Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 11 July 2017
Sec. Consciousness Research

Commentary: Spontaneous expression of mirror self-recognition in monkeys after learning precise visual-proprioceptive association for mirror images

  • 1School of Psychology, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing, China
  • 2College of Life Science and Technology, Jinan University, Guangzhou, China

A commentary on
Spontaneous expression of mirror self-recognition in monkeys after learning precise visual-proprioceptive association for mirror images

by Chang, L., Zhang, S., Poo, M. M., and Gong, N. (2017). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 3258–3263. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620764114

Self-awareness is believed to be limited in human and great apes (Heschl and Burkart, 2006). Attempting to touch the face mark before a mirror is regarded as the evidence of self-recognition, namely the mirror self-recognition (MSR) test (Anderson and Gallup, 2015). The MSR, as an index of self-recognition, indicates the ability for one separates himself from other people, thoughts and environments (Ma et al., 2015). In Gallop's original experiment, when red odorless dye spots were applied to chimpanzees' faces, chimpanzees would touch face marks as well as do self-directed behaviors before a mirror, for instance, performing exaggerated expressions and exploring invisible body parts like nose, eyes or genitals. Does it mean rhesus monkeys lacking for self-awareness when they could not pass the MSR test consistently? It is questioned by scientists that passing the standard test only can prove the existence of MSR positively, while it's hard to conclude the absence of self-recognition if the animal failed, thus modified precise MSR tests were required to eliminate the limitation (Heschl and Burkart, 2006).

In 2015, a pioneer study employed the visual-somatosensory training to facilitate rhesus monkeys to comprehend that the mirror image was itself (Chang et al., 2015). With high-power laser red spots were projected to face, the rhesus monkey, who sit on the mirror-facing head-fixed chair, was able to touch the laser spots successfully according to the somatosensory feeling, which was further paired with food reward to reinforce the learning process. After 12–38 days' training, the touching rate was up to 95%. The monkey maintained this behavior even in absence of laser induced hot feeling, or food reward. The study was the first success to provide direct evidence that rhesus monkey can pass the MSR after training.

In their newest study, the same group of authors improved the visual-proprioceptive association training to motivate monkey to locate the marked spot spontaneously by the instrumental mirror use. In the training, the monkey was in the head-fixation chair when facing the mirror. A low power laser spot was projected to flat boards, and if the monkey touched the spot precisely via the mirror image, food reward was delivered. After 2–4 weeks' training, successful rate was up to 100%, and persisted without reward. Even the light spot was substituted by colored dye mark, the trained monkey still exhibited mark touch in front of the mirror.

To explore that the trained monkey exactly recognized itself in a mirror instead of simply learning the rule of touching marks, the glass/mirror switching experiment was designed, in which a naïve monkey and a trained monkey were released to a same home cage and separated by glass or a mirror. In the glass period, the social interaction was exhibited between both monkeys, but in the mirror-facing time, touching response was only observed in trained monkeys. Furthermore, trained monkeys behaved more mirror-induced genital-related (GR) and genital-unrelated (UR) behaviors than naïve monkeys when facing the mirror wall. Data collected supported the hypothesis: trained rhesus monkeys are capable to recognize themselves via mirror image.

In the visual-somatosensory training, heat-induced high-power spots were projected to monkeys' face, which is controversial that after training, the mark touching response in monkey is a conditioned behavior not spontaneous MSR.

The improved study perfected training devices to help monkeys precisely learn the association between the face mark position and the image by simply using a mirror. Then, the new study approved the transition from the mirror use to mirror-induced self-recognition performance in the trained monkey and demonstrated the ability to use a mirror and MSR are distinguishing cognitive abilities.

Previous studies suggested that monkey species could not exhibit self-recognition behaviors and failed the MSR test (Roma et al., 2007; Macellini et al., 2010). The new methodology in the present study would therefore inspire more studies to explore self-recognition in primates and even non-primates (Salzen and Cornell, 1968; Berry and Parker, 2016; Deregnaucourt and Bovet, 2016). Otherwise, the new strategy is probable to remedy self-recognition disorder in schizophrenia (SZ) via specific training. The cognitive process of self-recognition is disrupted in schizophrenia, resulting in patients fail to distinguish their own movements and thoughts from those of others (Waters et al., 2012). Although cognitive and neurological deficits happened to in SZ patients, this finding might restore self-recognition ability partially (Gallup et al., 2014).

The underlying mechanism of MSR is unknown. Along with human, chimpanzees are inherent of MSR without training, but monkeys not, which possibly means enhance sensory integration to a certain degree that required for MSR. It's possible that mirror neurons, which are discovered in the monkeys and considered the neural mechanism of imitation behaviors, are the reason of generating self-consciousness in rhesus monkey (Chang et al., 2017). In addition, related studies have found the neural basis of MSR involves frontal regions or the right hemisphere in human. It will be helpful to further elucidate mirror related cortical regions of MSR and neural circuit changes with fMRI after training protocol (Sugiura et al., 2015), for instance, to understand the acquisition and expression of “learned” self-awareness.

There are still some limitations in the present study. Tooling use is an evident indicator of high intelligence in human and high-rank primate and can be transferred within the group (Bayart and Anderson, 1985; Mercader et al., 2007). It will be interesting to see if this “learned” MSR task can be transferred as well. In addition, how early or how late is this learning ability maintained in a monkey's life? When the transition happened in a monkey from using a mirror instrumentally to spontaneous self-directed behaviors in a mirror? Is this due to the involvement of one specific brain region, probably the key region of self-consciousness? Could the monkey own other types of consciousness other than self-awareness? There will be more than one mirror to learn.

Author Contributions

XL and TL completed the first version of the paper and XH presented suggestions to revise the paper. The workload of the three authors was equal.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank their department for supports.

References

Anderson, J. R., Gallup, G. G. Jr. (2015). Mirror self-recognition: a review and critique of attempts to promote and engineer self-recognition in primates. Primates 56, 317–326. doi: 10.1007/s10329-015-0488-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayart, F., and Anderson, J. R. (1985). Mirror-image reactions in a tool-using, adult male Macaca tonkeana. Behav. Processes 10, 219–227. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(85)90069-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, J. A., and Parker, A. C. (2016). The Elephant in the Mirror: Bridging the Brain's Explanatory Gap of Consciousness. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 10:108. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2016.00108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, L., Fang, Q., Zhang, S., Poo, M. M., and Gong, N. (2015). Mirror-induced self-directed behaviors in rhesus monkeys after visual-somatosensory training. Curr. Biol. 25, 212–217. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, L., Zhang, S., Poo, M. M., and Gong, N. (2017). Spontaneous expression of mirror self-recognition in monkeys after learning precise visual-proprioceptive association for mirror images. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 3258–3263. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620764114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deregnaucourt, S., and Bovet, D. (2016). The perception of self in birds. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 69, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallup, G. G., Platek, S. M., and Spaulding, N. K. (2014). The nature of visual self-recognition revisited. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18:57. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heschl, A., and Burkart, J. (2006). A new mark test for mirror self-recognition in non-human primates. Primates 47, 187–198. doi: 10.1007/s10329-005-0170-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, X. Z., Jin, Y., Luo, B., Zhang, G. Q., Wei, R. P., and Liu, D. Z. (2015). Giant pandas failed to show mirror self-recognition. Anim. Cogn. 18, 713–721. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0838-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macellini, S., Ferrari, P. F., Bonini, L., Fogassi, L., and Paukner, A. (2010). A modified mark test for own-body recognition in pig-tailed macaques (Macaca nemestrina). Anim. Cogn. 13, 631–639. doi: 10.1007/s10071-010-0313-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercader, J., Barton, H., Gillespie, J., Harris, J., Kuhn, S., Tyler, R., et al. (2007). 4,300-year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 3043–3048. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0607909104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roma, P., Silberberg, A., Huntsberry, M., Ruggier, A., and Suomi, S. (2007). Mark tests for mirror self-recognition in Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) trained to touch marks. Am. J. Primatol. 69:989. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salzen, E. A., and Cornell, J. M. (1968). Self-perception and species recognition in birds. Behaviour 30, 44–65. doi: 10.1163/156853968X00171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sugiura, M., Miyauchi, C. M., Kotozaki, Y., Akimoto, Y., Nozawa, T., Yomogida, Y., et al. (2015). Neural Mechanism for Mirrored Self-face Recognition. Cereb. Cortex 25, 2806–2814. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waters, F., Woodward, T., Allen, P., Aleman, A., and Sommer, I. (2012). Self-recognition Deficits in Schizophrenia Patients With Auditory Hallucinations: a Meta-analysis of the Literature. Schizophr. Bull. 38, 741–750. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mirror self-recognition, rhesus monkey, mirror mark test, mirror neurons, tooling use

Citation: Liu X, Liu T and Huang X (2017) Commentary: Spontaneous expression of mirror self-recognition in monkeys after learning precise visual-proprioceptive association for mirror images. Front. Psychol. 8:1151. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01151

Received: 28 March 2017; Accepted: 23 June 2017;
Published: 11 July 2017.

Edited by:

Adam B. Barrett, University of Sussex, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Neng Gong, Institute of Neuroscience, Shanghai Institutes for Biological Sciences (CAS), China

Copyright © 2017 Liu, Liu and Huang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xiaodan Huang, 1206207800@qq.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.