms of task control following frontal lobe damage and Parkinson’s disease. Brain 121, 815–842. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.5.815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seo, J. P., and Jang, S. H. (2013). Different characteristics of the corticospinal tract according to the cerebral origin: DTI study. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 34, 1359–1363. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A3389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowell, E. R., Peterson, B. S., Thompson, P. M., Welcome, S. E., Henkenius, A. L., and Toga, A. W. (2003). Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 309–315. doi: 10.1038/nn1008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Holmes, C. J., Batth, R., Jernigan, T. L., and Toga, A. W. (1999). Localizing age-related changes in brain structure between childhood and adolescence using statistical parametric mapping. Neuroimage 9, 587–597. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Leonard, C. M., Welcome, S. E., Kan, E., and Toga, A. W. (2004). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and brain growth in normal children. J. Neurosci. 24, 8223–8231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1798-04.2004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., and Toga, A. W. (2007). “Mapping adolescent brain maturation using structural magnetic resonance imaging,” in Adolescent Psychopathology and the Developing Brain, eds D. Romer and E. R. Walker (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 55–84. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195306255.003.0003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, P. M., Giedd, J. N., Woods, R. P., MacDonald, D., Evans, A. C., and Toga, A. W. (2000). Growth patterns in the developing brain detected by using continuum mechanical tensor maps. Nature 404, 190–193. doi: 10.1038/35004593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todorov, E. (2000). Direct cortical control of muscle activation in voluntary arm movements: a model. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 391–398. doi: 10.1038/73964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, F. F., Maxwell, J. P., Hu, Y., Zhang, Z. G., Lam, W. K., Poolton, J. M., et al. (2010). EEG activity during the verbal-cognitive stage of motor skill acquisition. Biol. Psychol. 84, 221–227. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: contour integration, finger tapping, Navon global–local task, V1, M1, DLPC, brain maturation, gender differences

Citation: Gerván P, Soltész P, Filep O, Berencsi A and Kovács I (2017) Posterior–Anterior Brain Maturation Reflected in Perceptual, Motor and Cognitive Performance. Front. Psychol. 8:674. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00674

Received: 04 March 2016; Accepted: 13 April 2017;
Published: 02 May 2017.

Edited by:

Dagmara Dimitriou, UCL Institute of Education, UK

Reviewed by:

Josie Booth, University of Edinburgh, UK
Maciej Haman, University of Warsaw, Poland

Copyright © 2017 Gerván, Soltész, Filep, Berencsi and Kovács. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Patrícia Gerván, pgervan@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.