Skip to main content

FOCUSED REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 21 June 2017
Sec. Decision Neuroscience

A Potential Role for mu-Opioids in Mediating the Positive Effects of Gratitude

  • 1Psychology, Brain and Creativity Institute, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 2Performance Science Institute, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States

Gratitude is a complex emotional feeling associated with universally desirable positive effects in personal, social, and physiological domains. Why or how gratitude achieves these functional outcomes is not clear. Toward the goal of identifying its' underlying physiological processes, we recently investigated the neural correlates of gratitude. In our study, participants were exposed to gratitude-inducing stimuli, and rated each according to how much gratitude it provoked. As expected, self-reported gratitude intensity correlated with brain activity in distinct regions of the medial pre-frontal cortex associated with social reward and moral cognition. Here we draw from our data and existing literature to offer a theoretical foundation for the physiological correlates of gratitude. We propose that mu-opioid signaling (1) accompanies the mental experience of gratitude, and (2) may account for the positive effects of gratitude on social relationships, subjective wellbeing, and physiological health.

1. Introduction

The intentional practice of gratitude brings our attention to the positive aspects of our lives, and empirical research indicates that this can lead to many powerful benefits (Emmons and McCullough, 2004). It is widely known that gratitude is beneficial, but a mechanistic understanding of why and how gratitude exerts these positive effects has remained elusive. A better understanding of the physiological processes underlying gratitude will offer a roadmap for the development of novel clinical interventions and personal strategies for living happier, healthier, more fulfilled lives, while also prompting us to rethink our approach to human interaction at all levels of society.

Definitions of gratitude typically describe a situational, social, and moral emotional feeling, a mediator of social cohesion, and a reinforcer of reciprocal prosocial behavior (McCullough et al., 2001). The most intense gratitude is provoked when we receive gifts or aid that fulfill a personal need, and especially when we understand that the aid was intentionally given for that purpose (Tesser et al., 1968; Lane and Anderson, 1976).

Previous studies have characterized significant positive effects of gratitude, but they have not closely examined the physiological mechanisms that underlie these effects. The purpose of the present discussion is to account for the positive effects of gratitude by identifying physiological mechanisms that are consistent with these effects, and whose involvement during the mental experience of gratitude is supported by behavioral and evolutionary considerations.

2. Neural Correlates of Gratitude

In 2015, we published a neuroimaging study that identified brain regions whose activation intensity correlated with the intensity of gratitude reported by our subjects. In designing our study, we turned to the USC Shoah Foundation's Visual History Archive, the world's largest repository of videotaped testimony from Holocaust survivors. A trend we observed among the testimonies was that survivors repeatedly reported receiving aid or gifts from strangers that often made the difference between life and death. The described “aid” ranged from sharing bread to being granted shelter by strangers at great personal risk during Nazi manhunts. We filtered the testimonies, flagging those that clearly described a benefactor, aid given, and a beneficiary, and transcribed the selected stories into second-person (e.g., “You are on the run from a Nazi manhunt; Your neighbors rescue you from the streets and help you into hiding for the winter, saving your life.”).

We created a database of 48 such stories from Holocaust survivors, describing receiving aid that ranged from relatively inconsequential to life-saving. Some aid met an acute need but did not require much effort on behalf of the benefactor (e.g., a cobbler giving away extra or damaged shoes) while other aid required tremendous benefactor effort, but was not useful to the beneficiary (e.g., being given peanuts that were procured at great risk but having an allergy that prevented eating them). Finally, some stories described life-saving aid that also put the benefactor in tremendous danger, such as sheltering a Jewish fugitive in ones' family home.

Research subjects were instructed to imagine themselves as the beneficiary in these stories, to feel the emotional context of the story as deeply as possible, and to imagine what it would feel like to receive the aid described. During this time, we collected functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) data, and after each story, participants were asked to rate how much gratitude they felt on a scale of 1–4. Their own ratings of gratitude were correlated in a general linear model with the BOLD signal activity during their reflection time. We hoped the results would identify core brain regions active during the mental experience of gratitude, and we predicted that these brain regions would be among those known to support moral reasoning and social cognition.

Indeed, a set of brain regions whose activity correlated with the intensity of reported gratitude was identified. These regions are located in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and include the peri-genual anterior cingulate cortex, and the more anterior portion of the mPFC. More generally, the event of receiving aid in our study elicited robust activity compared to baseline, implicating multiple overlapping social, stress-related, and somatosensory brain regions (O'Connell and Hofmann, 2011). This result aligned with our hypotheses, considering the regions identified have established roles in moral reasoning and social cognition (Zahn et al., 2008), and the involvement of these regions in gratitude has since been supported by a study from Kini et al. (2016).

Identifying neural correlates is an important step toward defining a physiological model of gratitude, and the results of our study have helped guide our thinking on the present topic. A lingering question from our study concerns what was being encoded by the brain activity we detected. One possibility is that because the brain activity was correlated with the intensity of self-reported gratitude for aid, the brain activity could represent the “value” of the aid to the beneficiary. In the context of our experiment, value for the beneficiary may be related to the reward or reduction in stress resulting from the aid received. This is relevant, considering plausible physiological correlates of gratitude must be consistent with the brain activity we identified, and must also be consistent with the well-characterized positive effects of gratitude on wellbeing and health.

3. The Varied Positive Effects of Gratitude

Gratitude quantifiably improves quality of life, with benefits that can be principally observed in social, personal, and health domains. Perhaps the most well-known effects of gratitude are improvement in interpersonal relationships, and increased prosocial behavior. Specifically, the reciprocal practice of gratitude has been shown to increase feelings of social inclusion and closeness in interpersonal relationships (Algoe et al., 2008, 2013), and individuals who feel grateful for a partner's help want to spend more time with them (Bartlett et al., 2011). Additionally, gratitude prompts major acts of reciprocal prosocial behavior (Ames et al., 2004) — an effect that has been attributed specifically to gratitude, and not to indebtedness or general positive affect alone (Bartlett and DeSteno, 2006). Indeed, the experimental induction of gratitude in subjects increases the likelihood of them “paying it forward” by sacrificing personal benefit to help others (Tsang, 2007). Finally, gratitude appears to be context-dependent, as the intensity of reported gratitude increases significantly when kindness exceeds our expectations (Wood et al., 2011). Hinting at physiological underpinnings, Sara Algoe's laboratory observed an increase in oxytocin release between romantic pair bonds after a gratitude intervention, although considering oxytocin has been ascribed a central role in mate selection and sexual attraction (Machin and Dunbar, 2011) the romantic nature of these relationships complicates the attribution of oxytocinergic processes to gratitude specifically (Algoe and Way, 2014).

KEY CONCEPT 1. Gratitude is beneficial for social relationships
Intentionally practiced or unexpectedly provoked, gratitude improves interpersonal relationships, increases feelings of social inclusion and closeness, predicts reciprocal prosocial behavior, and can reduce symptoms of mental illness.

In addition to its effects on social relationships and social behavior, gratitude has been shown to correlate with measures of subjective wellbeing and life-satisfaction. Teachers instructed to complete a gratitude intervention reported more meaning in their lives and lower levels of burnout (Chan, 2010), and another study identified gratitude as a better predictor of life-satisfaction than the big-five personality traits (Wood et al., 2008a). Corroborating this indirectly, the landmark Grant Study followed male Harvard sophomores for 70 years, searching for the life-history variables that predict life-satisfaction. The study concluded that warm social relationships throughout life have the greatest positive impact on life-satisfaction (Vaillant, 2012). Finally, in addition to the positive effects already mentioned, gratitude, and the close personal relationships it supports, have been associated with improved physiological health (Tops et al., 2013).

KEY CONCEPT 2. Gratitude is beneficial for health
Gratitude correlates with subjective wellbeing and improvements in physiological health. Specifically, gratitude is associated with increased life satisfaction, resiliency to health issues, and better sleep quality, in addition to lower levels of burnout, and reductions in stress, inflammation, and depression.

Studies of gratitude's effect on health report that subjects who score higher in assessments of dispositional propensity for gratitude are more resilient to health issues, cope better with heart failure, exhibit reduced levels of inflammatory biomarkers and depression, and also sleep better (Wood et al., 2009; Mills et al., 2015). These positive health effects appear to be mediated by gratitude specifically, as opposed to personality differences, as subjects participating in a gratitude-focused journaling condition reported fewer health problems and slept more hours per night (Emmons and McCullough, 2003).

It is apparent that developing a mindset of gratitude can significantly improve our quality of life, but an understanding of why and how these effects manifest requires a physiological model of gratitude. Unfortunately, the physiological correlates of gratitude have only been minimally studied in humans, which has limited our ability to understand the underlying mechanisms. However, certain stereotyped primate behaviors arise in social contexts and produce effects that are strikingly consistent with the social contexts and effects of gratitude observed in humans. For instance, it is clear that other species engage in reciprocal, cooperative, value-based behavioral exchanges, one example of which is social grooming, otherwise known as “allogrooming” (Emmons and McCullough, 2004). Primate allogrooming offers a compelling animal model of gratitude, and the physiological mechanisms underlying the effects of allogrooming in primates inform our search for the physiological mechanisms underlying the effects of gratitude in humans.

4. Allogrooming as an Animal Model of the Physiological Effects of Gratitude

Allogrooming represents the intersection of sociality, reciprocal prosocial behavior, and improved health. Almost all social vertebrates engage in some form of allogrooming, but the dynamics of the behavior vary considerably between species. For example, impalas have been observed to engage in a tightly regulated, highly reciprocal allogrooming routine in which they take turns grooming the head and neck regions of their grooming partner (Schneeberger, 2016). This allogrooming behavior is relatively utilitarian, and serves the clear function of removing ectoparasites from regions that, if you were an impala, would be difficult to reach on your own. This utilitarian allogrooming dynamic contrasts with the socially oriented primate variety.

As with impalas and other mammals, primate allogrooming serves a hygienic function, but primates spend up to 17% of their waking hours grooming each other, far more than is necessary for hygienic reasons alone (Dunbar, 1991; Machin and Dunbar, 2011). The principle effect of primate allogrooming appears to be the maintenance of harmonious societies (Russell and Phelps, 2013); primate social groups tend to be larger than those of other animals, and their social relationships tend to be more complex, diverse, and long lasting (Machin and Dunbar, 2011). Allogrooming settles in-group tensions, maintains social bonds, reinforces social hierarchies, and provokes reciprocal prosocial behavior in the form of reciprocal grooming, food sharing, or coalitionary support (de Waal, 1997; Akinyi et al., 2013; Jaeggi and Gurven, 2013; Lutermann et al., 2013; Borgeaud and Bshary, 2015; Garrido et al., 2016; Richard et al., 2016). The parallels between the effects of allogrooming and the effects of gratitude on social behavior are apparent, and a possible link between the social effects of allogrooming and physiology emerges from the observation that when an individual is being groomed, they visibly relax and their heart rate decreases significantly (Grandi and Ishida, 2015).

This physiological effect of allogrooming has been attributed to C-Tactile (C-T) afferent sensory nerves that are selectively activated by body-temperature, stroking touch (Grandi, 2016; Olausson et al., 2016). C-T fibers have been widely implicated in social touch in primates as well as humans, and their activation increases hedonic liking of a person or place, manifesting in increased trust, compliance, and prosocial behavior (Björnsdotter et al., 2010). It has been theorized that the functional role of C-T fibers has shifted throughout mammalian evolution, from initially being related to parental nurturing in sub-primate species, to promoting social bonding in the more socially active primates (Olausson et al., 2010).

The relaxing effect of C-T fiber activation has been attributed to the release of oxytocin and endogenous mu-opioid peptides (Olausson et al., 2008). Our understanding of the molecular mediators of social behavior has long centered on oxytocin and vasopressin, but an alternative account offers an updated view. The Brain Opioid Theory of Social Attachment (BOTSA) suggests that while the oxytocin/vasopressin system is central to mate selection, attraction, and the maintenance of sub-primate social bonds, the oxytocin/vasopressin system cannot account for the unique dynamics of primate social behavior (Machin and Dunbar, 2011). Instead, the BOTSA contends that the complex, long-term, affective social relationships observed in primates and humans are principally mediated by endogenous mu-opioid peptide molecules and the associated mu-opioid receptor (MOR) system (Machin and Dunbar, 2011).

The MOR system has previously been implicated in mediating social reward (Trezza et al., 2011), and motivation (Kringelbach and Berridge, 2015), but the BOTSA posits a more central role for the MOR system in primate social behavior. The BOTSA contends that while oxytocin, vasopressin, dopamine, and serotonin systems may be implicated in the onset of primate and human bonding, as well as in intense romantic relationships, the MOR system is likely to be principally involved in the maintenance of the stable, long term relationships characteristic of our species (Machin and Dunbar, 2011). This posited role for the MOR system is supported by research demonstrating that primate solitication of allogrooming behavior is largely mediated by MOR interactions.

An early study discovered that talapoin monkeys treated with naloxone (general opioid antagonist) exhibited much higher rates of allogrooming than controls (Meller et al., 1980), and other studies demonstrated that (1) receipt of allogrooming in previously socially isolated monkeys increases the levels of MOR agonists in the central nervous system (CNS), and (2) solicitations for grooming were increased and decreased by administration of naloxone and morphine (a MOR agonist), respectively (Keverne et al., 1989; Martel et al., 1995). This suggests that opioid blockade reduced feelings of social connection in the monkeys and thus provoked the solicitation of grooming, while MOR activation increased feelings of social connection. This pattern is supported by a recent study of human subjects, in which Inagaki et al. demonstrated, with an experimental condition designed to evoke feelings of social connection, that inhibition of opioid interactions by naltrexone (general opioid antagonist) reduced feelings of social connection compared to controls (Inagaki et al., 2016).

The social effects of allogrooming are consistent with the social effects of gratitude, and theoretical and empirical evidence implicates the MOR system in mediating these effects in both allogrooming and gratitude. Implication of the MOR system in the social effects of gratitude offers a physiological process that may account for these effects; and in fact, the same MOR system may also account for the physiological health benefits that have been associated with gratitude.

5. CNS mu-Opioids Play a Major Role in Physiological Stress-Reduction

In the context of biological systems, “health” effectively translates to “homeostasis.” “Homeostasis” describes a physiological state where the internal chemistries of the body are maintained within ranges conducive to relaxed and efficient continuation of the life process (Damasio and Carvalho, 2013). When deviations from these ranges occur, homeostasis deteriorates, and if left uncorrected, damage, disease, or death will result.

The “stress response” is a biochemical cascade provoked by homeostatic or environmental challenge that prepares the organism to confront an imminent threat to homeostasis. Acute stress has been shown to prepare an organism to fight or flee, but engaging a stress-response is energy intensive (Richard and Brian, 1989) and when the stress is not quickly resolved, homeostasis — and health — suffer. Indeed, chronic stress has been causally associated with deterioration in health and subjective wellbeing (Glaser and Kiecolt-Glaser, 2005; Rygula et al., 2005; Marin et al., 2011).

While the idea that the MOR system may be involved in the maintenance of long term social bonds is relatively new, their involvement in stress-reduction has been understood since their discovery in the 1970's. MOR agonists are released during conditions of physiological stress-relief (Curtis et al., 2001) as well as reward (Merrer et al., 2009) and they powerfully attenuate the stress-response (Wynne and Sarkar, 2013). Repeated stress reduction achieved through intentionally practicing gratitude could account for the positive health effects associated with gratitude.

Our 2015 study did not explicitly test whether physiological stress-reduction accompanied the mental experience of gratitude, however, some of the most intense gratitude reported by our subjects was (unsurprisingly) provoked by stories in which a stranger saved the protagonists' life at great personal risk. This alone is not sufficient grounds to claim involvement of the stress-reducing MOR system, but it is reasonable to expect that recognizing one's life is in danger produces stress; and it is similarly reasonable to expect that realizing that a stranger has gone out of their way to save your life would replace this stress with an intense feeling of gratitude.

Of course, not all gratitude-provoking situations involve stress-relief, but previous studies have noted that subjects report decreased levels of stress following the experimental induction of gratitude (Krause, 2006; Wood et al., 2008b, 2010). This indicates that even in gratitude-provoking conditions that do not involve stress-relief, MOR system activation appears likely, as subjective reports of decreased stress following the experimental induction of gratitude are consistent with established effects of the MOR system.

KEY CONCEPT 3. Mu-opioids and stress-relief may account for the positive effects of gratitude
The mu-opioid system is centrally involved in the restoration or improvement of homeostasis, as evidenced by its roles in analgesia, positive affect and reward, social motivation, long term affective bonds, and relaxation from stress. Meanwhile, the experience of gratitude is characterized by positive affect, reciprocal prosocial motivation, long term affective bonds, and subjective reports of stress reduction. Qualitative and behavioral parallels between the experience of gratitude and the effects of the mu-opioid system suggest mu-opioid involvement in the experience of gratitude, which could offer a physiological account of many of the positive effects of gratitude. Future studies are necessary to test this hypothesis.

6. Conclusions

The purpose of the current article has been to account for the positive effects of gratitude by exploring its underlying physiological correlates. To this end, we propose that large-scale activation of the MOR system during the mental experience of gratitude may account for the social, individual, and health effects of gratitude that have been observed. Specifically, we propose that the social effects of gratitude may be mediated largely by the MOR system in accordance with the Brain Opioid Theory of Social Attachment (Machin and Dunbar, 2011); and we propose that gratitude improves health by (1) reducing physiological stress, and (2) producing reward – both personal and social – in its place via the same MOR system. The effects of the MOR system on social relationships, individual wellbeing, and physiological health are well-supported, and consistent with the positive effects of gratitude we have described.

MORs are likely involved in all positive emotional experiences, and so implicating the MOR system as a principle mediator of the positive effects of gratitude could be taken to suggest that these positive effects may not be unique to gratitude. While it is likely that all positive mental experiences exert some degree of positive effects (see Barbara Frederikson's Broaden and Build Theory of Positive Emotions Fredrickson, 2004), these effects are likely to be magnified in gratitude. The feeling of gratitude integrates social and personal reward, and this combination likely produces a positive feedback loop that amplifies the positive effects beyond what could be achieved from a non-social rewarding experience.

We have made a case that activation of the MOR system may be a physiological correlate of gratitude, and may underlie its positive effects, but it is unlikely that opioidergic mechanisms act alone. Additional research will be necessary to tease apart the relative contributions made by the oxytocin, vasopressin, serotonin, dopamine, and other systems in producing the effects of this powerful social emotional feeling.

7. Implications

Physiological signals can modify qualitative aspects of mental experience, and mental experience can modify physiological state. In this frame, feelings can be understood as means to access body state, allowing the intentional practice of gratitude to reduce physiological stress and improve our health as part of the broader concert of homeostatic processes. And vice versa, the body can be a means to access the mind, allowing the direct reduction of physiological stress through massage or social touch to reduce mental stress, improving our emotional wellbeing. Elaboration of this research will help us further understand the systematic relationship between mind and body, informing the creation of novel strategies to live happier, more fulfilling, and healthier lives.

KEY CONCEPT 4. Gratitude is best understood in a homeostatic context
We propose that it is helpful to think of gratitude, and emotions more broadly, in a homeostatic context. Since gratitude is a positive emotion, it is reasonable to expect the conditions surrounding the experience of gratitude to be beneficial for homeostasis, while the opposite should be true for negative emotions. The recognition that subjective emotional experience is rooted in the homeostasis of our organism (1) facilitates basic research investigations of the physiological correlates of emotion, and (2) suggests that intentional mindfulness of our emotional state may improve behavioral decision-making.

By definition, the power of gratitude extends beyond the individual. A primary function of primate allogrooming is settling tensions and improving social group cohesion, and in human societies a similar effect is likely wrought through moral codes, religious systems, and philosophical appeals to humanity that advocate the intentional practice of gratitude. We nod to Seneca in contending that an empirically-driven resurgence of the intentional practice of gratitude could have significant positive implications for the harmony of the human civilization, from interpersonal relationships to international diplomacy.

Author Contributions

MH outlined and wrote the manuscript. GF wrote and edited the manuscript. JK, HD, and AD each provided writing, feedback and editing.

Funding

This study was supported by the Oskar Schindler Humanities Foundation, the Shoah Foundation Institute, and the Greater Good Science Center.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank our colleagues Ariana Shives and Griffin Molly for providing feedback on this manuscript.

Author Biography

yes Glenn R. Fox earned his Ph.D. in Neuroscience from the University of Southern California's Neuroscience Graduate Program in 2014, where he was mentored by Hanna and Antonio Damasio. His thesis work focused on elucidating the physiological mechanisms of the experience of gratitude using heart rate and brain imaging techniques. He held a post-doctoral fellowship with Antonio Damasio from 2015 to 2016, during which he published one of the first studies of the neural correlates of gratitude. In 2016, he became head of the USC Performance Science Institute. His current research is focused on how emotion and mindset influence performance in occupational, developmental, and social domains.

References

Akinyi, M., Tung, J., Jeneby, M., Patel, N., and Altmann, J. (2013). Role of grooming in reducing tick load in wild baboons (Papio cynocephalus). Anim. Behav. 85, 559–568. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Algoe, S., Haidt, J., and Gable, S. (2008). Beyond reciprocity: gratitude and relationships in everyday life. Emotion 8, 425–429. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Algoe, S., and Way, B. (2014). Evidence for a role of the oxytocin system, indexed by genetic variation in cd38 in the social bonding effects of expressed gratitude. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1855–1861. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Algoe, S. B., Fredrickson, B. L., and Gable, S. L. (2013). The social functions of the emotion of gratitude via expression. Emotion 13, 605–609. doi: 10.1037/a0032701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ames, D., Flynn, F., and Weber, E. (2004). It's the thought that counts: on perceiving how helpers decide to lend a hand. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30, 461–474. doi: 10.1177/0146167203261890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartlett, M., Condon, P., Cruz, J., Baumann, J., and Desteno, D. (2011). Gratitude: Prompting behaviours that build relationships. Cogn. Emot. 26, 1–12. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.561297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartlett, M., and DeSteno, D. (2006). Gratitude and prosocial behavior: helping when it costs you. Psychol. Sci. 17, 319–325. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01705.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Björnsdotter, M., Morrison, I., and Olausson, H. (2010). Feeling good: on the role of c fiber mediated touch in interoception. Exp. Brain Res. 207, 149–155. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2408-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borgeaud, C., and Bshary, R. (2015). Wild vervet monkeys trade tolerance and specific coalitionary support for grooming in experimentally induced conflicts. Curr. Biol. 25, 3011–3016. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, D. W. (2010). Gratitude, gratitude intervention and subjective well-being among chinese school teachers in hong kong. Educ. Psychol. 30, 139–153. doi: 10.1080/01443410903493934

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curtis, A., Bello, N., and Valentino, R. (2001). Evidence for functional release of endogenous opioids in the locus ceruleus during stress termination. J. Neurosci. 21:RC152.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Damasio, A., and Carvalho, G. B. (2013). The nature of feelings: evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 143–152. doi: 10.1038/nrn3403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Waal, F. (1997). The chimpanzee's service economy: food for grooming. Evol. Human Behav. 18, 375–386. doi: 10.1016/S1090-5138(97)00085-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunbar, R. (1991). Functional significance of social grooming in primates. Folia Primatol. 57, 1991.

Google Scholar

Emmons, R., and McCullough, M. (2003). Counting blessings versus burdens: an experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 377–389. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emmons, R., and McCullough, M. (2004). The Psychology of Gratitude. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. (2004). The broaden–and–build theory of positive emotions. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 359, 1367–1377. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrido, M., Adler, V., Pnini, M., Abramsky, Z., Krasnov, B. R., Gutman, R., et al. (2016). Time budget, oxygen consumption and body mass responses to parasites in juvenile and adult wild rodents. Parasite Vector 9, 1–14. doi: 10.1186/s13071-016-1407-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glaser, R., and Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (2005). Stress-induced immune dysfunction: implications for health. Nat. Rev. Immunol. 5, 243–251. doi: 10.1038/nri1571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandi, L. (2016). From sweeping to the caress: Similarities and discrepancies between human and Non-Human primates' pleasant touch. Front. Psychol. 7:1371. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grandi, L., and Ishida, H. (2015). The physiological effect of human grooming on the heart rate and the heart rate variability of laboratory Non-Human primates: A pilot study in male rhesus monkeys. Front. Vet. Sci. 2:50. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2015.00050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inagaki, T., Ray, L., Irwin, M., Way, B., and Eisenberger, N. (2016). Opioids and social bonding: naltrexone reduces feelings of social connection. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neursci. 11, 728–735. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaeggi, A. V., and Gurven, M. (2013). Reciprocity explains food sharing in humans and other primates independent of kin selection and tolerated scrounging: a phylogenetic meta-analysis. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 280:20131615. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.1615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keverne, E. B., Martensz, N. D., and Tuite, B. (1989). Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid of monkeys are influenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrinology 14, 155–161. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(89)90065-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kini, P., Wong, J., McInnis, S., Gabana, N., and Brown, J. W. (2016). The effects of gratitude expression on neural activity. NeuroImage 128, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.12.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krause, N. (2006). Gratitude toward god, stress, and health in late life. Res. Aging 28, 163–183. doi: 10.1177/0164027505284048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kringelbach, M. L., and Berridge, K. C. (2015). Motivation and Pleasure in the Brain. New York, NY: The Psychology of Desire.

Google Scholar

Lane, J., and Anderson, N. (1976). Integration of intention and outcome in moral judgment. Mem. Cogn. 4, 1–5. doi: 10.3758/BF03213247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutermann, H., Bennett, N., Speakman, J., and Scantlebury, M. (2013). Energetic benefits of sociality offset the costs of parasitism in a cooperative mammal. PLoS ONE 8:e57969. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Machin, A., and Dunbar, R. (2011). The brain opioid theory of social attachment: a review of the evidence. Behaviour 148, 985–1025. doi: 10.1163/000579511X596624

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marin, M.-F., Lord, C., Andrews, J., Juster, R.-P., Sindi, S., Arsenault-Lapierre, G., et al. (2011). Chronic stress, cognitive functioning and mental health. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 96, 583–595. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martel, F. L., Nevison, C. M., Simpson, M. J., and Keverne, E. B. (1995). Effects of opioid receptor blockade on the social behavior of rhesus monkeys living in large family groups. Dev. Psychobiol. 28, 71–84. doi: 10.1002/dev.420280202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCullough, M., Kilpatrick, S., Emmons, R., and Larson, D. (2001). Is gratitude a moral affect? Psychol. Bull. 127, 249–266. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meller, R. E., Keverne, E., and Herbert, J. (1980). Behavioural and endocrine effects of naltrexone in male talapoin monkeys. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 13, 663–672. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(80)90010-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merrer, J., Becker, J., Befort, K., and Kieffer, B. (2009). Reward processing by the opioid system in the brain. Physiol. Rev. 89, 1379–1412. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00005.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mills, P. J., Redwine, L., Wilson, K., Pung, M. A., Chinh, K., Greenberg, B. H., et al. (2015). The role of gratitude in spiritual well-being in asymptomatic heart failure patients. Spirituality Clin. Pract. 2, 5. doi: 10.1037/scp0000050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Connell, L. A., and Hofmann, H. A. (2011). The vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: a comparative synthesis. J. Comp. Neurol. 519, 3599–3639. doi: 10.1002/cne.22735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olausson, H., Cole, J., Rylander, K., McGlone, F., Lamarre, Y., Wallin, B. G., et al. (2008). Functional role of unmyelinated tactile afferents in human hairy skin: sympathetic response and perceptual localization. Exp. Brain Res. 184, 135–140. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1175-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olausson, H., Wessberg, J., and McGlone, F. (2016). Affective Touch and the Neurophysiology of CT Afferents. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Olausson, H., Wessberg, J., McGlone, F., and Vallbo, Å. (2010). The neurophysiology of unmyelinated tactile afferents. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 185–191. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richard, K., and Brian, B. (1989). Towards a physiological and genetical understanding of the energetics of the stress response. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 37, 157–171. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1989.tb02100.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richard, M., Henzi, P. S., Barrett, L., Wanigaratne, A., Coetzee, E., Fuller, A., et al. (2016). Thermal consequences of increased pelt loft infer an additional utilitarian function for grooming. Am. J. Primatol. 78, 456–461. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22519

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, Y., and Phelps, S. (2013). How do you measure pleasure? a discussion about intrinsic costs and benefits in primate allogrooming. Biol. Philos. 28:1005. doi: 10.1007/s10539-013-9372-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rygula, R., Abumaria, N., Flügge, G., Fuchs, E., Rüther, E., and Ursula, H. (2005). Anhedonia and motivational deficits in rats: Impact of chronic social stress. Behav. Brain Res. 162, 127–134. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneeberger, K. (2016). “Cooperative grooming,” in Cooperative grooming, eds T. K. Shackelford and V. A. Weekes-Shackelford (New York, NY: Springer International Publishing). doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3058-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tesser, A., Gatewood, R., and Driver, M. (1968). Some determinants of gratitude. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 9, 233–236. doi: 10.1037/h0025905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tops, M., Koole, S., IJzerman, H., and Buisman-Pijlman, F. (2013). Why social attachment and oxytocin protect against addiction and stress: Insights from the dynamics between ventral and dorsal corticostriatal systems. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 119, 39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2013.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trezza, V., Damsteegt, R., Achterberg, E., and Vanderschuren, L. (2011). Nucleus accumbens mu-opioid receptors mediate social reward. J. Neurosci. 31, 6362–6370. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5492-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsang, J. (2007). Gratitude for small and large favors: a behavioral test. J. Posit. Psychol. 2–3, 157–167. doi: 10.1080/17439760701229019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaillant, G. (2012). Triumphs of Experience: The Men of the Harvard Grant Study. Chichester, UK: Belknap Press; An Imprint of Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Wood, A., Brown, G., and Maltby, J. (2011). Thanks, but i'm used to better: a relative rank model of gratitude. Emotion 11, 175–180. doi: 10.1037/a0021553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, A., Froh, J., and Geraghty, A. (2010). Gratitude and well-being: a review and theoretical integration. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 890–905. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, A., Joseph, S., Lloyd, J., and Atkins, S. (2009). Gratitude influences sleep through the mechanism of pre-sleep cognitions. J. Psychosom. Res. 66, 43–48. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, A., Joseph, S., and Maltby, J. (2008a). Gratitude uniquely predicts satisfaction with life: Incremental validity above the domains and facets of the five factor model. Pers. Individ. Dif. 45, 49–54. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.02.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wood, A., Maltby, J., Stewart, N., Linley, P., and Joseph, S. (2008b). A social-cognitive model of trait and state levels of gratitude. Emotion 8, 281–290. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wynne, O., and Sarkar, D. (2013). Stress and Neuroendocrine–Immune Interaction: A Therapeutic Role for Beta-endorphin. New York, NY: The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Psychoneuroimmunology.

Google Scholar

Zahn, R., Moll, J., Paiva, M., Garrido, G., Krueger, F., Huey, E., et al. (2008). The neural basis of human social values: evidence from functional mri. Cereb. Cortex 9, 276–283. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: gratitude, fMRI, stress-relief, social support, mu-opioid, health

Citation: Henning M, Fox GR, Kaplan J, Damasio H and Damasio A (2017) A Potential Role for mu-Opioids in Mediating the Positive Effects of Gratitude. Front. Psychol. 8:868. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00868

Received: 15 November 2016; Accepted: 11 May 2017;
Published: 21 June 2017.

Edited by:

Marie-Helene Grosbras, Aix-Marseille University, France

Reviewed by:

Monica Bartlett, Gonzaga University, United States
Christina M. Karns, University of Oregon, United States

Copyright © 2017 Henning, Fox, Kaplan, Damasio and Damasio. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: glennfox@marshall.usc.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.