Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 24 July 2017
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement
This article is part of the Research Topic Systematic Observation: Engaging Researchers in the Study of Daily Life as It Is Lived View all 24 articles

Observation of Interactions in Adolescent Group Therapy: A Mixed Methods Study

\r\nEullia Arias-Pujol*Eulàlia Arias-Pujol1*M. Teresa AngueraM. Teresa Anguera2
  • 1FPCEE Blanquerna, Ramon Llull University, Barcelona, Spain
  • 2Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain

Group psychotherapy is a useful clinical practice for adolescents with mental health issues. Groups typically consist of young people of similar ages but with different personalities, and this results in a complex communication network. The goal of group psychoanalytic psychotherapy is to improve participants' mentalization abilities, facilitating interactions between peers and their therapist in a safe, containing environment. The main aim of this study was to analyze conversation turn-taking between a lead therapist, a co-therapist, and six adolescents over the course of 24 treatment sessions divided into four blocks over 8 months. We employed a mixed-methods design based on systematic observation, which we consider to be a mixed method itself, as the qualitative data collected in the initial observation phase is transformed into quantitative data and subsequently interpreted qualitatively with the aid of clinical vignettes. The observational methodology design was nomothetic, follow-up, and multidimensional. The choice of methodology is justified as we used an ad-hoc observation instrument combining a field format and a category system. Interobserver agreement was analyzed quantitatively by Cohen's kappa using the free QSEQ5 software program. Once we had confirmed the reliability of the data, these were analyzed by polar coordinate analysis, which is a powerful data reduction technique that provides a vector representation of relationships between categories. The results show significant relationships between the therapist and (1) the activation of turn-taking by the participants and the co-therapist and silence and (2) conversation-facilitating interventions and interventions designed to improve mentalization abilities. Detailed analysis of questions demonstrating interest in others showed how the communication changed from radial interactions stemming from the therapist at the beginning of therapy to circular interactions half way through. Repetition was found to be a powerful conversation facilitator. The results also illustrate the role of the therapist, who (1) did not facilitate interventions by all participants equally, (2) encouraged turn-taking from more inhibited members of the group, (3) stimulated conversation from the early stages of therapy, and (4) favored mentalization toward the end. Despite its complexity, polar coordinate analysis produces easy-to-interpret results in the form of vector maps.

Introduction

Peer groups are a natural setting for young people (Erikson, 1968). In the social context, Malekoff (2014) and Tellerman (2001) consider group work to be a protective factor for teenagers, pre-teenagers, and their families. In the field of public health, group psychotherapy is a useful clinical practice for adolescents with varying mental health issues (Reid and Kolvin, 1993; Cramer-Azima, 2002). Adolescent mental health disorders have increased over the last three decades (Nuffield Foundation, 2013) and today's teenagers have higher rates of anxiety, behavioral problems, and mood disorders (Merikangas et al., 2010).

Little has been published on group therapy in children or adolescents. Most of the studies conducted to date have reported on brief cognitive-behavioral interventions with specified diagnostic populations (Pollock and Kymissis, 2001). There has also been research into group counseling and psychotherapy with children and adolescents that indicates that the peer feedback that occurs in such settings is a key part of the process of change (Shechtman, 2007). The theoretical orientation behind this study was a combination of interpersonal and psychodynamic theories. Pingitore (2016) validated the benefits of interpersonal group therapy, an approach originally proposed by Yalom (2005), by quantitatively analyzing audio recordings of interventions by eight adolescents who took part in a process-oriented psychotherapy group for 3 months. Within a Kleinian psychoanalytic framework and following the contributions of Devi and Fenn (2012) published a systematic thematic analysis of a latency-aged children's group. Through clinical extracts, they showed how the children shifted from paranoid-schizoid functioning to depressive functioning over the course of therapy. They concluded that psychotherapy was beneficial in latency-aged children, as it provided them with the opportunity to observe and try to attach meaning to the interactions of other people, to respond to these interactions, to initiate contact and to help and be helped in a safe environment. Such experiences improve individuals' ability to recognize and observe mental states in both themselves and others and to develop empathy.

More research has been conducted in adults. A recent meta-analysis of group psychotherapy for social anxiety disorders concluded that group interventions were as effective as individual psychotherapy or pharmacotherapy (Barkowski et al., 2016). Group therapy is also beneficial for adults with moderate or severe depression (Pylvänäinen et al., 2015) or eating disorders (Simpson et al., 2010) and it has been shown to reduce symptoms of anxiety, depression, and avoidance in adults with personality disorders (Skewes et al., 2015). Schwartze et al. (2016) recently published a meta-analysis that showed that cognitive behavioral therapy was effective for patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Another randomized controlled study that compared the outcomes of short- and long-term psychodynamic psychotherapy (90-min weekly sessions for 20 or 80 weeks) in 167 adult outpatients with mood, anxiety, and personality disorders found that patients in both groups made significant gains, and concluded that short- and long-term therapy seemed equally effective for typical outpatients seeking group psychotherapy, with the exception of symptomatic distress, for which a more favorable treatment effect was found for the long-term therapy (Lorentzen et al., 2013). A recent open prospective controlled study showed the efficacy of short-term dynamic group psychotherapy (37–39 sessions lasting 75 min over 9 months) in primary care patients with depressive symptoms (Bros et al., 2016).

In pyschotherapy research, there is growing concern for integrating qualitative methods, which provide a more holistic view of the person, and quantitative methods, which seek to provide a more objective view (Lutz and Hill, 2009). Despite the dearth of publications in the last decade, there are encouraging signs of a growing interest in the use of mixed-methods research in psychology (Roberts and Povee, 2014). By integrating complementary perspectives derived from quantitative and qualitative methods and analyses, mixed-methods research offers both rigor and flexibility and is likely to see an increase in future years (Anguera and Hernández-Mendo, 2016).

In this article, we describe the results of a study based on systematic observation, which we consider to be a mixed method in itself (Anguera and Hernández-Mendo, 2016). The study consisted of systematically observing video-recordings of adolescent group therapy sessions over a period of several months. The observation produced a large set of qualitative conversational data, subsequently analyzed quantitatively via polar coordinate analysis to detect changes in behaviors over the course of therapy.

The aim of the group therapy analyzed was to promote autonomy and maturity through interactions between peers and their therapist in a safe, containing environment (Torras de Beà, 2013). Group sessions of this type produce complex communication networks. Participants are typically young people of similar ages with different personalities who have difficulty relating to others and often perform poorly at school.

Psychodynamic interventions have been described as “conversation therapies,” as the relationship between the person seeking treatment and the therapist forms the basis of the therapy (Malmberg and Fenton, 2008). We studied group communication as a conversation in which we analyzed turn-taking (who) and content (what).

Foulkes (1986) described two roles for group analysis leaders, or conductors: a role as dynamic-administrator and a role as analyst-interpreter. The function of the first is to set up the group, establish norms and boundaries, and create a safe, supportive, and containing environment designed to increase participation, expressiveness, and interaction and communication. The function of the second, by contrast, is related to mental activity, and consists of observation, listening, and understanding, and the ability to put into words everything they are understanding.

In the group studied, interventions by a therapist largely seek to (a) facilitate conversation and (b) promote mentalization, i.e., stimulate thought, reflection, and understanding about oneself and one's relationships with others.

In the ad-hoc observation instrument used in the study, we labeled this first group of interventions DYN, as they have a dynamic, stimulating function. They are interventions in the form os a request or question in which the emitter (the therapist or participants) show interest in the life of the receiver. Demonstrating interest in others by asking questions, allowing them to intervene, and showing curiosity in their answers is considered to be a specific benefit of group therapy as opposed to individual therapy (Yalom, 2005; Torras de Beà, 2013). In previous studies, we saw that DYN interventions were very common in all sessions and that over the course of therapy, their use increased among participants and decreased among therapists, forming significant behavioral patterns (Arias and Anguera, 2004, 2005; Arias, 2011).

The second group of interventions in the observation instrument was called MNT to reflect the concept of mentalization described by Fonagy et al. (Fonagy, 1991; Fonagy et al., 1995), which is understood as the ability to explain and give meaning to one's own behaviors and those of others within a process of mental representation, thoughts, desires, and expectations. This ability is not innate: it needs to be developed within a safe, affective environment, which in psychoanalytic group psychotherapy is achieved by maintaining a stable internal and external setting while containing anxieties. MNT interventions are part of the therapist's role (Bateman and Fonagy, 2012), while DYN interventions correspond to either the therapist or the participants over the course of the sessions.

At the beginning of these group sessions, communication is generally radial, i.e., it diverges outwards toward the participants from the formal leader of the group, the therapist. With time, it becomes circular, with participants spontaneously intervening and demonstrating interest in each other. This shift in the direction of communication is an indicator of the group process, and our aim was to objectively analyze this process by studying the therapist's interventions.

The main aim of this study was to apply polar coordinate analysis to analyze conversation turn-taking and DYN and MNT interventions in a group therapy program involving a lead therapist, a co-therapist, and six adolescents. The program consisted of 24 group sessions, divided into four blocks, held over a period of 8 months.

Materials and Methods

Design

In this mixed-methods study, we applied systematic observation, which meets the rigorous standards of scientific inquiry while at the same time offers the flexibility needed in real-life settings. Observational methodology permits the capture of spontaneous behaviors as they occur in a natural environment (Sackett, 1978; Anguera, 1979, 2003; Bakeman and Quera, 1995b, 2011; Portell et al., 2015a,b). It is thus an ideal methodology for studying communication in group therapy, and has proven to be suitable for studying the changes that occur over the course of therapy (Pascual-Leone et al., 2009).

There are eight possible study designs in observational methodology (Blanco-Villaseñor et al., 2003; Sánchez-Algarra and Anguera, 2013). The design used in this study was N/F/M (nomothetic/follow-up/multidimensional). It was nomothetic because we conducted a parallel analysis of the therapist, the co-therapist, and six adolecents, follow-up because we performed both intersessional analyses (24 successive sessions) and intrasessional analyses (sequential recording of all behaviors from the start to finish of each session), and multidimensional because the ad-hoc observation instrument contained various dimensions selected on the basis of the theoretical framework and our experience.

The systematic observation was non-participative and the behaviors were highly perceivable.

Participants

There were eight participants: the therapist (T), the co-therapist (coT), and six adolescents (G, D, JM, F, L, M). The adolescents (four boys and two girls) had requested support at the Center for Child and Adolescent Mental Health of the Eulàlia Torras de Beà Foundation in Barcelona, Spain. They all had difficulties relating to others and difficulties learning at school; they had normal or normal-low intelligence according to the Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fourth Edition (WISC-IV, Weschler, 2006). Two had a mild behavioral disorder, three had anxiety problems, and one tended to disconnect (Table 1, codes ICD-9-CM, Ministerio de Sanidad, Servicios Sociales e Igualdad, 2014).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Patient characteristics.

The inclusion criteria were (a) an age of 12–15 years and (b) recommendation for group therapy following diagnostic evaluation at the Mental Health Center. The exclusion criteria were (a) anticipated difficulty attending all the therapy sessions and (b) contraindication for group therapy.

The group was led by an expert therapist, assisted by a co-therapist who participated as an observer. Both were clinical psychologists trained in group psychoanalytic psychotherapy.

In accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and the Ethical Code of the General Council of the Official College of Psychologists of Spain, the participants were informed that they were being filmed. They were shown the location of the video cameras, which were positioned discretely to minimize reactivity bias. Informed consent was also obtained from the parents of the minors.

Instruments

In systematic observation (Anguera, 2003; Sánchez-Algarra and Anguera, 2013), a distinction is made between recording instruments (i.e., those used to record or code data) and observation instruments (purposed-designed instruments to analyze a given subject).

Recording Instrument

The group sessions were recorded using two video cameras, two microphones, two video units, and two screens comprising a closed-circuit television system. The dataset was built in the software program GSEQ5, v.5.1 (Bakeman and Quera, 2011) using an initial transcription of the video content. In accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and the Ethical Code of the General Council of the Official College of Psychologists of Spain, the participants were informed that they were being filmed. They were shown the location of the video cameras, which were positioned discretely to minimize reactivity bias.

According to the terminology proposed by Bakeman (1978), the data recorded were type II data, i.e., they were concurrent (as we considered various dimensions and each behavior needs to be coded using a specific code) and event-based (as the behaviors were coded as they occurred, thereby providing information on order and sequence, two essential factors for our study). It is also possible to record duration, but this was not relevant to the purpose of our study. Once annotated, each behavior generates a co-occurrence of codes (corresponding to the different dimensions) and is methodologcally considered to be a multievent (Bakeman, 1978). A total of 30,436 multievents were coded in our study.

Observation Instrument

The ad-hoc observation instrument used in the study combined a field format and category systems. It is a flexible instrument in which the different dimensions considered can be broken down into different categories according to the theoretical framework and experience. Considering the specific goals of the study and based on previous experiences (Arias and Anguera, 2004, 2005), the observation instrument was redesigned to include 15 forms of communication. These forms, or dimensions of communication, were derived from the work of Torras de Beà (2013) on group psychotherapy and of Tusón (1995) and Calsamiglia and Tusón (1999) on conversation analysis.

The 15 dimensions included in the observation instrument are Facilitating of conversation, Reflective function, Expressivity, Defensive expressions, Dislike, Ordering, Humor, Confrontation, Exclamation, Degradation of vocal behavior, Whispering, Touching, Noise, Surrounding noise, and Silence (Table 2). Each of these dimensions was broken down to build a category system that fulfilled the requirements of exhaustivity and mutual exclusivity (Anguera, 2003).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Dimensions and category systems in the observation instrument for therapists and patients.

It should be noted that some dimensions gave rise to a single category, but given their conceptual relevance, we considered it important to include them as dimensions in the instrument. The dimensions and categories are shown in Table 2.

Procedure

The parents of the six adolescents were notified that their children had been proposed for group therapy after a diagnostic evaluation period. In addition, they all agreed to participate in a parallel group led by another therapist.

All the sessions were video-recorded and transcribed in full. Thirty sessions were held but due to technical difficulties with the recording, six were discarded because of poor audio. Therefore, 24 sessions were included in the final analysis. Each of the sessions lasted an hour. The sessions were grouped into four periods spanning an 8-month period.

Data Quality Control Analysis: Inter-observer Agreement

For the data quality control analysis, two observers analyzed and coded four of the therapy sessions. They had been previously trained using the approach described by Anguera (2003). Agreement was assessed quantitatively using Cohen's kappa statistic (Cohen, 1960, 1968) in GSEQ5 (version 5.1) following the recommendations of Bakeman and Quera (1995a,b, 2001, 2011). According to the criteria of Landis and Koch (1977), the level of agreement was “almost perfect”, with kappa values ranging between 0.86 and 0.93 for all the sessions.

Data Analysis

Polar coordinate analysis was used to analyze DYN and MNT interventions in accordance with the study objective. Polar coordinate analysis is a commonly used quantitative analytical method in observational methodology that identifies the statistical relationship between a behavior of interest (referred to in polar coordinate analysis as the focal behavior) and other behaviors (referred to as conditional behaviors). Associations between pairs of behaviors are represented graphically by vectors. Polar coordinate analysis requires a prior stage consisting of lag sequential analysis (Bakeman, 1978, 1991), a technique used to reveal behavioral patterns based on occurrence of behaviors after (prospective) or before (retrospective) a given behavior (as the focal behavior is known in lag sequential analysis). The technique is based on calculating conditional and unconditional probabilities (based, respectively, on matched frequencies and simple frequencies) for each of the time lags considered, which may be positive or negative.

Lag sequential analysis produces large volumes of data, which are subsequently reduced through a powerful data reduction algorithm based on the Zsum = Σzn parameter proposed by Cochran (1954), where z is the standard value corresponding to each lag for each of the conditional behaviors (known as target behaviors) and n is the number of lags considered. The Zsum is calculated for each target behavior for both positive lags (prospective Zsum) and negative lags (retrospective Zsum). The technique thus yields a statistical relationship between the given behavior and each of the target behaviors, which is reflected by a prospective and a retrospective Zsum value, as proposed by Sackett (1980, 1987). To optimize the procedure, Anguera (1997) proposed a modification to the original technique (1980, 1987) based on the concept of genuine retrospectivity. This modified technique has been used on multiple occasions in the past two decades and was employed in the current study.

Polar coordinate analysis integrates the prospective and retrospective perspectives with the help of a vectorial map that contains four quadrants in which the prospective and retrospective Zsum values are plotted along the X and Y axis, respectively. Each target behavior analysis thus can be located in one of the four quadrants depending on the combination of negative/positive signs (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Polar coordinate analysis results corresponding to interventions by the therapist (T) as the focal behavior and interventions by the participants (G D JM F L M), interventions by the co-therapist (CT), and silence as conditional behaviors.

Polar coordinate analysis uses the prospective and retrospective Zsum values for each conditional behavior to calculate the length and angle of the corresponding vector, thus allowing these to be graphically represented. The length of the vector is (Zsum2Prospective+Zsum2Retrospective), and is considered to be statistically significant (p < 0.05) when it exceeds 1.96 The angle of the vector is calculated as follows: φ = arc sen ZsumRetrospectiveLength and it is then adjusted according to the quadrant in which it is located: quadrant I (0 < φ <90) = φ; quadrant II (90 < φ <180) = 180 − φ; quadrant III (180 < φ < 270) = 180 + φ; quadrant IV (270° < φ < 360°) = 360° − φ.

The meanings of the different quadrants are shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Characteristics of the quadrants in which the vectors are located according to the activation (+) or inhibition (–) sign carried by the Prospective and Retrospective Zsum values.

Quadrants I and III are symmetrical in terms of the relationship they depict between the focal behavior and the different conditional behaviors they contain. Quadrant I (++) indicates mutual activation while quadrant III (−) indicates mutual inhibition. Quadrants II and IV, in turn, depict asymmetrical relationships. Quadrant II (−+) indicates that the focal behavior inhibits but at the same time is activated by the conditional behaviors, while quadrant IV (+−) indicates the opposite (i.e., the focal behavior activates and is inhibited by the corresponding conditional behaviors).

The polar coordinate analysis for this study was performed in HOISAN v. 1.6.3.2 (Hernández-Mendo et al., 2012), which contains all the necessary modules and also produces partial results for adjusted residuals and z values in addition to analytical parameters and polar coordinate maps. The analysis was conducted by exporting the data file from GSEQ5 to HOISAN.

Polar coordinate analysis has been used in certain areas of clinical psychology, such as groups of children with autistic siblings (Venturella, 2016). It has also been widely applied in sports (Perea et al., 2012; Robles-Prieto et al., 2014; Echeazarra et al., 2015; López-López et al., 2015; Morillo-Baro et al., 2015; Sousa et al., 2015; Castañer et al., 2016, 2017; Aragón et al., 2017) and school settings (Herrero Nivela, 2000; Anguera et al., 2003; López et al., 2016; Santoyo et al., 2017). As a final note of interest, when Sackett (1980) first presented polar coordinate analysis, he used it to study turn-taking in conversation.

Results and Discussion

In the sections below, we describe the relationships detected between interventions by the therapist and the group participants using polar coordinate analysis.

Relationships between Turn-Taking by the Therapist, Turn-Taking by the Participants and the Co-therapist, and Silence

The focal behavior was intervention by the therapist (T) and the conditional behaviors were interventions by the participants (G, D, JM, F L, and M), interventions by the co-therapist (coT), and silence (Q) in the four blocks of sessions spanning 8 weeks.

As shown in Table 3, the majority of results were significant.

The graphs in Figure 2 show the vectors representing turn-taking by the participants and the co-therapist and silence. In the case of the adolescents, some of the vectors are located in the mutual inhibition quadrant (quadrant III) while others are located in the mutual activation quadrant (quadrant I). On analyzing the four blocks of sessions grouped by time, it can clearly be seen that the turn-taking behavior by D, L, and M changed over the course of therapy, that of the co-therapist and silence remained stable.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Vectors corresponding to interventions by the therapist (T) as the focal behavior and interventions by the participants (G, D, JM, F L, and M), interventions by the co-therapist (coT), and silence (Q) as conditional behaviors. Session blocks 1-2-3-4 (from left to right).

Relationship between the Therapist and DYN and MNT Interventions

Again, the focal behavior was intervention by the therapist (T) and the conditional behaviors were the DYN categories FF, FO, RP, RT, QA, QC, and QV and the MNT category.

The majority of results in this case were also significant (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Polar coordinate analysis results with interventions by the therapist (T) as the focal behavior and DYN categories (broken down) and MNT as conditional behaviors.

The graphs in Figure 3 show the vectors for the different relationships distributed among the four quadrants. On examining the figures by blocks of time, it can be seen that the vectors tend to form clusters, with the majority located in the mutual activation quadrant (quadrant I) by the end of the therapy. Note that the length of the radius for repetition (RP) and the quadrant in which it was located (quadrant I) remained stable over the four periods.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Vectors corresponding to interventions by the therapist (T) as the focal behavior and conversationfacilitating DYN categories (FF, FO, RP, RT, QA, QC, QV) and the mentalizing or reflective function MNT category as conditional behaviors. Session blocks 1-2-3-4 (from left to right).

Below we discuss the significance of the relationships detected by polar coordinate analysis in five sections. We also illustrate our findings with clinical vignettes containing coded transcripts of the interventions.

Turn-Taking by the Therapist and the Adolescents

All the significant results are located in two opposing quadrants, indicating two clearly differentiated types of relationship: mutual activation and mutual inhibition. The therapist always facilitates intervention by Fred, the participant with the greatest difficulty relating to others, and in the early phases of therapy, she also encourages interaction from Danny, John M, and Meg. Her interventions never activate those of the two impulsive participants, Gabriel and Lucy. This does not mean that she excludes these participants, simply that they intervene on their own initiative. The changes detected in Danny, John M, and Meg are an indication of the progress they make over the therapy. Block 1 is characterized by radial communication between the therapist and all the participants. Vignette 1 shows an example of an interaction between the therapist and Danny (Table 5).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Clinical vignette 1.

However, not all interactions are the same. Gabriel and Lucy, for example, spontaneously take turns in these early sessions (Table 6).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Clinical vignette 2.

Lucy raises conflicts about herself that interest everyone (Table 7).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Clinical vignette 3.

John M is a reserved person with anxiety problems. He has difficulty intervening and when he does, he often mumbles, says very little, and adheres to what has just been said (Table 8).

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Clinical vignette 4.

Haen and Weil (2010) have highlighted the difficulties that adolescents have engaging during this initial stage of therapy. In our study, as the therapy progresses, the adolescents start to communicate much more naturally and spontaneously and bring up issues that concern them, such as going out, the end of the school year, and their expectations for the coming year. Vignette 5, which contains an excerpt from this last block, shows how Danny, John M, Lucy, and Meg chat freely amongst themselves, without encouragement from the therapist. Amidst jokes, exclamations, gestures, and laughter, they talk about meeting outside the group and about their fears of traveling alone on the train or underground for the first time (Table 9).

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Clinical vignette 5.

Turn-Taking by the Therapist and the Co-therapist

The co-therapist and the therapist was mutually activated (quadrant 1). The co-therapist's interventions reflect her role of interfering as little as possible in the group dynamics. They complement those of the main therapist. Together, they form a team and create and maintain a safe environment (Shechtman 2007; Torras de Beà, 2013; Malekoff, 2014).

The Therapist and Silence

The therapist generates silence but also breaks it (quadrant 1).

The examples below show how the adolescents fall silent when faced with difficult issues, such as verbalizing why they are in the group or talking about their relationship with their parents or their concerns about sexuality (Tables 1012).

TABLE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Clinical vignette 6.

TABLE 11
www.frontiersin.org

Table 11. Clinical vignette 7.

TABLE 12
www.frontiersin.org

Table 12. Clinical vignette 8.

The Therapist and DYN Interventions

The different strategies for facilitating conversation (FF, FO, RP, RT, QA, QC, and QV) showed varying patterns of change over the course of therapy but converged at the end.

Repetition (RP) was the most powerful strategy, as it activated conversation from the start of the therapy program. The next most powerful strategies were phatic function (FF) and greetings (FO). The transcripts of the sessions show that in the early sessions, it was the therapist who verbally greeted the adolescents (by saying hello and goodbye). However, few of them responded and the others returned the greeting or made a non-verbal gesture. This behavior changes after the first block, indicating an increase in reciprocity between the therapist and the participants.

The appearance of QA (questions directed at others) in the second half of the therapy is, in our opinion, a highly significant indicator of the group process. It tells us that the communication is no longer radial and that the adolescents have achieved one of the most important benefits of group therapy, which is showing interest in others (Yalom, 2005) in the presence of the therapist (Torras de Beà, 2013).

It is also interesting to see how QV (repetition of a previous utterance in the form of a question) changes from being mutually inhibitory to being mutually activating. We think that this strategy initially surprised the adolescents but was then gradually adopted by them. The same was not observed for QC (clarifying questions), which were used only by the therapist when the adolescents were “doing their own thing” and she was “excluded” from the group. Examples of what she said were: “I'm not quite following you now…maybe I'm being a bit dense, can you help me understand what's going on?” This strategy is similar to the attitude of respectful curiosity shown by therapists in the Adolescent Mentalization-Based Integrative Treatment (AMBIT) approach (mentalizing stance), which is designed to help put a halt to non-mentalization mental states (Benvington et al., 2012; Dangerfield, 2016).

Bringing back a central topic of conversation (RT) and suggesting looking at this in greater depth was only done by the therapist.

At the end of therapy, all the categories in the DYN dimension except RT are located in the mutually activating quadrant. This supports the idea that the communication strategies used by the therapist were adopted by the participants, enabling them to talk more autonomously and facilitating their personal growth (Yalom, 2005; Torras de Beà, 2013).

The Therapist and MNT Interventions

The changes observed in the MNT category, which corresponds to interventions aimed at improving the adolescents' mentalization abilities, also reflect interesting aspects of the group process. The MNT category changed from inhibitory (quadrant III) to partially inhibitory (quadrant II) and finally to mutually activating (quadrant I). The changes also show that the therapist's role changed over time, as mentalization strategies were only used by her. We can deduce that the participants gradually overcame their early inhibitions and dependence and acquired more sophisticated mentalizing abilities, helping them to become more aware of themselves and of others. This result is consistent with the concept known as the interpretative function of the therapist within the theories of Foulkes (1986) and Torras de Beà (2013).

Conclusions

Polar coordinate analysis provides a new approach for gaining insights into dialogue in group pyschotherapy. The results show that the technique provides a novel means of analyzing the role of the therapist and describing her conversational style. The therapist proved to be an expert in creating a communicative environment that allowed the adolescents to grow. She employed four core strategies: (1) she did not facilitate communication equally for all participants, (2) she encouraged turn-taking by the more inhibited members of the group, (3) she stimulated conversation from the early stages of therapy, and (4) she promoted mentalization toward the end of therapy.

We were particularly pleased to see that the use of repetition (RP) facilitated communication flows from the beginning. The positive results indicate that rather than simply acting as an echo or a loudspeaker, this strategy produces a mirroring effect similar to that described in the social biofeedback theory of parental affect-mirring (Gergely and Watson, 1996), in which the person talking, apart from being listened to, is brought into a mirror-like interaction. This regulatory effect is a prerequisite for the mentalization process that facilitates the development of the self (Fonagy et al., 2002).

Observational methodology and polar coordinate analysis could prove to be of great value for detecting changes in psychotherapy models based on spoken conversation.

Author Contributions

EA developed the project. MA performed the method section and polar coordinate analysis. Both authors have participated in the writing of the article.

Funding

This study was supported by the Catalan government under grant number 2014 SGR 1088 for the project Grup de recerca comunicació i salut (COMSAL) and Grup de recerca i innovació en dissenys (GRID) and under Grant number 2014 SGR 971 for the project Tecnologia i aplicació multimedia i digital als dissenys observacionals. We also gratefully acknowledge the support of the Spanish government (Ministerio de Economía y Competitvidad) within the Projects Avances metodológicos y tecnológicos en el estudio observacional del comportamiento deportivo [Grant PSI2015-71947-REDT; MINECO/FEDER, UE] (2015-2017), and La actividad física y el deporte como potenciadores del estilo de vida saludable: evaluación del comportamiento deportivo desde metodologías no intrusivas [Grant DEP2015-66069-P; MINECO/FEDER, UE] (2016-2018). Lastly, we also acknowledge the support of Ramon Llull University (PGRiD of FPCEE Blanquerna) and University of Barcelona (Vice-Chancellorship of Doctorate and Research Promotion).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank all those at the Center for Child and Adolescent Mental Health of the Eulàlia Torras de Beà Foundation in Barcelona, Spain, who so willingly helped to make this study possible, as well as all the adolescents and families who participated.

References

Anguera, M. T. (1979). Observational typology. Qual. Quant. 13, 449–484.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T. (1997). From Prospective Patterns in Behavior to Joint Analysis with a Retrospective Perspective. En Colloque sur Invitation « Méthodologie D'analyse des Interactions Sociales». Paris: Université de la Sorbonne.

Anguera, M. T. (2003). “Observational methods (general),” in Encyclopedia of Psychological Assessment, Vol. 2, ed R. Fernández-Ballesteros (London: Sage), 632–637.

Anguera, M. T., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2016). Avances en estudios observacionales de ciencias del deporte desde los mixed methods [Advances in mixed methods observational studies in sports sciences]. Cuad. Psicol. Dep. 16, 17–30.

Anguera, M. T., Santoyo, C., and Espinosa, M. C. (2003). “Evaluating links intensity in social networks in a school context through observational designs,” in Culture, Environmental Action and Sustainability, eds R. García Mira, J. M. Sabucedo Cameselle, and J. Romay Martínez (Göttingen: Hogrefe and Huber), 286–298.

Google Scholar

Aragón, S., Lapresa, D., Arana, J., Anguera, M. T., and Garzón, B. (2017). An example of the informative potential of polar coordinate analysis: sprint tactics in elite 1,500-m track events. Meas. Phys. Educ. Exerc. Sci. doi: 10.1080/1091367X.2016.1245192. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arias, E. (2011). Metodología observacional e investigación del proceso grupal. [Observational Methodology and Group Process Research]. Teoría Práct. Grupoanalítica 1, 435–448.

Arias, E., and Anguera, M. T. (2004). Detección de patrones de conducta comunicativa en un grupo terapéutico de adolescentes [Detection of communicative behavior patterns in a therapeutic group of adolescents]. Acción Psicol. 3, 199–206. doi: 10.5944/ap.3.3.513

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arias, E., and Anguera, M. T. (2005). Análisis de la comunicación en un grupo terapéutico de adolescentes: estudio diacrónico [Analysis of Communication in a Therapeutic Group of Adolescents: A Diachronic Study]. Rev. Psicopatol. Salud Ment. Niño Adolesc. M1, 25–36.

Bakeman, R. (1978). “Untangling streams of behavior: Sequential analysis of observation data,” in Observing Behavior, Vol. 2: Data Collection and Analysis Methods, ed G. P. Sackett (Baltimore, MD: University of Park Press), 63–78.

Bakeman, R. (1991). From lags to logs: advances in sequential analysis. Mex. J. Behav. Anal. 17, 65–83.

Google Scholar

Bakeman, R., and Quera, V. (1995a). Log-lineal approaches to lag-sequential analysis when consecutive codes may cannot repeat. Psychol. Bull. 118, 272–284. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.2.272

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakeman, R., and Quera, V. (1995b). Analyzing Interaction: Sequential Analysis with SDIS and GSEQ. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Bakeman, R., and Quera, V. (2001). Using GSEQ with SPSS. Metod. C Comport. 3, 195–214.

Google Scholar

Bakeman, R., and Quera, V. (2011). Sequential Analysis and Observational Methods for the Behavioral Sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Barkowski, S., Schwartze, D., Strauss, B., Burlingame, G. M., Barth, J., and Rosendahl, J. (2016). Efficacy of group psychotherapy for social anxiety disorder: a meta-analysis of randomized-controlled trials. J. Anxiety Disord. 39, 44–64. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bateman, A., and Fonagy, P. (ed.) (2012). Handbook of Mentalizing in Mental Health Practice. London: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Google Scholar

Benvington, D., Fuggle, P., Fonagy, P., Target, M., and Asen, E. (2012). Innovations in practice: Adolescent Mentalization-Based Integrative Therapy (AMBIT) – a new integrated approach to working with the most hard to reach adolescents with severe complex mental health needs. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 18, 46–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-3588.2012.00666.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanco-Villaseñor, A., Losada, J. L., and Anguera, M. T. (2003). Analytic techniques in observational designs in environment-behavior relation. Medio Ambient. Comport. Hum. Rev. Int. Psicol. Ambient. 4, 111–126.

Bros, I., Notó, P., and Bulbena, A. (2016). Effectiveness of short-term dynamic group psychotherapy in primary care for patients with depressive symptoms. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2029. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calsamiglia, H., and Tusón, A. (1999). Las Cosas del Decir [Things about saying]. Barcelona: Ariel.

Google Scholar

Castañer, M., Barreira, D., Camerino, O., Anguera, M. T., Canton, A., and Hileno, R. (2016). Goal scoring in soccer: a polar coordinate analysis of motor skills used by lionel messi. Front. Psychol. 7:806. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castañer, M., Barreira, D., Camerino, O., Anguera, M. T., Fernandes, T., and Hileno, R. (2017). Mastery in goal scoring, T-pattern detection and polar coordinate analysis of motor skills used by lionel messi and cristiano ronaldo. Front. Psychol. 8:741. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cochran, W. G. (1954). Some methods for streghning the common χ2 test. Biometrics 10, 417–451. doi: 10.2307/3001616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 20, 37–46. doi: 10.1177/001316446002000104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1968). Weighted kappa: nominal scale agreement with provision for scaled disagreement of partial credit. Psychol. Bull. 70, 213–220. doi: 10.1037/h0026256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cramer-Azima, F. J. (2002). “Group psychotherapy for children and adolescents,” in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: A Comprehensive Textbook, 3rd Edn., ed M. Lewis (Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams Wilkins), 1032–1036.

Dangerfield, M. (2016). “Sense un lloc a la teva ment, sense un lloc al món”. Aspectes tècnics del treball amb adolescents negligits. [“No place in your mind, no place in the world”. Technical aspects of working with neglected adolescents]. Rev. Catalana Psicoanàl. 33, 99–129.

Devi, A., and Fenn, E. (2012). ‘Make sure you keep our house safe!’ Thematic analysis of a children's psychotherapy group. J. Child Psychother. 38, 318–334. doi: 10.1080/0075417X.2012.720104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Echeazarra, I., Castellano, J., Usabiaga, O., and Hernández-Medo, A. (2015). Diferencias en el uso estratégico del espacio en categorías infantil y cadete de fútbol: una aplicación del análisis de coordenadas polares [Differences in the strategic use of space in under 14 and under 16 soccer: a polar coordinate analysis]. Cuadern. Psicol. Deporte 15, 169–180. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232015000100017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erikson, E. (1968). Identity, Youth, and Crisis. New York, NY: Norton.

Fonagy, P. (1991). Thinking about thinking: some clinical and theoretical considerations in the treatment of a borderline patient. Int. J. Psychoanal. 72, 639–656.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E., and Target, M. (2002). Affect Regulation, Mentalization, and the Development of the Self. New York, NY: Other Press.

Google Scholar

Fonagy, P., Leigh, T., Kennedy, R., Mattoon, G., Steele, H., Target, M., et al. (1995). “Attachment, borderline states and the representations of emotions and cognitions in self and other,” in Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopatology: Cognition and Emotion, eds D. Cicchetti and S. T. Rochester (New York, NY: University of Rochester Press), 371–414.

Foulkes, S. H. (1986). Psicoterapia Grupo- Analítica. Métodos y Principios. [Group Analytic Psychotherapy: Methods and Principles]. México: Gedisa.

Gergely, G., and Watson, J. (1996). The social biofeedback theory of parental affect-mirroring: the development of emotional self-awareness and self-control in infancy. Int. J. Psychoanal. 77, 1181–1212.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Haen, C., and Weil, M. (2010). Group therapy on the edge: adolescence, creativity, and group work. Group 34, 37–52. doi: 10.2307/41719263

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernández-Mendo, A., López-López, J. A., Castellano, J., Morales-Sánchez, V., and Pastrana, J. L. (2012). Hoisan 1.2: programa informático para uso en metodología observacional [Hoisan 1.2: Software for observational methodology]. Cuad. Psicol. Dep. 12, 55–78. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232012000100006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrero Nivela, M. L. (2000). Utilización de la técnica de coordenadas polares en el estudio de la interacción infantil en el marco escolar [Use of polar coordinate analysis to study children's interactions at school]. Psicothema 12, 292–297.

Google Scholar

Landis, J. R., and Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33, 159–174. doi: 10.2307/2529310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López, J., Valero, A., Anguera, M. T., and Díaz, A. (2016). Disruptive behavior among elementary students in physical education. SpringerPlus 5, 1154. doi: 10.1186/s40064-016-2764-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-López, J. A., Menescardi, C., Estevan, I., Falcó, C., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2015). Análisis técnico-táctico en Taekwondo con coordenadas polares a través del software HOISAN [Technical-tactical analysis in Taekwondo with polar coordinates through software HOISAN]. Cuad. Psicol. Dep. 15, 131–142. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232015000100013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorentzen, S., Ruud, T., Fjeldstad, A., and Høglend, P. (2013). Comparison of short- and long-term dynamic group psychotherapy: randomised clinical trial. Br. J. Psychiatry 203, 280–287. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.113688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, W., and Hill, C. E. (2009). Quantitative and qualitative methods for psychotherapy research: introduction to special section. Psychother. Res. 19, 369–373. doi: 10.1080/10503300902948053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malekoff, A. (2014). Group Work with Adolescents. Principles and Practice, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Guildford Press.

Malmberg, L., and Fenton, M. (2008). “Psicoterapia psicodinámica individual y psicoanálisis para la esquizofrenia y enfermedades mentales graves,” [Individual psychodynamic psychotherapy and psychoanalysis for schizophrenia and serious mental disorders] (Revisión Cochrane traducida), in La Biblioteca Cochrane Plus (Oxford; Chichester: Update Software Ltd.; The Cochrane Library; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.). Available online at: http://www.update-software.com

Merikangas, K. R., He, J., Burstein, M., Swanson, S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., et al. (2010). Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in US adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication-Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 49, 980–989. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ministerio de Sanidad, Servicios Sociales e Igualdad (2014). Clasificación Internacional de enfermedades 9a Revisión, Modificación Clínica (CIE-9-MC) [International Classification of Diseases, Ninth Revision, Clinical Modification (ICD-9-CM)] Available online at: www.msssi.gob.es

Morillo-Baro, J. P., Reigal, R. E., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2015). Análisis del ataque posicional de balonmano playa masculino y femenino mediante coordenadas polares [Analysis of positional attack in beach handball male and female with polar coordinates]. RICYDE Rev. Int. Cienc. Deporte XI, 226–244. doi: 10.5232/ricyde

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nuffield Foundation (2013). Social Trends and Mental Health: Introducing the Main Findings. London: Nuffield Foundation.

Pascual-Leone, A., Greenberg, L. S., and Pascual-Leone, J. (2009). Developments in task analysis: new methods to study change. Psychother. Res. 19, 527–542. doi: 10.1080/10503300902897797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perea, A., Castellano, J., Alday, L., and Hernández Mendo, A. (2012). Analysis of behaviour in sports through Polar Coordinate Analysis with MATLAB. Qual. Quant. 46, 1249–1260. doi: 10.1007/s11135-011-9435-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pingitore, F. (2016). The “voice” and perspectives of adolescents participating in a short-term psychotherapy group. Int. J. Group Psychother. 67, 1–23. doi: 10.1080/00207284.2016.1260460

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollock, K., and Kymissis, P. (2001). The future of adolescent group therapy: an analysis of historical trends and current momentum. J. Child Adolesc. Group Ther. 11, 3–11. doi: 10.1023/A:1016689625271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portell, M., Anguera, M. T., Chacón, S., and Sanduvete, S. (2015a). Guidelines for reporting evaluations based on observational methodology (GREOM). Psicothema 27, 283–289. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2014.276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portell, M., Anguera, M. T., Hernández-Mendo, A., and Jonsson, G. K. (2015b). Quantifying biopsychosocial aspects in everyday contexts: an integrative methodological approach from the behavioral sciences. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 8, 153–160. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S82417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pylvänäinen, P. M., Muotka, J. S., and Lappalainen, R. (2015). A dance movement therapy group for depressed adult patients in a psychiatric outpatient clinic: effects of the treatment. Front. Psychol. 6:980. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reid, S., and Kolvin, I. (1993). Group psychotherapy for children and adolescentes. Arch. Dis. Child. 69, 244–250 doi: 10.1136/adc.69.2.244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, L. D., and Povee, K. (2014). A brief measure of attitudes toward mixed methods research in psychology. Front. Psychol. 5:1312. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robles-Prieto, F. J., Castellano, J., and Perea, A. E. (2014). Diferencias del juego entre la Selección Española de Fútbol y sus rivales [Differences of the game betweeb the Football Spanish Team and its rivals]. RICCAFD Rev. Iberoameric. Cienc. Activ. Física Deporte 3, 1–8.

Google Scholar

Sackett, G. P. (ed.) (1978). Observing Behavior (Vol. 2): Data Collection and Analysis Methods. Baltimore, MD: University of Park Press.

Sackett, G. P. (1980). “Lag Sequential Analysis as a data reduction technique in social interaction research,” in Exceptional Infant. Psychosocial Risks in Infant-Environment Transactions, eds D. B. Sawin, R. C. Hawkins, L. O. Walker, and J. H. Penticuff (New York, NY: Brunner/Mazel), 300–340.

Sackett, G. P. (1987). “Analysis of sequential social interaction data: some issues. recent developments, and a causal inference model,” in Handbook of Infant Development, ed J. D. Osofsky (New York, NY: Wiley), 855–878.

Google Scholar

Sánchez-Algarra, P., and Anguera, M. T. (2013). Qualitative/quantitative integration in the inductive observational study of interactive behaviour: impact of recording and coding predominating perspectives. Qual. Quant. 47, 1237–1257. doi: 10.1007/s11135-012-9764-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santoyo, C., Jonsson, G. K., Anguera, M. T., and López-López, J. A. (2017). Observational Analysis of the Organization of On-Task Behavior in the Classroom Using Complementary Data Analyses. An. Psicol.- Spain.

Schwartze, D., Barkowski, S., Burlingame, G. M., Strauss, B., and Rosendahl, J. (2016). Efficacy of group psychotherapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Obsessive Compuls. Relat. Disord. 10, 49–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jocrd.2016.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shechtman, Z. (2007). Group Counseling and Psychotherapy with Children and Adolescents: Theory, Research and Practice. New York, NY: Routledge

Google Scholar

Simpson, S. G., Morrow, E., van Vreeswijk, M. F., and Reid, C. (2010). Group schema therapy for eating disorders: a pilot study. Front. Psychol. 1:182. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skewes, S. A., Samson, R. A., Simpson, S. G., and van Vreeswijk, M. (2015). Shortterm group schema therapy for mixed personality disorders: a pilot study. Front. Psychol. 5:1592. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01592

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sousa, D. J., Prudente, J. N., Sequeira, P., López-López, J. A., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2015). Análisis de las situaciones de juego 2vs2 en el campeonato europeo masculino de balonmano 2012: aplicación de la técnica de coordenadas polares [Analysis of 2vs2 situations in men's european handball championship 2012: application of polar coordinates technique]. Cuadern. Psicol. Deporte 15, 181–194. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232015000100018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tellerman, J. S. (2001). The Solutions Unlimited Now-SUN(sm) program: psychodynamic/cognitive structured groups for teens, pre-teens, and families. J. Child Adolesc. Group Ther. 11, 117–134. doi: 10.1023/A:1014758406520

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torras de Beà, E. (2013). Psicoterapia de Grupo [Group Psychotherapy]. Barcelona: Octaedro.

Tusón, A. (1995). Anàlisi de la Conversa [Conversation analysis]. Barcelona: Empúries.

Venturella, M. (2016). Autismo, un Hermano, Otros Hermanos, un Grupo. Estudio Observacional del Proceso de un Grupo de Niños con Hermanos Diagnosticados de Trastorno de Espectro Autista (TEA) [Autism, a Sibling, Other Siblings, a Group. Observational Study of the Process within a Group of Children with Siblings with Autism Spectrum Disorder] Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Universidad Ramon Llull, Barcelona.

Weschler, D. (2006). Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th Edn (WISC-IV). Madrid: TEA Ediciones.

Google Scholar

Yalom, I. D. (2005). The Theory and Practice of Group Psychotherapy, 5th Edn. New York, NY: BasicBooks.

Keywords: group therapy, adolescent interactions, mixed-method, polar coordinates analysis, mentalization

Citation: Arias-Pujol E and Anguera MT (2017) Observation of Interactions in Adolescent Group Therapy: A Mixed Methods Study. Front. Psychol. 8:1188. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01188

Received: 13 January 2017; Accepted: 29 June 2017;
Published: 24 July 2017.

Edited by:

Pietro Cipresso, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Italy

Reviewed by:

Antonio Calcagnì, University of Trento, Italy
Eleonora Riva, Università degli Studi di Milano, Italy

Copyright © 2017 Arias-Pujol and Anguera. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Eulàlia Arias-Pujol, eulaliaap@blanquerna.url.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.